Abstract
The functional topography of the human primary somatosensory cortex hand area is a widely studied model system to understand sensory organization and plasticity. It is so far unclear whether the underlying 3D structural architecture also shows a topographic organization. We used 7 Tesla (7T) magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) data to quantify layer-specific myelin, iron, and mineralization in relation to population receptive field maps of individual finger representations in Brodman area 3b (BA 3b) of human S1 in female and male younger adults. This 3D description allowed us to identify a characteristic profile of layer-specific myelin and iron deposition in the BA 3b hand area, but revealed an absence of structural differences, an absence of low-myelin borders, and high similarity of 3D microstructure profiles between individual fingers. However, structural differences and borders were detected between the hand and face areas. We conclude that the 3D structural architecture of the human hand area is nontopographic, unlike in some monkey species, which suggests a high degree of flexibility for functional finger organization and a new perspective on human topographic plasticity.
Significance Statement
Using ultra-high-field MRI, we provide the first comprehensive in vivo description of the 3D structural architecture of the human BA 3b hand area in relation to functional population receptive field maps. High similarity of precise finger-specific 3D profiles, together with an absence of structural differences and an absence of low-myelin borders between individual fingers, reveals the 3D structural architecture of the human hand area to be nontopographic. This suggests reduced structural limitations to cortical plasticity and reorganization and allows for shared representational features across fingers.
Introduction
In the mammalian brain, the topographic architecture of the primary somatosensory cortex (S1) is often studied as a model system to understand cortical functional organization and plasticity (Florence et al., 1997; Feldman and Brecht, 2005; Kuehn and Pleger, 2020). Because of their clear and fine-grained architectures, the most studied topographic areas within S1 are whisker representations in rodents (Feldman and Brecht, 2005; Petersen, 2007) and finger representations in monkeys and humans (Shoham and Grinvald, 2001; Blake et al., 2002; Pleger et al., 2016; Kuehn et al., 2018a). Although the microstructural features of these areas in relation to functional map architectures have been widely studied in rodents and monkeys (Meyer et al., 2013; Welker, 1976; Welker and Woolsey, 1974; Peters, 2009; Qi and Kaas, 2004; Jain et al., 1998), this remains undescribed in humans. This knowledge gap prevents us from gaining a full understanding of the neuronal mechanisms that underlie topographic organization and plasticity in humans.
Physiological studies show that low-myelin borders (so-called septa) separate major body part representations (Manger et al., 1997) and finger representations in Brodmann area 3b (BA 3b, homolog of rodent S1) of macaque monkeys (Qi and Kaas, 2004) and owl monkeys (Jain et al., 1998), as well as whisker representations in rodents (Woolsey and Van der Loos, 1970). The role of such septa is complex and ranges from functional separation to sensory encoding; it has also been suggested that septa may limit cortical plasticity (Sereno, 2005). Low-myelin borders have been identified in human BA 3b between major body part representations (Glasser et al., 2016; Kuehn et al., 2017). However, it is so far unknown whether low-myelin borders also separate functional single-finger representations in human BA 3b or whether the human hand area can be regarded as one structurally homogeneous cortical field (Sereno et al., 2022), where (1) single finger septa and (2) structural gradients (previously related to large-scale functional organization; Huntenburg et al., 2018; Paquola et al., 2019; Valk et al., 2022; Wagstyl et al., 2015) are absent, and (3) single finger representations appear structurally similar.
Conceptually, this question is important because it touches on a fundamental aspect of brain organization. In the somatosensory system, unlike in the visual system, topographic representations are discontinuous. That is, single finger representations (Schweizer et al., 2008; Martuzzi et al., 2014; Schellekens et al., 2021) and major body part representations (Sereno et al., 2022), such as the hand and the face, are distinctly represented as has been shown with functional MRI (fMRI). This representation of body parts reflects the discontinuous shape of the body in the real world, where the hand and the face, for example, are spatially distant (and can move independently), although they cover neighboring areas in S1. Therefore, it has been suggested that low-myelin borders in BA 3b separate representations that are nearby in the cortex but distant in the real world (Kuehn et al., 2017), such as the hand and face. Alternatively, low-myelin borders may separate representations that are nearby in the cortex and nearby in the real world but that sometimes receive distinct sensory inputs, such as individual fingers. Answering this question will facilitate relating cortical myeloarchitectonic features to real world features.
We here used parcellation-inspired techniques to characterize finger-specific structural features of the hand representation in human BA 3b, not only along the surface (two dimensions) but also in depth (three dimensions). Quantitative MRI proxies were used to estimate layer-specific myelin, diamagnetic and paramagnetic tissue substances (such as calcium and iron), and overall mineralization. Participants underwent a tactile stimulation protocol to obtain population receptive field (pRF) characteristics to investigate whether the 3D structural architecture of the human hand area is homogenous or whether there are systematic microstructural differences between BA 3b finger representations. Targeting these questions provides us with critical information on topographic stability and/or plasticity in conditions of health and disease.
fMRI data in this paper have been published in part previously in Liu et al. (2021).
Materials and Methods
Participants
Twenty-four healthy volunteers underwent 7T MRI. Because of severe head motion during scanning, 4 participants were excluded from the study, leaving a total of 20 participants for analysis (10 females; mean age, 25 ± 3 years). The participant number was motivated by previous layer-dependent 7T MRI studies using quantitative in vivo proxies to describe the structural architecture of human BA 3b (Dinse et al., 2015; Kuehn et al., 2017) and is well above previously reported sample sizes. According to the Edinburgh Handedness Questionnaire (Oldfield, 1971), all participants were right-handed (laterality index ranging from +33 to +100, mean = 82 ± 21 SD). Chronic illness, central acting medications, and MRI contraindications (e.g., active implants, nonremovable metallic objects, claustrophobia, tinnitus or hearing impairments, consumption of alcohol/drugs) were a priori exclusion criteria.
Participants showed no anomalies of sensory perception (e.g., numbness, tingling sensations) or motor movement (e.g., reduced motor control, restricted finger movement), and no diagnoses of diabetes or hypertension. No professional musicians participated, given evidence of enlarged cortical hand representations and superior tactile perception in string and piano players (Elbert et al., 1995; Ragert et al., 2004; Schwenkreis et al., 2007). Finally, none of the participants showed signs of cognitive impairments as indicated by the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (Nasreddine et al., 2005; mean = 29 ± 1 SD, scores ranging from 26 to 30). Participants were recruited from the database of the German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases, Magdeburg, Germany. All participants gave written informed consent and were paid for their participation. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Otto-von-Guericke University Magdeburg.
General procedure
All participants took part in two appointments, (1) a structural MRI session and (2) a functional MRI session (Fig. 1 shows the experimental design and analysis pipeline of MRI data.).
MRI assessment
MR Sequences
MRI data were acquired at a 7T MAGNETOM scanner (Siemens Healthcare) equipped with a 32-Channel Nova Medical Head Coil. First, magnetization-prepared 2 rapid acquisition gradient echo (MP2RAGE; Marques et al., 2010) whole-brain images were acquired at 0.7 mm isotropic resolution [240 sagittal slices, field of view read = 224 mm, repetition time = 4800 ms, echo time = 2.01 ms, inversion time TI1/TI2 = 900/2750 ms, flip angle = 5°/3°, bandwidth = 250 Hz/pixel, Generalized Autocalibrating Partially Parallel Acquisition (GRAPPA) 2], and MP2RAGE part brain images (covering the sensorimotor cortex) were acquired subsequently at 0.5 mm isotropic resolution (208 transversal slices, field of view read = 224 mm, repetition time = 4800 ms, echo time = 2.62 ms, inversion time TI1/TI2 = 900/2750 ms, flip angle = 5°/3°, bandwidth = 250 Hz/pixel, GRAPPA 2, phase oversampling = 0%, slice oversampling = 7.7%). Then, we acquired uncombined susceptibility-weighted imaging (SWI; Haacke et al., 2004) data with part brain coverage of sensorimotor cortex at 0.5 mm isotropic resolution using a 3D gradient-echo pulse sequence (208 transversal slices, field of view read = 192 mm, repetition time = 22 ms, echo time = 9.00 ms, flip angle = 10°, bandwidth = 160 Hz/pixel, GRAPPA 2, phase oversampling = 0%, slice oversampling = 7.7%). The total scanning time was ∼60 min. Functional data were collected in a separate scanning session. First, shimming was performed before two echo planar imaging (EPI) readouts with opposite phase-encoding (PE) polarity were acquired. Functional EPI sequences (gradient echo) were acquired using the following parameters: voxel resolution of 1 mm isotropic, field of view read = 192 mm, repetition time = 2000 ms, echo time = 22 ms, GRAPPA 4, interleaved acquisition, 36 slices.
fMRI task
Five independently controlled MR-compatible piezoelectric stimulators (QuaeroSys; http://www.quaerosys.com) were used to stimulate the five fingertips of the right hand of the participants in the scanner (Schweisfurth et al., 2015, 2014, 2011). A stimulator was attached to each fingertip of the right hand, using a custom-built, metal-free applicator to adjust to individual hand and finger sizes. Each stimulator consisted of 8 individually controlled pins arranged in a 2 × 4 matrix, covering 2.5 × 9 mm2 of skin (Fig. 1). Vibrotactile stimulation was applied at a frequency of 16 Hz (Schweizer et al., 2008), and stimulation intensity was adjusted individually for each participant and each finger to 2.5 times the individual tactile detection thresholds. To minimize adaptation-related variation in map activity between participants, two randomly selected pins were raised one at a time, yielding 16 pin combinations per second (Schweisfurth et al., 2015, 2014, 2011).
Participants first underwent two phase-encoded protocols (runs 1 and 2 of the experiment), which included 2 runs of 20 cycles each. Each cycle lasted 25.6 s where each fingertip was stimulated once for 5.12 s. Stimulation was applied either in a forward [thumb (D1) to little finger (D5)] or in a reverse order (D5 to D1 Fig. 1). Half of the participants started with the forward run, and the other half started with the reverse run. One run consisted of 256 scans (512 s for a TR of 2 s) and lasted 8 min and 31 s. Participants were instructed to covertly count short randomly distributed pauses during the tactile stimulation (duration 180 ms). We used the same number of gaps per finger, resulting in 15 gaps in total per run. Participants then underwent the blocked-design protocol (runs 3 and 4 of the experiment), which included six conditions, stimulation to D1, D2 (index finger), D3 (middle finger), D4 (ring finger), D5, and a rest condition with no stimulation. The same task instructions and stimulation protocol as described for the phase-encoded paradigm was used, although fingers were stimulated in a pseudorandom way in that fingers were not stimulated more than two times in a row. Between two subsequent stimulations, there was a 2 s pause (in 70% of the trials), or a 6 s pause (in 30% of the trials), which was counterbalanced across fingers. Each finger was stimulated 10 times. One run comprised 208 scans and lasted 6.56 min. The blocked-design run was repeated twice. Subsequently, two runs were recorded, where a 1-TR stimulation of all five fingers was followed by an 11-TR resting phase without any stimulation. The sequence was repeated 10 times for both runs. Altogether, functional measurements took ∼40 min.
MRI analyses
Structural data processing
Preprocessing
Structural data quality was evaluated by two independent raters, and data showing severe artifacts (i.e., of n = 4 participants) were excluded. We only used data showing mild truncation artifacts (not affecting S1) or no artifacts at all. Quantitative susceptibility maps (QSMs) were reconstructed from coil-uncombined SWI magnitude and phase images using the Bayesian multiscale dipole inversion algorithm (Acosta-Cabronero et al., 2018), implemented in the software qsmbox (version 2.0; https://gitlab.com/acostaj/QSMbox) written for MATLAB (R2017b, MathWorks). Structural MP2RAGE and QSM images were then processed using JIST (Java Image Science Toolkit; Lucas et al., 2010) and CBS Tools (Bazin et al., 2014) as plug-ins for the research application Medical Image Processing, Analysis, and Visualization (MIPAV; https://mipav.cit.nih.gov/).
Registration
First, the quantitative T1 map (qT1) and QSM slab images were coregistered to the upsampled (0.7 to 0.5 mm isotropic) whole-brain qT1 image. To precisely register the qT1 slab image onto the resampled whole-brain qT1 image, we combined linear transformation (MIPAV version 7.3.0; Optimized Automated Registration algorithm, 6 degrees of freedom, cost function of correlation ratio) and nonlinear deformation [Advanced Normalization Tools (ANTs) release 1.9.x; Avants et al., 2011; embedded in cbstools wrapper Embedded Syn, cost function of cross-correlation] in one single step, using nearest neighbor interpolation. For registration of the QSM slab image, we applied a combination of rigid and affine automated registration using the software ITK-SNAP (version 3.6.0; www.itksnap.org). The registration quality of resulting qT1 and QSM slab images was evaluated by two independent raters. The generated registration matrix for the qT1 slab image was then applied to the T1-weighted uniform image (UNI) and to the second inversion image.
Segmentation
qT1 slab and whole-brain images were fused using a weighted combination of images, resulting in one whole-brain structural image with improved resolution in the sensorimotor cortex. Using the merged images, brain surrounding tissues (i.e., skull and dura mater) were removed, and resulting brain masks were manually refined (using both the qT1 and UNI images) to ensure that all nonbrain matter was removed from the region of interest. The cortex was then segmented using the UNI image as input for the TOADS (topology-preserving, anatomy-driven segmentation) algorithm (Bazin and Pham, 2007) to estimate tissue membership probability of each voxel. The results were used as input for the CRUISE (cortical reconstruction using implicit surface evolution) algorithm (Han et al., 2004) to estimate the inner and outer gray matter (GM) borders, that is, to the white matter (WM) and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF), respectively. The resulting level set images (surfaces in Cartesian space using a level set framework, Sethian, 1999) were optimized to precisely match S1 by thresholding the maximum values of the inner and outer level set images to −2.8 and −0.2, respectively.
Layering and surface mapping
The level set images were then used to generate individual surfaces for cortical mapping. Intracortical qT1 and QSM values, used as proxies for myelin (Stüber et al., 2014) and mineralization (Acosta-Cabronero et al., 2016, 2018; Betts et al., 2016), respectively, were estimated in reference to individual cortical folding patterns using the validated equivolume model (Waehnert et al., 2014). The cortical sheath was initially divided into 21 level set surfaces where we sampled qT1 and QSM values to derive cortical depth-dependent profiles. Finally, the extracted values were mapped onto the individual's inflated cortical surface (method of closest point; Tosun et al., 2004). Note that for cortical depth-dependent mapping of quantitative values, we used the nonmerged high-resolution qT1 and QSM slab data to ensure high-data quality. We extracted three different parameter maps from the QSM data—negative QSM (nQSM), positive QSM (pQSM), and absolute QSM (aQSM)—to estimate information on different underlying tissue properties (diamagnetic tissue contrast for nQSM, paramagnetic tissue contrast for pQSM, level of mineralization for aQSM).
Extracted cortical depth-dependent profiles were further averaged into fewer compartments following two different approaches; (1) To ensure comparability of our results to previous data, the three most superficial and the two deepest profiles were removed to reduce partial volume effects (Tardif et al., 2015) before the remaining 16 profiles were averaged into four equally spaced cortical depth compartments (hereafter referred to as “equally spaced layers”, superficial, outer middle, inner middle, deep), and (2) we followed previous ex vivo in vivo validation studies in S1 that allow definition of anatomically relevant cortical compartments from ultra-high-resolution MRI data (Dinse et al., 2015; Fig. 2). We used previously validated myelin profiles of BA 3b (Dinse et al., 2015) to identify cortical compartments based on averaged qT1 profiles (at the group level) by plotting modeled histological data as well as in vivo MRI data of Dinse et al. (2015) in reference to our in vivo MRI data. Calculating minima and maxima of the first derivative of the initial qT1 profile (including 21 compartments) allowed us to extract three data-driven cortical-depth compartments that are anatomically relevant. After removing the two deepest layers (where qT1 stabilized and a plateau was reached; Fig. 2), the remaining 19 compartments were averaged into an inner (eight deepest layers), middle (seven layers), and outer (four most superficial layers) cortical-depth compartment, where based on Dinse et al. (2015) the input layer IV is located in the middle compartment, and the deep layers V/VI are located in the inner compartment. Analyses are shown both for three anatomically relevant layer compartments and for four equally spaced layer compartments.
Anatomical definition of BA 3b
We manually generated subject-specific BA 3b masks of the left hemisphere based on anatomical landmarks in the qT1 images (Geyer et al., 1999; Yousry et al., 1997), using the software ITK-SNAP (version 3.6.0). Masks were drawn from the edge (beginning of the crown) of the postcentral gyrus to the fundus of the central sulcus (to make sure to include everything of BA 3b), covering the anterior wall of the postcentral gyrus (Geyer et al., 1999). All slices were masked in which the knob-like (axial slices) or the hook-like shape (sagittal slices) of the motor hand area was visible (Yousry et al., 1997). Adjustments were made where required until the surface mask presented as homogenous and without holes.
To specifically reduce the effect of possible segmentation errors on surface-based gradient and between-finger septa analyses (explained below), more conservative versions of our manually generated BA 3b masks were created in 15 cases (P01, P02, P03, P04, P07, P08, P10, P11, P13, P14, P15, P16, P19, P20) where structural and/or functional data indicated that we may have sampled a few vertices of BA1. This refinement allowed us to restrict geodesic paths used for gradient and between-finger septa analyses to BA 3b (see Fig. 6) and to minimize the influence of neighboring subregions on the results (i.e., lower myelin content in BA 1; Glasser et al., 2016; Glasser and Van Essen, 2011; Sánchez-Panchuelo et al., 2014). However, these more conservative masks might also result in missing parts of BA 3b (and were therefore only applied to the gradient and between-finger septa analyses). In line with previous parcellation approaches (Glasser et al., 2016; Glasser and Van Essen, 2011), we identified the border between BA 3b and BA 1 of S1 based on a drop in myelin (increase in qT1 values) along the edge of the rostral bank of the postcentral gyrus. Note that a clear sharp border cannot be detected in individual myelin-sensitive MR images (Sánchez-Panchuelo et al., 2014), which may be because of the presence of transition zones (Geyer et al., 1999). Therefore, an exact delineation of S1 subregions cannot not be achieved, which is why an operational definition based on anatomic landmarks extracted from cytoarchitectonic and multimodal parcellation studies was chosen in the present study.
Functional data processing
Distortion correction of opposite polarity EPIs was performed using a point spread function (PSF) mapping (In et al., 2016). To account for differences in the amount of spatial information between the opposite PE EPIs, a weighted combination of the two distortion-corrected images was applied to generate the final corrected image. The EPI images of the functional scans were motion corrected to time point zero. PSF mapping was applied to the motion-corrected images to perform geometrically accurate image reconstruction. Functional time series were slice-time corrected to account for differences in image acquisition time between slices using SPM8 (Statistical Parametric Mapping, Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, University College London). Slice-time corrected functional data resulting from phase-encoded protocols were concatenated.
Registration of functional data
Functional time series were semimanually registered to the MP2RAGE qT1 image using ITK-SNAP (version 3.6.0, nonrigid transformation, 9 degrees of freedom). Resulting registration matrices were applied to the corresponding functional parameter maps (i.e., t-maps, pRF estimates) in a single step (ANTs, version 2.1.0, nearest neighbor interpolation). The inverse of the resulting registration matrices were used to transform individual ROI masks from structural into functional data space (ANTs, version 2.1.0, nearest neighbor interpolation), allowing us to perform ROI analysis on nonsmoothed functional data.
General linear model analysis of blocked design data
We used the general linear model (GLM) as implemented in SPM8 to individually calculate fixed-effects models on the first level of the two blocked-design runs (runs 3 and 4 of the experiment; Fig. 1). Because each finger was treated individually and independently, BOLD activation driven by tactile stimulation of each finger was included in the quantification as an independent measure (Kuehn et al., 2018a; Stringer et al., 2014). Each session was modeled with five regressors of interest (stimulation to D1, D2, D3, D4, D5) and allowed the computation of five linear contrast estimates, Touch to D1, D2, D3, D4, and D5 [e.g., the contrast (−1 4 −1 −1 −1) for touch to D2].
Bayesian pRF modeling
pRF modeling was performed on the phase-encoded fMRI data following the same procedure as introduced by Liu et al. (2021), incorporating the SPM-based BayespRF Toolbox (https://github.com/pzeidman/BayespRF) written for MATLAB (SPM12 and MATLAB R2017b). We performed a two-stage analysis. First, a first level GLM analysis was conducted using SPM12 to prepare the data for pRF modeling by reducing the number of voxel time courses. At this stage, the task regressors were defined. Five regressors were constructed, corresponding to the five fingers of the right hand. Only time-series data that passed a significance threshold of p < 0.05 uncorrected were taken forward for pRF modeling (Zeidman et al., 2018; Puckett et al., 2020). pRF modeling was restricted to BA 3b (using the above described BA 3b masks) to reduce the computing time (Note that pRF modeling of one participant takes ∼24 h for the given input data, i.e., it would take several days to compute whole brain data of one participant.). pRF modeling was then conducted on a voxel-by-voxel basis to optimize the fit between an estimated waveform and the empirically measured BOLD response by modifying the size and position of the pRF model. We thresholded the posterior model probability at >0.95 (Liu et al., 2021; Puckett et al., 2020; Zeidman et al., 2018). To define the somatosensory space, the dimensions of the 2D matrix were limited to ±12.5. Finally, pRF modeling was performed on the inferior-to-superior dimension (x dimension) of topographic alignment. The minimal pRF size was restricted to 1/10th of the sensory space occupied by a single fingertip, whereas the maximum pRF size was restricted to the equivalence of all five fingers (i.e., 25 units; Liu et al., 2021). A Gaussian pRF profile was chosen as the response function for the pRF analysis (code available at https://gitlab.com/pengliu1120/bayesian-prf-modelling.git). The model was characterized by a normal, excitatory distribution with pRF center location (x) and pRF width (i.e., σ, the SD of the Gaussian profile) as parameter estimates. The extracted distance parameter was used to define the digit ROIs (locations of activated voxels for each finger), whereas the extracted width parameter was used as the pRF size estimate of activated voxels. Because the stimulus space was one dimensional, only pRF distance (i.e., center location) and pRF size parameters were analyzed in surface space.
Localizing single fingers in BA 3b
Resulting pRF center location maps were used to locate single finger representations in BA 3b. We applied a winner-takes-all approach to the pRF center location maps to ensure vertices are sampled only once (i.e., excluding overlapping map areas between finger representations). Vertices of overlapping areas (introduced by splitting pRF maps into five single finger maps and mapping single finger maps onto the inflated surfaces) were exclusively assigned to one area by taking the highest variance (explained by the pRF model) as criterion. The hand area was defined by combining the five fingers to one larger ROI.
Localizing the face-hand area in BA 3b
A subsample (n = 16 participants) also underwent functional 7T MRI [gradient-echo EPI sequence of 1.5 mm isotropic resolution with part-brain coverage of motor cortex (M1) and S1] while carrying out motor movements of the tongue and the fingers to locate the face-hand area in M1 (Northall et al., 2023). These movement-related data were used to locate the face and hand areas in BA 3b. After the functional data were manually registered to the MP2RAGE qT1 image based on anatomical landmarks (ITK-SNAP, version 3.6.0, nonrigid transformation, 9 degrees of freedom), functional activation maps of tongue and finger movements were estimated (peak clusters of t values, thresholded at p < 0.01 with a minimum cluster size of k = 3) using the GLM as implemented in SPM12 (first-level analysis based on contrast estimates for each body part; Northall et al., 2023 contains details). A winner-takes-all approach was applied to the resulting localizers to ensure vertices are sampled only once (i.e., excluding overlapping map areas between body part representations).
Surface mapping of functional parameters
Registered functional parameters were mapped onto the individual surfaces in anatomical space using the method of closest point (CBS Tools; Bazin et al., 2014; Surface Mesh Mapping algorithm), and BA 3b masks were applied. To minimize the effect of superficial veins on BOLD signal change, the most superficial 20% of cortical values were disregarded, and the mean value of the remaining layers (20–100% cortical depth) was used to perform statistical analyses. Note that no cortical depth-dependent analyses were performed for the functional data.
Extracting microstructural profiles
We sampled qT1, nQSM, pQSM, and aQSM values perpendicular to the cortical sheet in 21 different cortical depths from the pRF center locations of the five fingers and from the face-hand area in BA 3b. Within subjects, extracted values were averaged across vertices (resulting in one value per location/body part and depth). We then calculated the first and the second derivative of resulting qT1 vectors using the gradient function implemented in MATLAB (R2017b). Minima and maxima of the first derivative were estimated by finding the zero locations in the second derivative.
Extraction of structural gradients
We analyzed qT1, signed QSM, and aQSM values in relation to geodesic distances to account for local and individual differences in cortical curvature. Geodesic distances and quantitative structural values (qT1, signed QSM, aQSM) were extracted from BA 3b along the shortest path between the D1 activation peak and the D5 activation peak (19/20 participants, based on blocked design data), using the Dijkstra algorithm as implemented in PyVista for Python (Sullivan and Kaszynski, 2019). In one case, where D1 and D2 activation maps appeared in reversed inferior-to superior order (P07), the shortest path was calculated between the D2 activation peak and the D5 activation peak. Extracted quantitative values and geodesic distances were used to perform statistical analyses.
Septa analysis
To investigate whether between-finger septa also exist in human BA 3b, a surface-based mapping approach combining functional and structural data were used (Kuehn et al., 2017). Layer-specific qT1 values and functional activity (t values generated based on blocked design data) were sampled along predefined paths between neighboring finger representations. In particular, we compared qT1 values extracted from peak locations (maximum t value of single finger contrast restricted by corresponding pRF map) with qT1 values extracted from border locations (intersection point of t value vectors sampled along the shortest path between peak locations of neighboring BA 3b finger representations; similar approach to Kuehn et al., 2017). We used the Dijkstra algorithm as implemented in PyVista for Python (Sullivan and Kaszynski, 2019) to calculate the shortest path between peak locations of neighboring BA 3b finger representations (D1–D2, D2–D3, D3–D4, D4–D5; as well as between D2–D1 and D1–D3 in one case where locations of D1 and D2 finger maps were reversed). qT1 values of n = 20 younger adults were therefore sampled from nine different locations (five peaks, four borders) at three different cortical depths (inner, middle, outer).
Additionally, we implemented an analysis that uses multidimensional sampling (inferior to superior and anterior to posterior) to detect potential low-myelin borders in surface-based qT1 patterns. This analysis is independent of the exact functional crossing vertex and therefore potentially more robust. To realize this approach, we chose the peak activation area of the thumb (as identified using pRF mapping) as a seed region. We then sampled multiple geodesic paths superior and inferior from this seed region, using the Dijkstra algorithm as implemented in PyVista for Python (Sullivan and Kaszynski, 2019). Start and end points of these paths were extracted along the y-axis of finger activation peaks (D1 and D2), as well as 10 mm below the D1 activation peak (geodesic distance on the shortest path between the D1 activation peak and the face activation peak). This 10 mm distance was estimated based on the expected location of the upper face (in particular the forehead; Root et al., 2022). Only those vertices that were scattered within one vertex-to-vertex distance (∼0.26 mm) around the y-axis were considered. Please note that in two cases (P11, P15), where sampling along the y-axis did not match the underlying surface-based qT1 pattern, start and end points of the geodesic paths were defined along the x-axis. For each participant we sampled five equally spaced geodesic paths (running from the approximated forehead representation via the thumb to the index finger representation; see Fig. 8A). Second, we extracted qT1 values from middle cortex depth (where low-myelin borders in BA 3b have been detected previously; Kuehn et al., 2017) and used 3D plotting together with sequential color mapping to visualize the underlying qT1 pattern along the extracted paths (see Fig. 8B).
Similarity analysis
To investigate similarity of finger-specific 3D structural profiles we calculated the Fréchet distance between all possible finger pairs individually for each participant using the Fréchet Distance Calculator (version 2.0, https://de.mathworks.com/matlabcentral/fileexchange/41956-frechet-distance-calculator) as implemented in MATLAB (version R2022a). The Fréchet distance is a measure of (dis)similarity between two curves in space that reflects the shortest path length sufficient to traverse both curves from start to finish while taking the course of the curve into account (Alt and Godau, 1995). When both curves are perfectly aligned, the value of the Fréchet distance is zero.
Statistics
Statistical analyses were computed in R software (version 3.4.4, https://www.r-project.org/). All sample distributions were analyzed for outliers using box plot methods and tested for normality using the Shapiro–Wilk test. In case of non-normal data, nonparametric or robust tests were conducted. For all tests, the significance level was set to p = 0.05. Bonferroni-corrected significance levels were applied for multiple testing to correct for familywise error accumulation. Pearson correlation coefficient r was calculated as an effect size estimator for Student's t tests (Field et al., 2012). We applied Cohen's criteria of 0.3 and 0.5 for a medium and large effect, respectively (Cohen, 1988). Eta squared (η2) was calculated as an effect size estimator for ANOVAs. We applied two-sided tests, except for clear one-sided hypotheses (i.e., testing for low-myelin borders).
To test for the existence of inferior-to-superior structural gradients in the human BA 3b hand area (potential linear relationships between geodesic distances and quantitative structural values), we used multilevel linear regression models to predict layer-specific qT1, signed QSM, and aQSM values from normalized geodesic distances (fixed effect) while controlling for the effect of individuals (random effect with random intercept and random slope). To conduct this analysis, we used the lme function (method maximum likelihood) from the nlme package (version 3.1–155, https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=nlme) as implemented in R (version 3.4.4, https://www.r-project.org/).
In addition, we fitted Bayesian linear multilevel models using the brm function of the brms package (version 2.18.0, Bürkner, 2017; Bürkner, 2018; Carpenter et al., 2017) as implemented in R (version 4.2.2, https://www.R-project.org/). We used the same fixed and random effects as described before for the frequentist approach. The prior distribution of the intercept for layer-dependent qT1 was informed by previous 7T MRI studies (Dinse et al., 2015; Kuehn et al., 2017). Based on this information, we chose normal prior distributions with different means and SDs for different layers (outer, mean = 2058, SD = 483; middle, mean = 1770, SD = 294; inner, mean = 1703, SD = 183). Previously reported values were averaged and rounded to whole numbers, SDs were multiplied by three to allow variation while accounting for possible outliers in the data. The prior for the effect of geodesic distance on qT1 was set to normal (mean = 0 for all layers, same SDs as reported for the intercepts). Signed QSM and aQSM priors were also informed by previous studies (Acosta-Cabronero et al., 2018, Donatelli et al., 2022). Signed QSM values were allowed to vary between −0.125 and 0.125, aQSM values were allowed to vary between 0 and 0.125. The mean of the prior distribution was set to 0.5 times the possible value range, the SD to 0.25 times the possible value range. We chose normal prior distributions for the intercept and the effect of geodesic distance on signed QSM (mean = 0, SD = 0.06) and aQSM (mean = 0.06, SD = 0.03) for all layers. Because we were interested in a linear relationship between structural measures (qT1, signed QSM, aQSM) and geodesic distance, the Gaussian family with the identity link was chosen as response distribution (Bürkner, 2017). All models were fitted using four chains with 2000 iterations each; the first 1000 of which were used to calibrate the sampler, resulting in a total of 4000 posterior samples (Bürkner, 2017).
To compare skewness and kurtosis of microstructure profiles between fingers (D1, D2, D3, D4, D5) as well as to compare Fréchet distances of microstructure profiles between pairs of neighboring fingers (D1, D2; D2, D3; D3, D4; D4, D5), one-way repeated-measures ANOVAs on qT1, nQSM, pQSM, and aQSM were computed given normality. In case of non-normal data, we conducted robust repeated-measures ANOVAs based on 20%-trimmed means, which were performed using the rmanova function from the WRS2 package (version 1.1–3, Mair and Wilcox, 2020) in R.
To test for layer-specific differences in qT1, nQSM, pQSM, and aQSM between finger representations, we calculated two-way repeated-measures ANOVAs with layer (inner, middle, outer) and location (D1, D2, D3, D4, D5) as within-subjects factors. In case of sphericity violations, Greenhouse–Geisser corrected results (when violations to sphericity were large, i.e., ε < 0.75) or Huynd–Feldt corrected results (when violations to sphericity were small, i.e., ε ≥ 0.75) were computed. Post hoc tests were performed as two-tailed paired-samples tests. Nonparametric Wilcoxon signed rank tests were additionally calculated where conditions of the parametric one-sample t test were violated. For sample distributions containing extreme outliers (values above the third quartile plus three times the interquartile range or values below the first quartile minus three times the interquartile range), two-way repeated-measures ANOVAs were computed both with and without extreme outliers included. Please note that to offer data that can more easily be compared with previous studies (Kuehn et al., 2017; Tardif et al., 2015), we also conducted the analyses using the equally spaced layer definition, which, however, may not reflect anatomically relevant cortical compartments.
To test for layer-specific differences in qT1, nQSM, pQSM, and aQSM between the face and the hand representation, we calculated two-way repeated-measures ANOVAs with layer (inner, middle, outer) and body part (face, hand) as within-subjects factors using the same correction methods and post hoc tests as described above.
To investigate whether between-finger septa also exist in human BA 3b, we performed layerwise comparisons on peak-to-border differences of qT1 values by applying Bayesian paired-samples t tests [resulting in12 tests given four different peak-to-border conditions (D1–D2, D2–D3, D3–D4, D4–D5) and three different cortical depths (inner, middle, outer)]. For this purpose, qT1 values were averaged across neighboring finger representations for each peak-to-border condition (i.e., D1–D2 average; D2–D3 average; D3–D4 average; D4–D5 average). The JASP software package (version 0.14.3, https://jasp-stats.org/) was used to calculate Bayesian paired-samples t tests with location (peak, border) as within-subjects factor. We used a Bayes factor favoring the alternative hypothesis. The default Cauchy-scaled prior of 0.707 was selected instead of modeling the expected effect size. However, we simulated the effect of the prior on the Bayes factor for a wider range of prior width to estimate how robust the conclusions were to the chosen prior.
Results
Cortical 3D profiles and layer definition
To study the 3D structural architecture of the human hand representation in BA 3b in reference to functional topography, we used qT1 as a proxy for cortical myelin (Stüber et al., 2014; Dinse et al., 2015) and QSMs as proxies for iron (pQSM values), diamagnetic tissue substances (nQSM values; Langkammer et al., 2012; Zheng et al., 2013; Hametner et al., 2018), and overall mineralization (aQSM values; Betts et al., 2016). In addition to anatomical scans, functional scans were acquired when participants were stimulated at the fingertips of their right hand by an automated piezoelectric stimulator, using both phase-encoded and blocked designs. We focused on investigating left BA 3b (contralateral to where tactile stimulation was applied). pRF center locations as revealed by Bayesian pRF modeling were used to locate each finger in BA 3b (thumb, D1; index finger, D2; middle finger, D3; ring finger, D4; little finger, D5; Liu et al., 2021).
When extracting qT1, nQSM, pQSM, and aQSM values from the hand area in BA 3b, we obtained precise intracortical contrasts (Fig. 3A). As expected (Stüber et al., 2014; Dinse et al., 2015), qT1 values decrease from superficial to deep cortical depths, reflecting high myelin close to the WM. This intracortical gradient from low to high myelin is interrupted in the upper half of the profile, an area known to contain high myelin content (i.e., Baillarger band in anatomical layer IV; Vogt and Vogt, 1919; Dinse et al., 2015). Based on previous in vivo ex vivo validation work (Dinse et al., 2015), we used the local rate of change of qT1 values to define three anatomically relevant cortical compartments (hereafter referred to as “outer layer,” “middle layer,” and “inner layer”; Fig. 3B,C), where input layer IV is assumed to be located in the middle layer, and layers V and VI are assumed to be located in the inner layer. Note that the upper peak of the pQSM profile (which showed a top hat distribution shaped by a plateau with a double peak) is located in what we define as middle layer (containing layer IV), whereas the lower peak is located in what we define as inner layer (containing layer V). With respect to nQSM, we observed a u-shaped profile with values closest to zero in the very middle of cortical depths, which is interrupted by a small plateau in the upper half of the profile where we expected layer IV to be located. Multicontrast mapping therefore confirmed our qT1-based layer definition. Statistical results are shown both for three layers (outer layer, middle layer, inner layer) and for four equally spaced layers to facilitate comparing our data with previous publications (Kuehn et al., 2017; Tardif et al., 2015).
Absence of inferior-to-superior structural gradients in the BA 3b hand area
First, we investigated whether there is a systematic structural gradient within the hand area of BA 3b (e.g., higher myelin in superior compared with inferior parts of the hand area). To this end, we extracted layer-specific qT1, signed QSM, and aQSM values along geodesic paths from inferior to superior and calculated multilevel linear regression models (fixed effect, normalized geodesic distance; random effect, individual). None of the tested models were significant (Table 1, Fig. 4), indicating an absence of a systematic inferior-to-superior structural gradient in the hand area in BA 3b, which was also confirmed for four equally spaced layers (Table 1), and by calculating Bayesian multilevel models (Table 2).
Absence of low-myelin borders between finger representations in BA 3b
Next, we targeted the question on the presence or absence of low-myelin borders between finger representations in human BA 3b by comparing qT1 values sampled from the functional peak (highest t value) of each finger representation with qT1 values sampled from the functional border between neighboring finger representations. The shortest path was estimated using the surface-based Dijkstra-algorithm (Fig. 5A), which is analogous to the way low-myelin borders between the hand and the face were detected (Kuehn et al., 2017). We calculated layer-specific Bayesian probabilities (Bayes factors) for all neighboring finger pairs (D1–D2, D2–D3, D3–D4, D4–D5). Our results revealed that the alternative hypothesis (finger peak qT1 values < border qT1 values) was predicted a maximum 0.86 times better than the null hypothesis (no difference in qT1 values between peak and border) in any layer (Table 3, Fig. 5E). This provides evidence in favor of the null hypothesis of no difference, indicating an absence of low-myelin borders between finger representations in human BA 3b.
To align our approach as closely as possible with detection approaches of low-myelin borders in monkey species (Qi and Kaas, 2004), we explored individual qT1 maps of the middle layer visually (i.e., where low-myelin borders should be located; Kuehn et al., 2017). Stripe-like architectures within the hand area (similar to those described in BA 3b of macaque monkeys; Qi and Kaas, 2004) could be observed in 10/20 participants in the following locations (Fig. 6): in 2/20 participants between D1 and D2 (P01, P08), in 7/20 participants between D2 and D3 (P01, P10, P11, P15, P16, P18, P20), in 1/20 participants between D2 and D4 (P10), in 2/20 participants between D3 and D4 (P15, P17), and in 2/20 participants between D3 and D5 (P12, P16). Therefore, no systematic stripe-like representation (of potential low-myelin borders separating individual fingers) could be identified between specific finger pairs. But, as expected, low-myelin borders were clearly visible between the hand and the face areas in all participants (except P04; Fig. 7).
Moreover, we conducted an additional analysis that uses multidimensional sampling (inferior to superior and anterior to posterior) to detect potential low-myelin borders in surface-based qT1 patterns that is independent of the exact functional crossing vertex. qT1 values were sampled from middle cortical depth (where low-myelin borders in BA 3b have been previously detected; Kuehn et al., 2017) along multiple extracted geodesic paths (running from seed region D1 inferior to the upper face region and superior to the D2 region in BA 3b; Fig. 8A). Using 3D data extraction together with sequential color mapping revealed, as expected, systematic low-myelin patterns (Fig. 8B,C, lighter colors) between the upper face and the thumb region in 14/16 participants (P02, P03, P06, P07, P08, P09, P10, P12, P13, P15, P18, P20, and presumably P11 and P19; see below). These patterns are not only indicative of the location of the hand-face border but also of its shape (when comparing patterns of light-colored qT1 segments with the appearance of low-myelin regions on individual cortical surfaces, shown next to the 3D plots; Fig. 8B,C). For example, in some cases, the light-colored segments of the different paths (indicative of low-myelin regions) form a u-shape (P02, P03), whereas in other cases they appear in a hook-like shape (P08, P10).
In line with the results mentioned above, systematic low-myelin patterns (i.e., repetitive qT1 valleys, which are visible in a minimum number of two neighboring sampling paths) were absent between D1 and D2 (Fig. 8B,C), except for 1/16 participants (P01), where this participant has been identified with stripe-like patterns between D1 and D2 in Figure 6. In two other cases (P11, P19), extracted D1 sampling points presumably crossed the hand-face border (Fig. 8C); thus, detected low-myelin patterns between D1 and D2 may actually reflect the hand-face border. In these cases, a shift of the low-myelin pattern from inferior to superior in reference to D1 (anchor to calculate geodesic distances) can be observed along the anterior-to-posterior axis (i.e., across the sampled paths). We note that in one case (P20), where D1 and D2 sampling points almost overlapped (Fig. 8C), the chance to detect a true border in the qT1 signal may have been reduced.
Nontopographic 3D structural architecture of BA 3b hand area
To investigate whether finger representations differ in their 3D structural architecture, we compared layer-specific qT1, nQSM, pQSM, and aQSM between individual finger representations using an ANOVA with the factors finger (D1, D2, D3, D4, D5) and layer (outer, middle, inner). For qT1 values, the analysis revealed a significant main effect of layer (F(1.24,23.51) = 466.84, p = 7.6 × 10−18, η2 = 0.82) with inner layers being more myelinated (showing lower qT1 values) than middle layers (inner, 1658.25 ± 6.35, mean ± SEM; middle, 1883.79 ± 7.13, t(19) = 28.89, p = 3.7 × 10−17, r = 0.99), and middle layers being more myelinated than outer layers (outer, 2105.19 ± 11.79, t(19) = −14.58, p = 9.0 × 10−12, r = 0.96; W = 0, p = 1.9 × 10−6), reflecting the intracortical myelin gradient described above. However, there was neither a significant main effect of finger (F(4,76) = 1.54, p = 0.2, η2 = 0.01) nor a significant interaction effect between layer and finger (F(3.92,74.53) = 0.83, p = 0.51, η2 = 0.004; Fig. 9).
The same ANOVA computed on pQSM values also showed a significant main effect of layer (F(1.28,20.51) = 4.89, p = 0.03, η2 = 0.05), here driven by middle layers (likely encompassing the input layer IV) showing higher pQSM values (more iron) than inner layers (likely encompassing layers V and VI; middle, 0.010 ± 5.8 × 10−4; inner, 0.009 ± 5.0 × 10−4; t(16) = 3.84, p = 0.001, r = 0.69; W = 14, p = 0.002). Similar for qT1 values, there was neither a significant main effect of finger (F(1.89,30.2) = 0.32, p = 0.71, η2 = 0.01) nor a significant interaction between layer and finger (F(2.81,44.88) = 1.52, p = 0.23, η2 = 0.02). The same ANOVA on aQSM values also showed a significant main effect of layer (F(1.46,24.86) = 16.38, p = 1.1 × 10−4, η2 = 0.12), here driven by outer layers showing more mineralization than middle layers (outer, 0.011 ± 4.2 × 10−4; middle, 0.010 ± 5.0 × 10−4; t(17) = 3.09, p = 0.007, r = 0.60; W = 22, p = 0.004), and middle layers showing more mineralization than inner layers (inner, 0.009 ± 4.0 × 10−4, t(17) = 3.71, p = 0.002, r = 0.67; W = 19, p = 0.002). However, similar as for qT1 and pQSM, there was neither a significant main effect of finger (F(2.44,41.54) = 0.21, p = 0.85, η2 = 0.004) nor a significant interaction effect (F(3.59,61.04) = 1.07, p = 0.38, η2 = 0.02).
Finally, the same ANOVA on nQSM values again showed a main effect of layer (F(2,30) = 45.72, p = 7.8 × 10−10, η2 = 0.26), here driven by more negative nQSM values (higher diamagnetic contrast) in outer compared with middle layers (outer, −0.012 ± 5.4 × 10−4; middle, −0.008 ± 4.7 × 10−4; t(15) = −9.37, p = 1.2 × 10−7, r = 0.92; W = 0, p = 3.1 × 10−5). Again, there was no significant main effect of finger (F(4,60) = 0.97, p = 0.43, η2 = 0.02) but a significant interaction between layer and finger (F(3.94,59.06) = 2.78, p = 0.04, η2 = 0.05). This interaction was driven by more negative nQSM values (higher diamagnetic tissue contrast) in the inner layers for D1 compared with D4 (D1, −0.010 ± 6.7 × 10−4; D4, −0.007 ± 4.2 × 10−4; t(15) = −3.82, p = 0.002, r = 0.70) and for D1 compared with D5 (D5, −0.007 ± 5.3 × 10−4, t(15) = −3.13, p = 0.007, r = 0.63).
In addition, we calculated the same ANOVAs after removing extreme outliers from pQSM and aQSM data. Again, the results show significant main effects of layer for pQSM values (F(1.44,18.66) = 5.08, p = 0.26, η2 = 0.07) and aQSM values (F(2,30) = 29.47, p = 8.3 × 10−8, η2 = 0.24). We found higher pQSM values (higher iron content) in middle compared with inner layers (middle, 0.01 ± 4.7 × 10−4; inner, 0.008 ± 4.1 × 10−4; t(13) = 3.37, p = 0.005, r = 0.68), higher aQSM values (higher mineralization) in outer compared with middle layers (outer, 0.011 ± 4.5 × 10−4; middle, 0.009 ± 3.4 × 10−4; t(15) = 4.96, p = 1.7 × 10−4, r = 0.79) and in middle compared with inner layers (inner, 0.008 ± 2.4 × 10−4, t(15) = 3.80, p = 0.002, r = 0.7). Again, there was neither a significant main effect of finger (pQSM, F(2.41,31.35) = 1.07, p = 0.37, η2 = 0.03; aQSM, F(4,60) = 1.19, p = 0.33, η2 = 0.02), nor a significant interaction effect between layer and finger (pQSM, F(2.81,36.50) = 1.68, p = 0.19, η2 = 0.04; aQSM, F(3.81,57.22) = 1.53, p = 0.21, η2 = 0.03).
Finally, we repeated the same ANOVAs for four equally spaced layers (superficial, outer middle, inner middle, deep). Again, the results show significant main effects of layer for all tested substances (qT1, F(1.86,35.36) = 581.16, p = 2.4 × 10−27, η2 = 0.80; nQSM, F(1.89,28.28) = 21.03, p = 3.53 × 10−6, η2 = 0.15; pQSM, F(1.98,31.72) = 23.70, p = 5.4 × 10−7, η2 = 0.12; aQSM, F(2.13,36.25) = 5.16, p = 0.009, η2 = 0.03), but neither a significant main effect of finger (qT1, F(4,76) = 1.85, p = 0.13, η2 = 0.02; nQSM, F(2.60,39.06) = 2.31, p = 0.1, η2 = 0.05; pQSM, F(2.22,35.56) = 0.41, p = 0.69, η2 = 0.01; aQSM, F(2.19,37.24) = 0.30, p = 0.77, η2 = 0.01), nor a significant interaction between layer and finger (qT1, F(3.86,73.41) = 1.06, p = 0.38, η2 = 0.01; pQSM, F(4.79,76.61) = 1.91, p = 0.11, η2 = 0.02; aQSM, F(3.14,53.37) = 0.59, p = 0.63, η2 = 0.01), except for nQSM showing a trend for a significant interaction effect (nQSM, F(4.40,65.98) = 2.34, p = 0.058, η2 = 0.04).
Together, except for more diamagnetic tissue contrast in D1 compared with D4 and D5 in inner layers of BA 3b, our results show that the 3D structural architecture with respect to myelin, iron, and mineralization does not significantly differ between the representations of single fingers in BA 3b.
High similarity of finger-specific 3D profiles
To investigate whether 3D structural profiles of different fingers are similar, we sampled qT1, nQSM, pQSM, and aQSM values along 21 cortical depths specifically for each finger (Fig. 10). We compared intracortical meta parameters (i.e., skewness and kurtosis, in classical parcellation often used to differentiate between brain areas; Dinse et al., 2015) between finger representations. Using robust one-way repeated-measures ANOVAs on 20% trimmed means with factor finger (D1, D2, D3, D4, D5), there were no significant differences in skewness and kurtosis of finger-specific 3D profiles (Fig. 10B, Table 4, exact statistical results).
More specifically, we calculated Fréchet distances for each participant between all possible pairs of finger-specific qT1 profiles as a measure of profile similarity (Fig. 11). Similarity matrices were calculated both individually (Fig. 11B) and across participants (se Fig. 11C). Overall, the results indicate high similarity between finger-specific 3D qT1 profiles. Note that values closer to zero reflect higher similarity. Using a robust one-way repeated-measures ANOVA on 20% trimmed means with factor finger pairs (levels D1–D2, D2–D3, D3–D4, D4–D5), there were no significant differences in qT1 profile similarity between neighboring finger pairs (F(3,33) = 0.32, p = 0.81).
We also calculated Fréchet distances for each participant between all possible pairs of finger-specific nQSM, pQSM, and aQSM profiles (Fig. 12). Overall, finger-specific QSM profiles appear visually less similar than finger-specific qT1 profiles. However, using robust one-way repeated-measures ANOVAs on 20% trimmed means with factor finger pairs (levels D1–D2, D2–D3, D3–D4, D4–D5), we found no significant differences in similarity between neighboring finger pairs, neither for nQSM profiles (F(3,27) = 0.50, p = 0.69), nor for pQSM profiles (F(1.77,17.67) = 1.77, p = 0.20), nor for aQSM profiles (F(2.85,31.34) = 2.02, p = 0.13).
Together, our results show that finger-specific 3D profiles of myelin, diamagnetic contrast, iron, and mineralization do not significantly differ in their meta parameters between fingers.
Topographic 3D structural architecture of BA 3b hand and face areas
To demonstrate that the methods used here are sensitive to capture topographic differences when they in fact exist, we conducted a control analysis comparing layer-specific qT1, pQSM, nQSM, and aQSM between the functionally localized face and hand areas (using action maps). A two-way repeated measures ANOVA on qT1 values with factors layer (inner, middle, outer) and body part (face, hand) revealed a significant interaction effect (F(1.37,20.55) = 15.72, p = 2.7 × 10−4, η2 = 0.02). qT1-based myelin content was lower in the hand compared with the face area in inner (face, 1601.19 ± 12.36; hand, 1638.62 ± 11.84; t(15) = 2.91, p = 0.011, r = 0.6) and outer layers (face, 2067.11 ± 21.31; hand, 2097.74 ± 20.39; t(15) = 2.22, p = 0.042, r = 0.5; Fig. 13). The same ANOVA computed on pQSM values revealed a significant main effect of body part (F(1,13) = 10.92, p = 0.006, η2 = 0.12). pQSM-based iron content was highest in the hand area (hand, 0.011 ± 4.3 × 10−4; face, 0.009 ± 3.6 × 10−4). For aQSM values there was also a significant main effect of body part (F(1,13) = 10.92, p = 0.006, η2 = 0.13), showing highest mineralization content in the hand area (hand, 0.010 ± 3.5 × 10−4; face, 0.009 ± 3.3 × 10−4), whereas for nQSM values there was no significant difference between body parts.
Together, our results show that the 3D structural architecture, with respect to myelin, iron, and mineralization, significantly differs between major body part representations (here the representations of the face and the hand in BA 3b); whereas, as explained above, such differences were not detected between individual fingers.
Discussion
We used submillimeter quantitative MRI proxies for myelin, iron, and mineralization to answer the question of whether the 3D architecture of the human BA 3b hand area is homogenous, or whether there are systematic microstructural differences between finger representations. We show that precise intracortical tissue contrasts can be extracted from a group of younger adults and can be used to define anatomically relevant layer compartments. Although low-myelin borders were visible between the representations of the hand and the face, such borders were not systematically detected between finger representations. Similarly, although the 3D structural architecture significantly differed between the representations of the hand and the face, it did not differ significantly between finger representations (except for deep layers of the thumb) or along the inferior-to-superior axis. In fact, we found 3D structural profiles to be highly similar between individual finger representations. Together, our data show that the 3D structural architecture of the human BA 3b hand area is homogenous in younger adults and essentially nontopographic, although functional finger representations are distinct and discontinuous. This difference compared with the structural architecture of the hand area in some monkey species, and to the hand-face area in humans, reveals novel insights into the neuronal mechanisms that underlie the architecture and flexibility of finger representations and cortical plasticity in humans.
As expected from histology (Vogt and Vogt, 1919) and in vivo ex vivo validation work (Stüber et al., 2014; Dinse et al., 2015), we show an intracortical myelin gradient with increasing myelin content (decreasing qT1 values) from superficial to deep cortical depth. By relating our data to remodeled in vivo ex vivo validated data (Dinse et al., 2015), we extracted three anatomically relevant cortical compartments that contained estimates for BA 3b layers IV and V/VI. This approach is novel, because in previous work, data of only few participants were investigated extensively (Alkemade et al., 2022; Huber et al., 2020), or layers were defined arbitrarily by dividing the cortex into equally spaced compartments (Kuehn et al., 2017; Tardif et al., 2015). Although our layer definition was based on cortical myelination (i.e., qT1 values), the pQSM and nQSM data also fit to this compartmentalization scheme as the two iron peaks coincide with the expected locations of the Baillarger bands in anatomical layers IV and V, and the u-shaped nQSM profile shows a small plateau where we expected layer IV to be located. This scheme was therefore used to describe the 3D architecture of BA 3b in more detail.
With respect to low-myelin borders, we do not find systematic evidence for such borders between finger representations in BA 3b, which seems to differentiate humans from some monkey species (Jain et al., 1998; Qi and Kaas, 2004). Whereas topographic borders have been related to reducing plasticity (Sereno, 2005), an absence of structural borders may introduce flexibility to finger (re)alignment. In older humans' BA 3b, more overlap between neighboring finger representations and reduced cortical distances between index and middle finger representations have been described (Liu et al., 2021). In older rats, however, upper limb and whisker representations that are separated by septa remain intact (Spengler et al., 1995; Coq and Xerri, 2000; Godde et al., 2002; David-Jürgens et al., 2008). In reelin-deficient mice, where the primary visual cortex is intracortically disorganized, visual cortical plasticity is enhanced (Pielecka-Fortuna et al., 2015). Thus, an absence of sharp structural borders (both vertically and horizontally) may allow more distributed information (Muret et al., 2022), facilitating cortical flexibility.
An absence of low-myelin borders between BA 3b finger representations has also conceptual implications. Although single fingers are functionally distinct units, our findings suggest that all fingers are represented as one structural unit. Low-myelin borders in the cortex may therefore separate cortical representations that are nearby in the cortex but distant in the real world, as previously shown for the hand and face (Glasser et al., 2016; Kuehn et al., 2017; Northall et al., 2023). When cortical representations are nearby in the cortex and also nearby in the real world, such as for individual fingers, GABA-ergic functional inhibition may allow their differentiation when they receive distinct input (Kuehn et al., 2014). Low-myelin borders may therefore reflect real-world features. This also explains why structural and functional parcellation does not always align (Zhi et al., 2022) and relates to the concept that local stability and global reorganization of finger representations are driven by distributed, rather than finger-specific, processing underlying the topographic map (Wesselink et al., 2022). An absence of low-myelin borders between BA 3b finger representations is therefore in line with recent observations and suggests that the hand area is encoded as an integrated unit, shaped in close relation to real-world features such as external spatial location (Haggard et al., 2006).
In human M1, large-scale body part representations are not only separated by low-myelin borders but also differ in their microstructural profiles (Northall et al., 2023). In BA 3b, both were present between the hand and the face but absent between individual fingers (except for the thumb). This indicates that low-myelin borders and 3D structural differences coincide (or are both absent), adding evidence that large-scale body part representations can be regarded as distinct cortical fields (Sereno et al., 2022).
The exception to the rule in our data is the thumb, which shows higher diamagnetic contrast in deeper layers compared with the ring and the little finger. Most neurons in the BA 3b thumb representation specifically respond to tactile stimulation of the thumb but not to tactile stimulation of other fingers and interconnect only sparsely to other digit representations. Also in macaque monkeys, the thumb representation may act as an independent module processing selective information, whereas information content is more distributed between other fingers (Lazar et al., 2022). This may be linked to its special role for tool use.
Previous research in humans demonstrated that brain areas of similar cortical myeloarchitecture and cytoarchitecture are more likely to be connected and tend to exhibit stronger corticocortical connections (Huntenburg et al., 2017; Wei et al., 2019). The high similarity of finger-specific 3D structural profiles emphasizes cortical spread between finger representations, which may facilitate changes in finger topography, for example, after synchronous stimulation or gluing of multiple fingers (Kalisch et al., 2008; Kolasinski et al., 2016), and may offer a mechanistic explanation of why finger representations are unstable in older age (Liu et al., 2021; Kuehn et al., 2018b). Our findings encourage the investigation of microstructure profiles to fully understand topographic plasticity in aging and neurodegenerative diseases (Schreiber et al., 2021).
Addressing study limitations, absence of evidence should not be equated with evidence of absence. It still remains possible that potential low-myelin borders between finger representations could not be detected with the given methodology. To overcome possible confounds, additional descriptive and multidimensional analyses were implemented. Future studies may use pattern recognition algorithms to achieve fully automated septa-like feature detection. A direct methodological validation using the hand-face septum was not possible because of a lack of forehead activation during our motor task (Root et al., 2022). Future studies may consider isolated movements of the eyebrow to detect the hand-face septum in BA 3b. Although the sample size of this study is high compared with previous 7T MRI studies in humans and histological studies in monkey species on the topic (Kuehn et al., 2017; Huber et al., 2020; Qi and Kaas, 2004; Jain et al., 1998), it is relatively low for group statistics. Consequently, type I and type II errors are more difficult to control (Sullivan et al., 2016). To reduce type I errors, we corrected for multiple comparisons and restricted the number of statistical tests to the minimum number needed. To reduce type II errors, we show statistical trends (i.e., p < 0.1; Figs. 4, 9, 13; Liu et al., 2021), and one-sided tests were applied to directional hypotheses (i.e., low-myelin borders). To increase sensitivity for the null hypothesis (no effect), we conducted Bayesian statistics. However, it still remains possible that vertices of BA 1 and/or BA 3a were included in the statistics, although we carefully checked segmentation results.
Together, our data provide the first comprehensive in vivo description of the 3D structural architecture of the human BA 3b hand area and show it to be nontopographic. This distinguishes the human hand area from the barrel field of rodents and the hand area of monkeys, which show low-myelin borders separating individual whiskers and fingers, respectively. The specific homogeneous structural organization of the human hand area in BA 3b suggests that there are less structural limitations to cortical plasticity and reorganization. Studying the individual microstructure of the cortex together with functional changes is critical to fully understand the mechanisms of topographic change in the course of aging, learning, or disease, and may also explain individual differences in these mechanisms. These new insights encourage future studies to incorporate the dimension of cortical layers into new testable models and consider structural limitations of plasticity for intervention methods.
Footnotes
This work was supported by German Research Foundation Grants KU 3711/2-1 and 425899996–SFB 1436. A.N. was supported by Else Kröner Fresenius Stiftung Grant 2019-A03. We thank Lilith-Sophie Lange and Miriam Weber for support with data collection, Pierre-Louis Bazin and Daniel Haenelt for advice on ultra-high-resolution image processing, Susanne Stoll for discussions about multilevel modeling, and Nikolaus Kinder for graphical support.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Juliane Doehler at juliane.doehler{at}med.ovgu.de.
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.