Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Cover ArticleResearch Articles, Behavioral/Cognitive

Effects of Stimulus Timing on the Acquisition of an Olfactory Working Memory Task in Head-Fixed Mice

Josefine Reuschenbach, Janine K. Reinert, Xiaochen Fu and Izumi Fukunaga
Journal of Neuroscience 26 April 2023, 43 (17) 3120-3130; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1636-22.2023
Josefine Reuschenbach
Sensory and Behavioural Neuroscience Unit, Okinawa Institute of Science and Technology Graduate University, Tancha, Onna, Okinawa 907-0497, Japan
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Janine K. Reinert
Sensory and Behavioural Neuroscience Unit, Okinawa Institute of Science and Technology Graduate University, Tancha, Onna, Okinawa 907-0497, Japan
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Xiaochen Fu
Sensory and Behavioural Neuroscience Unit, Okinawa Institute of Science and Technology Graduate University, Tancha, Onna, Okinawa 907-0497, Japan
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Izumi Fukunaga
Sensory and Behavioural Neuroscience Unit, Okinawa Institute of Science and Technology Graduate University, Tancha, Onna, Okinawa 907-0497, Japan
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Article Figures & Data

Figures

  • Tables
  • Figure 1.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 1.

    An olfactometer design for stable odor presentations. A, A schematic of the design. Filtered air supply is split into three paths, (i) a stream that normally flows to the animal, (ii) a stream used for the sample odor, and (iii) a stream used for the test odor. Each odorizing stream has a pair of three-way valves. When one path is engaged for odor presentation, the air from the selected odor path passes through an odor canister before being directed toward the final valve (red highlight). Simultaneously, the other path diverts air directly toward the final valve (blue highlight), bypassing the odor canisters. B, Odors used in this paradigm. C, Four sample odor (O1) test odor (O2) delays used for assessing the olfactometer performance, which were randomly interleaved. D, Example photoionization detector signals for the four permutations of sample and test odors (average of 12 trials), color coded by the interval used as in C. E, Photoionization detector signals for the test odor overlaid for all intervals tested. F, Test odors were passed through blank odor canisters to test the level of cross-contamination at four delay intervals as in C. G, Photoionization detector signals during the test stimulus periods were expressed as a fraction of the sample odor signal level. Levels from individual trials are shown.

  • Figure 2.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 2.

    Go/No-Go olfactory delayed nonmatch-to-sample task with a 5 s delay. A, Illustration of the olfactory DNMS. On a given trial, two odors (Odor 1 and Odor 2) are presented. Rewarded trials are trials where two odors are not the same (nonmatch). Reward is 20 µl of water. B, Timeline of training. Behavioral training started 2 weeks after surgery for head plate implantation. C, Schematic showing the trial structure. Flashes of an LED indicate the trial start. Sample and test odors (O1 and O2, respectively) are presented with an interval (5 s for the initial training, and 12 s afterward). Water reward was delivered on all rewarded trials. Reward was delivered earlier when mice generated anticipatory licks earlier (range of possible reward times, 0.5–2.5 s from O2 offset). D, Lick raster for an example session, separated by nonmatch trials (left) versus match trials (right). E, Example peristimulus time histogram of licks separated by four trial types from a proficient mouse. F, Relationship between stray licks (licks during the sample-test odor interval) and accuracy of DNMS performance. Symbols indicate individual mice. G, Learning curves for 5 s delay (hollow symbols) and 12 s delay (stars) shown with respect to the trials from the start of training (block size, 50 trials; left) and with respect to days (right); n = 6 mice. Mean and SEM are indicated.

  • Figure 3.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 3.

    DNMS with a shorter interval is easier to learn. A, Two training designs are compared. One cohort of mice that started the training with sample-test odor interval of 1.7 s (green) was compared with those that started with 5 s interval (blue; same data as Fig. 2). B, Learning curves for the two cohorts, showing behavioral accuracy in auROC for for 1.7 s delay (filled circles), 5 s delay (hollow circles), and 12 s delay (stars). Color scheme as in A. Block size, 50 trials; n = 6 and 5 mice for initial training with 5 s and 1.7 s, respectively. C, Summary comparison of acquisition speeds in terms of number of trials (left) and number of days (right) taken to reach the criterion (auROC = 0.8); *p = 0.043 (left) and 0.036 (right), one-way ANOVA with post hoc Tukey–Kramer comparisons; n.s., p = 0.82 (left) and 0.80 (right). Mean and SEM are indicated. D, Relationship between stray licks (licks during the sample-test odor interval) and accuracy of DNMS performance. Pearson's r = −0.549, p = 4.73 × 10−6. Symbols correspond to individual mice. n.s. = not significant at the 0.05 level.

  • Figure 4.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 4.

    Effect of reward timing on DNMS acquisition. A, With conditional reward timing (green), the water reward was delivered earlier if mice licked earlier (possible reward onset ranged from 0.5 to 2.5 s after the odor 2 offset). With fixed reward delivery time (burgundy), water reward was delivered at 2.5 s after the odor 2 offset. B, Timeline of training for the two cohorts, with sample-test odor interval indicated inside the parentheses. C, Examples of lick timing (vertical lines) relative to odor 2 (duration 0.6 s) and water delivery (light blue). For the conditional reward time group (left), the timing of reward onset depended on lick timing. For the fixed reward onset (right), water timing was fixed regardless of licking behavior. D, Histograms of water onset times for the two groups, for days 1–3 of the training. E, DNMS learning curves for the two cohorts for 1.7 s (filled circles), 5 s (hollow circles), and 12 s (stars) intervals. Horizontal line indicates the criterion level (auROC = 0.8). F, Summary of learning speeds, measured by number of trials taken to reach criterion performance at sample-test odor interval of 1.7 s (left), 5 s (middle), and 12 s (right) for the two cohorts; p = 0.047, 0.068, 0.87 (two-sample t tests), respectively; n = 5 mice for conditional reward timing and 6 mice for fixed reward timing. G, Relationship between stray licks (licks during the sample-test odor interval) and accuracy of DNMS performance. Pearson's r = −0.483, p = 1.83 × 10−4. Symbols correspond to individual mice. Cond., Conditional. n.s. = not significant at the 0.05 level.

  • Figure 5.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 5.

    Olfactory stimulus interval affects the slope, whereas reward timing affects the intercept of learning curves. A, Slope of the logistic curve, derived from the fitted parameter, beta, for the three protocols used. Beta/4 describes the slope at the steepest point; p = 0.036, one-way ANOVA, followed by Tukey–Kramer post hoc comparisons (p = 0.1202, 0.0377, and 0.8662 for 1 vs 2, 2 vs 3, and 1 vs 3 comparisons). B, Intercept values for the fitted logistic curves; p = 0.0393, one-way ANOVA, followed by Tukey–Kramer post hoc comparisons (p = 0.7387, 0.1307, and 0.0405 for 1 vs 2, 2 vs 3, and 1 vs 3 comparisons). C, Initial accuracy when switching to longer sample-test interval, normalized by the last session performance; p = 0.0455 for equal performance at 5 s interval, one-way ANOVA; p = 0.0498 for equal performance at 12 s, one-way ANOVA with post hoc Tukey–Kramer comparisons; n = 6 mice, 5 mice, and 6 mice for cohorts 1, 2, and 3, respectively. Cond., Conditional. n.s. = not significant at the 0.05 level.

  • Figure 6.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 6.

    Interval-dependent performance and generalization. A, Once mice were trained to perform DNMS with a 12 s interval, they went through one session where the sample-test odor interval was randomly selected from 1.7 s, 5 s, 12 s, or 20 s on a given trial [DNMS (var.)]. B, Example trial order, where gray intensity indicates the sample-test odor interval duration. C, Lick raster, sorted by nonmatch (left) and match (right) trials and by the duration of sample-test odor intervals, with grayscale indicating the interval duration. White areas indicate the time of odor presentations. D, Accuracy for each interval used, for two cohorts, with colors corresponding to the cohorts described in A. Mean and SEM are indicated; n = 4 mice for each cohort. E, After recovery from surgery, naive mice went through habituation and training for olfactory DNMS with 1.7 s interodor interval and reward delivery fixed at 2.6 s after odor offset. Once trained, the mice performed 1–2 sessions of DNMS with variable interodor delays. F, Generalization to longer intervals. Accuracy of performance for the interodor intervals is indicated, expressed auROC; ** and * indicate statistically significant deviation using t test from auROC = 0.5 at the significance level of 0.01 and 0.05 with Bonferroni correction, respectively; p = 0.0007, 0.0003, 0.0013, 0.0032, and 0.0189 for 1.7 s, 5 s, 8 s, 10 s, and 20 s delay intervals, respectively; n = 7 mice.

  • Figure 7.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 7.

    Stimulus similarity and the generalizability. A, B, Generalization to new odor comparisons. A, Timeline of behavioral training. B, Odors used for the DNMS tasks, as listed in A. Mice underwent DNMS training with dissimilar odor pair (butyl acetate vs methyl salicylate; C/D vs C/D), similar odor pair (ethyl valerate vs methyl valerate; E/F vs E/F), and ethyl butyrate at two concentrations (G/H vs G/H). C, D, Assessing the similarity of olfactory bulb activity patterns evoked in anesthetized mice by odors used in the DNMS task. C, Raster plots for an example unit for all odors used. D, Correlation coefficients for pairs of odors used in the DNMS tasks. Each data point corresponds to the similarity of population activity for each recording site; n = 7 locations, 3 mice. E, Learning curves for the new odor pairs. Each data point represents the accuracy for a block of 30 trials; n = 4 mice. F, Same data as in E but aligned for the start of training for the three new odor pairs for easy comparison. G, Lick raster plots from an example mouse for the four new nonmatch pairs introduced in the multiodor DNMS stage. Color coding follows the scheme in B. H, Evolution of anticipatory licks over time for new nonmatch odors (left) and match pairs (right); n = 4 mice. Mean and SEM are indicated.

  • Figure 8.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 8.

    Behavioral output patterns depend on reward timing. A, Example lick raster plots from mice that went through different training paradigms. Left, a mouse that started with 5 s sample-test odor interval and conditional reward timing; middle, starting with a 1.7 s interval and conditional reward timing; right, starting with a 1.7 s interval and fixed reward timing. B, Evolution of stray licking during sample-test odor interval for the three cohorts (n = 6, 5, and 6 mice, respectively). Thick lines indicate mean; shadings indicate mean ± SEM. C, Illustration of reaction times; timings of first three licks after odor 2 onset are averaged and expressed relative to the onset of odor 2. D, Evolution of reaction time with training for the three cohorts indicated with the same color scheme as in A. Thin lines represent individual mice, thick lines represent average. E, Peristimulus time histograms for licks generated on match (unrewarded) trials were compared between cohorts that started with the 1.7 s delay but differed in the reward timing (n = 5 and 6 mice, respectively). Thick lines indicate mean; shading indicates mean ± SEM.

  • Figure 9.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 9.

    Interplay between reward timing, behavioral threshold, and the discriminability of sensory representations. A, A drift-diffusion model with one bound (blue line) to model the Go/No-Go behavior. Drift rates, µs+ and µs−, are the strengths of momentary evidence following the S+ (nonmatch) stimulus and S− (match) stimulus, respectively. At each time point, a fixed amount of noise is added and accumulated over time (sensory evidence). Reaction time is the time at which the sensory evidence crosses the bound. B, Histograms of reaction times for an example animal over five training days. Observed licks (correct licks are licks on S+ trials; false alarms are licks on S− trials) shown as bars. Simulated result using fitted parameters superimposed with lines (solid lines indicate S+ simulation; dotted lines indicate S− simulation). C, Estimated bounds for cohorts with fixed reward timing (burgundy) and conditional reward timing (green). Di, Estimated drift rates for the cohort with late, fixed reward timing. Rew., Reward. Dii, Estimated drift rates for two cohorts with conditional reward timing. E, Comparison of estimated bounds (top) and the ratio of drift rates (µs+/µs−, bottom) for the three cohorts from the last day of initial training. For decision bounds, p = 0.011 and 0.099 across reward timing and p = 0.43 for cohorts with same reward timing but different sample-test intervals (one-way ANOVA with post hoc Tukey–Kramer tests). For drift rates, p = 0.0183 and 0.0639 across reward timing, and p = 0.86 for cohorts with same reward timing (one-way ANOVA with post hoc Tukey–Kramer tests). Mean and SEM are indicated. n.s. = not significant at the 0.05 level.

  • Figure 10.
    • Download figure
    • Open in new tab
    • Download powerpoint
    Figure 10.

    Reward timing effect is likely general; the acquisition of fine olfactory discrimination task is also affected. A, Schematic of the behavioral paradigm. After habituation, head-fixed mice were trained to discriminate between EB and eugenol (Eug) mixtures that differed in the mixed ratios. After reaching the criterion level of performance (80% correct) on 80/20 versus 20/80 discrimination, the mice underwent training for 60/40 versus 40/60 mixture discrimination. B, Trial structure. For the early reward group, the water reward arrived 1.2 s after the onset of odor. For the late reward group, the latency to reward was 3.2 s. C, Lick raster plots from example mice for S+ and S− odors as indicated, for early reward group (top) and late reward group (bottom). D, Peristimulus time histogram of lick occurrences for the last training session for each task. Rel., relative to E, Evolution of lick onset timing for the two cohorts over training. Dotted lines indicate the reward arrival times for the two cohorts; n = 6 mice for each cohort. Mean and SEM are indicated. F, Learning curves for the two cohorts. Each data point corresponds to the average accuracy (auROC) for a block of 40 trials sliding every 20 trials. Mean ± SEM is indicated. G, Number of trials taken to reach the criterion level of accuracy (auROC = 0.8) for the initial acquisition; p = 0.014, two-sample t test; n = 6 mice in each cohort. Mean and SEM are indicated.

Tables

  • Figures
    • View popup
    Table 1.

    Statistical tests and their details

    Location in articleTest usedSample sizeTest statisticExact p value
    Figure 2FPearson correlation coefficient112 Blocks from 6 micer = −0.7851.1731 × 10−24
    Figure 3C (left)One-way ANOVA with post hoc multiple comparisons (Tukey–Kramer)Two groups (6 mice and 5 mice)F = 4.260.0378
    Figure 3C (right)One-way ANOVA with post hoc multiple comparisons (Tukey–Kramer)Two groups (6 mice and 5 mice)F = 5.820.0157
    Figure 3DPearson correlation coefficient61 Blocks from 5 micer = −0.5494.727 × 10−6
    Fig. 4F (left)two-sample t testTwo groups (5 mice and 6 mice)t = 2.30140.0469
    Fig. 4F (middle)two-sample t testTwo groups (5 mice and 6 mice)t = 2.07270.0681
    Fig. 4F (right)two-sample t testTwo groups (5 mice and 6 mice)t = −0.17360.8665
    Figure 4IPearson correlation coefficient55 Blocks from 6 micer = −0.4831.833 × 10−4
    Figure 5AOne-way ANOVA with post hoc multiple comparisons (Tukey–Kramer)Three groups (6 mice, 5 mice, 6 mice)F = 4.250.036
    Figure 5BOne-way ANOVA with post hoc multiple comparisons (Tukey–Kramer)Three groups (6 mice, 5 mice, 6 mice)F = 4.120.0393
    Figure 5C (left)One-way ANOVA with post hoc multiple comparisons (Tukey–Kramer)Two groups (5 mice and 6 mice)F = 5.380.0455
    Figure 5C (right)One-way ANOVA with post hoc multiple comparisons (Tukey–Kramer)Three groups (6 mice, 5 mice, 6 mice)F = 3.750.0498
    Figure 6Ft Test with Bonferroni multiple-comparison correctionSeven micet Statistics (1.7 s, 5 s, 8 s, 12 s, 20 s) = 7.3560, 8.6679, 6.5347, 5.2867, 3.4159)0.0007, 0.0003, 0.0013, 0.0032, 0.0189
    Fig. 9E (top)One-way ANOVA with post hoc multiple comparisons (Tukey-Kramer)Three groups (6 mice, 5 mice, 6 mice)F = 6.05880.0127
    Fig. 9E (bottom)One-way ANOVA with post hoc multiple comparisons (Tukey–Kramer)Three groups (6 mice, 5 mice, 6 mice)F = 5.58410.0165
    Fig. 10FTwo-sample t testSix mice in each cohortt = −2.97580.0139
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 43 (17)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 43, Issue 17
26 Apr 2023
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Masthead (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Effects of Stimulus Timing on the Acquisition of an Olfactory Working Memory Task in Head-Fixed Mice
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Effects of Stimulus Timing on the Acquisition of an Olfactory Working Memory Task in Head-Fixed Mice
Josefine Reuschenbach, Janine K. Reinert, Xiaochen Fu, Izumi Fukunaga
Journal of Neuroscience 26 April 2023, 43 (17) 3120-3130; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1636-22.2023

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Effects of Stimulus Timing on the Acquisition of an Olfactory Working Memory Task in Head-Fixed Mice
Josefine Reuschenbach, Janine K. Reinert, Xiaochen Fu, Izumi Fukunaga
Journal of Neuroscience 26 April 2023, 43 (17) 3120-3130; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1636-22.2023
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • learning
  • olfaction
  • speed-accuracy trade-off
  • working memory

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Articles

  • NaX channel is a physiological [Na+] detector in oxytocin and vasopressin releasing magnocellular neurosecretory cells of the rat supraoptic nucleus
  • Decoding reach direction in early “visual” cortex of congenitally blind individuals
  • Evidence that ultrafast non-quantal transmission underlies synchronized vestibular action potential generation
Show more Research Articles

Behavioral/Cognitive

  • Decoding reach direction in early “visual” cortex of congenitally blind individuals
  • Signatures of Electrical Stimulation Driven Network Interactions in the Human Limbic System
  • Dissociable Neural Mechanisms Underlie the Effects of Attention on Visual Appearance and Response Bias
Show more Behavioral/Cognitive
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.