Abstract
Many movements in daily life are embedded in motion sequences that involve more than one limb, demanding the motor system to monitor and control different body parts in quick succession. During such movements, systematic changes in the environment or the body might require motor adaptation of specific segments. However, previous motor adaptation research has focused primarily on motion sequences produced by a single limb, or on simultaneous movements of several limbs. For example, adaptation to opposing force fields is possible in unimanual reaching tasks when the direction of a prior or subsequent movement is predictive of force field direction. It is unclear, however, whether multilimb sequences can support motor adaptation processes in a similar way. In the present study (38 females, 38 males), we investigated whether reaches can be adapted to different force fields in a bimanual motor sequence when the information about the perturbation is associated with the prior movement direction of the other arm. In addition, we examined whether prior perceptual (visual or proprioceptive) feedback of the opposite arm contributes to force field-specific motor adaptation. Our key finding is that only active participation in the bimanual sequential task supports pronounced adaptation. This result suggests that active segments in bimanual motion sequences are linked across limbs. If there is a consistent association between movement kinematics of the linked and goal movement, the learning process of the goal movement can be facilitated. More generally, if motion sequences are repeated often, prior segments can evoke specific adjustments of subsequent movements.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Movements in a limb's motion sequence can be adjusted based on linked movements. A prerequisite is that kinematics of the linked movements correctly predict which adjustments are needed. We show that use of kinematic information to improve performance is even possible when a prior linked movement is performed with a different limb. For example, a skilled juggler might have learned how to correctly adjust his catching movement of the left hand when the right hand performed a throwing action in a specific way. Linkage is possibly a key mechanism of the human motor system for learning complex bimanual skills. Our study emphasizes that learning of specific movements should not be studied in isolation but within their motor sequence context.
Introduction
Many movements in daily life are embedded in motion sequences that involve more than one limb. Interaction between two arms in frequently repeated sequences is usually fast and effortless. For example, a juggler is able to transfer juggling balls rapidly and precisely between two hands. Because of variability in single motor segments, the juggler has to be able to adjust movements accordingly. In the current study, we investigate how the motor system is able to learn such intricate mechanisms to adjust movements in bimanual sequences. We show that, once a motor sequence is learned, kinematic information from the beginning of the sequence can be implicitly used to modify later segments.
If two or more movements in a sequence are repeated in the same order many times, the individual motor elements seem to be linked together in a single motor action (Diedrichsen and Kornysheva, 2015; Verwey et al., 2015). Thus, a motor element which is strongly linked in a sequence can influence prior and following motor segments (Hansen et al., 2018). If a reach is linked to a prior movement of the same arm, kinematic characteristics of that prior movement can even facilitate motor adaptation (Howard et al., 2012). In other words, information from a preceding same-limb movement can be used to adjust the following movement accordingly.
Motor adaptation studies involving a single force field have shown that, even without the use of cognitive strategies, an internal model of the motor dynamics to counteract external forces is acquired over time (Anwar et al., 2011). When multiple force fields are experienced, interference problems arise. Simple visual cues that indicate perturbation direction are ineffective in eliciting motor adaptation to opposing force fields in more complex motor adaptation designs requiring implicit adaptation (e.g., Cothros et al., 2009; for discussion of cue effectiveness differences because of study design, see Addou et al., 2011). In contrast, cues that allow overcoming the interference of multiple force fields are related to the motor plan (Wainscott et al., 2005; Hirashima and Nozaki, 2012; Sarwary et al., 2015; Sheahan et al., 2016; Howard et al., 2017) or the sensory state of the arm (Howard et al., 2013; Sarwary et al., 2013; Green and Labelle, 2015; Crevecoeur et al., 2022). Such cues are thought to enable cue-specific motor memory formation and retrieval by putting the sensorimotor system in the right preparatory state (Howard et al., 2020). Linked movements in particular seem to be effective cues because the representation of the entire motor sequence is specific to each perturbation direction and thus allows the creation of separate sensorimotor memories for each force field. In addition, sensory same-arm movement cues (passive or visual prior movements) are as effective for field-specific adaptation as actively performed linked movements (Howard et al., 2012). This suggests that perceptual information that implies the sensory consequences of same-arm movement execution can be linked to the active target reach.
Bimanual motor adaptation research, however, has been focused primarily on simultaneously executed movements rather than sequential movements (e.g., Nozaki et al., 2006; Tcheang et al., 2007; Howard et al., 2008; Kadota et al., 2014). Therefore, despite its relevance for human motor behavior, it is yet unknown whether a prior opposite-arm movement can serve as an effective cue for force field specific adaptation. Successful adaptation would indicate that distinct sequential segments of bimanual sequences can be linked together. During juggling, this would mean that, for instance, specific kinematics of the throwing action of one arm could be linked to specific adjustments during the catching motion of the other arm.
In the present study we, thus, investigated linkage of movements of two arms. In addition to the replication of previous unimanual findings, we examined whether, and to what extent, a prior movement of the opposite arm could facilitate implicit adaptation of the following movement in a force field interference task. To answer what aspects are key for establishing a link between such movements, we tested whether prior sensory information from the other arm (vision or proprioception) could be used as an effective cue for force field specific adaptation.
Materials and Methods
Our study design, including sample sizes, hypotheses, and main analysis plan, was preregistered on OSF (https://osf.io/qy9rn). In addition, data and analysis scripts we used to arrive at the results presented here are made available. In total, 80 right-handed volunteers aged 18-35 years (40 female, 40 male) participated in our study. We excluded 4 participants from analysis (see below); thus, our sample comprised 76 (38 female, 38 male, mean ± SD age, 25.7 ± 4.4 years) participants. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and were free of any known neurologic, perceptual, and motor impairments and disorders. The experiment was approved by the local ethics committee of the University of Leipzig. Participants gave informed written consent before the experiment.
All participants were required to make reaching movements in a Kinarm Exoskeleton Lab (for a video of the task, see OSF repository, see Fig. 1A). The Kinarm Exoskeleton Lab is a robotic device that can measure movements of the arms in the horizontal plane at a sampling rate of 1000 Hz. The robot can apply forces to the arms and provide visual feedback in a two-dimensional augmented virtual environment.
Experimental design
Participants were randomly assigned to one of five groups (Fig. 1B). Each group performed reaches to targets in the Kinarm. In two of the five groups, participants were required to move only their right arm (unimanual groups), while the other three groups incorporated both hands in subsequent reaching movements (bimanual groups). In the unimanual groups, three targets were displayed during each trial: the cue, middle, and final target (Fig. 1C). The middle target was individually calibrated to be at the position of the hand when the elbow was flexed 90° and the shoulder angle was 60°. In the bimanual groups, there were four targets: the cue, middle-left, middle-right, and final target. The distance between the two middle targets was 18 cm, and the midway point between them was fixed at 90° elbow and 60° shoulder angle for each participant. Each trial consisted of one or two active reaches. The final reach in each trial was the same for all experimental groups: participants made a reaching movement from the middle or middle-right target to the final target with their right hand.
In all groups, there were four possible final target positions: 12 cm to the right, left, up, or down from the middle (-right) target. There were two possible cue positions for each final target position (Fig. 1D). The distance between cue and middle (-left) target was 10 cm. The cursor displaying current hand position was red and 0.5 cm in diameter. All targets were 1.25 cm in diameter. The final target was yellow. All other targets were initially gray and changed their color to white during the trial (see below).
All groups went through three experimental phases: baseline, adaptation, and washout (Fig. 1E). Bimanual groups went through two additional experimental phases thereafter, namely, readaptation and another washout phase. These latter two phases aimed at assessing potential transfer of learning from different bimanual sequence group conditions to a bimanual group condition which involved actively moving both arms.
During trials in the adaptation and readaptation phases, a velocity-dependent curl field was present between the middle (-right) and the final targets. This force field systematically perturbed the right hand's movements. The force field started with a ramp up time of 100 ms once the right hand was >2 cm away from the midpoint of the middle target and stopped once the final target had been reached. The cue's location in relation to the final target was uniquely associated with the direction of the force field. Half of the participants learned the association between a positive angle between cue and final target and a clockwise (CW) force field, the other half between a positive angle and a counterclockwise (CCW) force field to control for any kinematic or biomechanical advantages of a specific combination. The association between the sign of the angle and the direction of the force field was fixed for each participant and did not change during the experiment.
The forces experienced during adaptation trials were perpendicular to movement direction and proportional to reaching velocity as follows:
In 11% of trials, chosen randomly throughout the trial sequence, the Kinarm forced movements to be straight by means of a force channel, that is, a force field that resembles a straight channel with impenetrable walls on its sides. In these trials, no curl force field was present. We term these trials clamp trials, as they restrict participants' movements to a straight trajectory while they attempt to counteract the expected (but absent) force field perturbation. The Kinarm measures the compensatory forces applied by the participant against the channel walls during the clamp trial, allowing us to quantify any possible feedforward learning, which would be expressed in the compensation of the expected (but absent) force field. Because of technical constraints, clamp trials did not always generate a strong enough force channel. Slight deviations from the straight line were possible; and in some cases, the hand broke through the virtual wall and deviated from the straight trajectory. We excluded these trials; and so, analysis of the last four adaptation block was based on 502 of 608 trials.
At the beginning of the experiment, participants were briefly familiarized with the task. They then performed 6 baseline, 50 adaptation, and 4 washout blocks. The bimanual groups performed 6 additional blocks: 5 readaptation and 1 additional washout block (see below). Each block consisted of 16 normal and 2 clamp trials. In total, participants performed at least 1080 trials. There were short breaks approximately every 200 trials and a 5 min break at the halfway point. Number and size of targets, timing, angles and force field strength were derived from the literature (e.g., Howard et al., 2012). At the end of the experiment, participants filled out pencil-paper questionnaires that asked them about potential strategies used in the experiment. In addition, we asked whether they had recognized a specific pattern between force field direction and cue position. Participants who indeed noticed the correct association between cue position and force field direction were excluded from analysis.
Group 1: control, unimanual single reach
We excluded and replaced 2 participants in the control group because they had detected the relationship between force field direction and position of targets and reported to have used this knowledge explicitly to move faster through the force field. The sample used for analysis was 20 (10 female, 10 male, mean ± SD age, 26.55 ± 5.17 years). We recorded EEG in the unimanual groups for another study, which is why the sample size was larger for unimanual than bimanual groups.
At the beginning of each trial, a white fixation cross on black background was displayed and the right arm of participants was moved to the middle target by the robot (Fig. 1F). This passive positioning took 1000 ms. Then, all targets (cue, middle, and final) and the hand position cursor were displayed. After a random time drawn from a uniform distribution ranging from 1000 to 1400 ms, the cue changed color from gray to white. This event was meaningful in the other experimental groups; in contrast, control group participants were not instructed to do anything yet. After 600 ms, the middle target changed color from gray to white, which was the go-signal for participants to reach to the final target. Once it was reached, feedback about the movement speed was displayed right above the middle target. If the movement time from 2 cm away from the middle to the final target was in between 150 and 300 ms, feedback (in German) was “good”; if it was outside this range “too fast” or “too slow,” respectively. The feedback was displayed for 600 ms. Finally, a white fixation cross was shown for 400 ms before the next passive positioning started for the next trial. When half the trials of a block were completed, the intertrial interval was 4 s instead of 400 ms.
Trials were immediately aborted and repeated within the same block when the cursor was not in the middle target when the color changes of the targets took place. If participants left the middle target earlier than 100 ms before or later than 500 ms after the go-signal, the trial was marked unsuccessful and repeated at a random position within the current block. Timing, feedback, and repetition criteria, as introduced here, were the same for all groups.
For 2 participants in the control group, the time between cue color change and middle target color change was set to 400 ms instead of 600 ms. The mean performance of these two was similar to other participants in the group: all three dependent variables deviated <1 SD from the mean of the 600 ms group; and so, we included them in our analysis.
Group 2: active unimanual sequence
There were 20 participants in the unimanual sequence group (10 female, 10 male, mean ± SD age, 25.9 ± 4.45 years). Unlike in the control group, the hand of the participants was moved to the position of the cue and not the middle target during the passive positioning. The color change of the cue was the indicator for participants to move from the cue to the middle target. Participants were instructed to try to reach the middle target approximately when it changed color and subsequently reach from the middle to the final target. The aim was to perform two separate straight reaches but to pause in the middle target as short as possible. Feedback at the trial's end referred only to the movement time from middle to final target. Trials were aborted when the cursor was not in the cue when the first color change occurred and when the cursor left the middle target too early or too late (see control group).
Four participants had to perform faster cue-middle target reaches because the time between cue color change and target color change was only 400 ms. Their mean performance was similar to that of other participants in the active unimanual sequence group: all three dependent variables deviated <1 SD from the mean of the 600 ms group; and so, we included them in the data analysis.
While we refer to sequence of movements in this and other groups, no motor sequence had to be learned in a way that required participants to perform the sequence without any perceptual input like is common in the sequence learning literature. Nevertheless, we think that the term “sequence of movements” is adequate since participants did indeed perform movements in sequence.
Group 3: active bimanual sequence
The active bimanual sequence group comprised 12 participants (6 female, 6 male, mean ± SD age, 25.7 ± 4.5 years). Because of large effect sizes observed in prior research (Howard et al., 2012; Sheahan et al., 2016), we chose this sample size for all bimanual groups. Although there was one more visual target in the bimanual groups, timing of color changes was exactly the same as in the unimanual groups. The two middle targets changed color at the same time, 600 ms after the cue color change.
At the beginning of a trial, the Kinarm robot moved both the right and the left arm; the left cursor to the cue position and the right cursor to the middle-right target. The color change of the cue was the signal for the participants to move the left arm to the middle-left target. Like in the unimanual sequence group, they were instructed to reach the middle-left target approximately when the middle targets changed color. The goal was to finish the left hand movement and subsequently reach with the right hand to the final target. To keep the trial abortion and repetition criteria consistent with the unimanual groups, participants had to have both cursors in their respective starting positions once the color change of the cue indicated the start of the left arm reach. Trials were also repeated when the right hand left the middle-right target >100 ms too early or >500 ms too late. Like in the unimanual sequence group, force fields and force channels (for clamp trials) were only ever present for the second reach, but never for the left hand. Feedback displayed after the movements was only about the right hand movement speed from the middle-right to the final position.
There were 6 additional blocks in the bimanual groups. After the last washout block, participants performed 5 blocks in an active bimanual sequence readaptation condition and subsequently one final active washout block. These blocks were identical to those of the first adaptation and washout phases.
Group 4: passive bimanual sequence
Our sample comprised 12 participants (6 female, 6 male, mean ± SD age, 25.7 ± 4.2 years). We excluded and replaced 2 participants because they used explicit strategies, which they reported in our debriefing questionnaire.
Like in the active bimanual sequence group, the left arm was positioned at the cue and the right arm at the middle-right target at the beginning of a trial. However, participants did not see a cursor at the position of the left hand, and they did not actively move their left arm during the three main phases of the experiment. Instead, participants were instructed to keep their left arm relaxed while the Kinarm moved the left hand from cue to middle-left target following a minimum jerk trajectory after the cue color change. This passive reach of the left arm started 100 ms after the go-signal and took 550 ms to mirror an average active reach and the preceding reaction time. Participants were instructed to start their right hand reach to the final target once they felt that the passive movement of the left hand finished. They were told that the color change of the middle targets did not always occur at the same time in relation to the passive left arm movement to discourage participants to discount the passive arm movement and only pay attention to the color change as a start signal. After the experiment, we asked participants whether they noticed that the end of the passive left arm movement always coincided with the middle targets' color change and whether they had used this color change as a strategy to initiate their right arm reach. This was the case for 1 participant; we evaluate this point in the Discussion.
After the main three phases, participants performed five readaptation blocks and one washout block with the same instructions as the active bimanual sequence group. They experienced the same force field directions as in the adaptation phase; however, now they had to actively move the left hand. This post-test assessed whether there was any transfer of force field adaptation that may have taken place in the experiment's prior phases from passive to active left hand movement.
Group 5: visual bimanual sequence
There were 12 participants in the visual bimanual sequence group (6 female, 6 male, mean ± SD age, 23.8 ± 3.0 years). During the main three phases of the experiment, participants did not move their left arm. At the start of each trial, only the right hand was positioned to the middle-right target. Once the targets were displayed, there was, however, also a red cursor at the cue position. After the cue color change, this cursor moved to the middle-left target with the same motion dynamics as the passive movement in the passive bimanual sequence group. Participants were instructed to start their right hand reach once the red cursor reached the middle-left target. They were also asked not to use the middle target color changes as a go-signal but instead focus on the moving red cursor. Three participants paid attention to the color changes; we address this point in the Discussion.
Like the other bimanual groups, participants performed 6 blocks of active bimanual movements at the end of the experiment to assess learning transfer.
Data analysis
We preprocessed data in MATLAB (R2021a). The Kinarm measured angles of the elbow and shoulder joints. We low-pass filtered this data with a cutoff at 10 Hz and added hand velocity, acceleration, and commanded forces. Commanded forces are the forces that were generated by the Kinarm to make the hand of the participants stay in force channels during clamp trials.
Maximal perpendicular error (MPE)
We performed our main analysis in Python (3.7) using the libraries numpy (Harris et al., 2020), pandas (McKinney et al., 2010), scipy (Virtanen et al., 2020), scikit-learn (Pedregosa et al., 2011), as well as matplotlib (Hunter, 2007) and seaborn (Waskom, 2021) for plotting. We excluded all aborted and repeated trials. Our first outcome measure was the MPE, defined as the signed maximal deviation in centimeters from the straight line between middle (-right) and final target of the right arm trajectory and reflected adaptation performance. A positive value denoted that participants had exhibited a curved trajectory in the direction of the force field. The MPE was defined between 2 cm away from the midpoint of the middle target and the end of the final target. We excluded trials when it was obvious that participants had started the reach toward an incorrect target (21 across all samples).
Force field compensation (FFC)
Our second outcome measure was FFC in clamp trials. To calculate FFC, we extracted force data within a 150 ms time window centered on the time of peak velocity. Next, we calculated the ideal force profile, which would have counteracted the missing force field based on movement velocity. The measured force against the channel walls was linearly regressed on the ideal force profile with the intercept forced to zero. We defined FFC as the slope of the regression multiplied with 100%.
Statistical analysis
The focus of our study was on motor adaptation learning and differences in this aspect between groups. We quantified current motor adaptation performance with the MPE in normal trials and FFC in clamp trials. We averaged MPE and FFC over block and calculated the mean and 95% confidence intervals for each group (see Figs. 3A,B, 4A,B).
To measure the degree of adaptation each participant exhibited, we calculated three dependent variables. First, we subtracted the average MPE of the first adaptation block from the average of the last two adaptation blocks for each participant (MPE change adaptation). A negative MPE change adaptation value indicates that straighter reaches occurred at the end of the adaptation phase compared with the beginning. The more negative the value, the greater was the performance improvement. This could be achieved by force field-specific compensatory mechanisms but also by making the reaching limb more stiff. To rule out such a generic strategy, we assessed two further measures. Namely, second, we calculated the difference of the average MPE of the last baseline and first washout block (MPE change baseline/washout). A negative value means that a participant made systematically more curved reaches counteracting the experienced force fields in the washout block than in the baseline block. Consequently, the more negative the value, the bigger was the force field after-effect. This reaching behavior indicates that participants adapted to the force field and exhibited after-effects when the force field was removed (e.g., Gandolfo et al., 1996). Third, we calculated the average FFC for each participant in the last four blocks in the adaptation phase (FFC final adaptation). A FFC final adaptation value of 100% would indicate that participants perfectly adjusted their reaches to the force fields. In addition, we calculated FFC change from the last four baseline to the last four adaptation blocks. Results mirrored the results of FFC final adaptation comparisons and are thus not reported.
To answer our research questions, we performed planned comparisons between groups (Fig. 1B). To assess differences in motor adaptation because of a same or opposite arm prior movement, we compared all three dependent variables between the control, unimanual, and bimanual active sequence groups. For each group comparison, we calculated the difference of the mean of the dependent variable. Next, we permuted group labels and computed the resulting means' difference. We repeated this 5 million times or until the number of exact possible permutations was reached. The p value was defined as the proportion of sampled permutations where the absolute difference was greater than the absolute observed difference. Following the same rationale, we compared all dependent variables within-group against zero (indicating no performance change) to assess whether group performance improved or worsened over time. For each family of permutation tests, we adjusted the p value using the Bonferroni–Holm correction. We defined a family of tests as tests evaluating the same dependent measure (1 family = 11 tests).
To investigate whether sensory information in one modality of the opposite arm during the prior movement is sufficient for adaptation to occur, we compared performance measures between the three bimanual groups and within the groups. In addition, based on the observation that the learning curves of the active unimanual and bimanual sequence groups differed, we investigated the slope of adaptation in both groups at the beginning of the adaptation phase (first 10 blocks) to identify potential learning differences at an early stage of the experiment. We performed a linear mixed effects analysis in R using afex (Singmann et al., 2016). We entered block number and group membership as well as the interaction term as fixed effects. As random effects, we included by-participant random intercepts. We used the Kenward–Roger method to obtain p values (model equation: mixed(MPE ∼ Group * Block + (1|VP), data = data, method = “KR”).
We used an explorative approach to examine performance in the readaptation phase in the bimanual sequence groups. We were interested to see whether any transfer of learning occurred from a passive/visual to an active bimanual sequence. Because of the small number of blocks in the readaptation phase, we did not look at performance improvement within this phase but rather compared performance in this phase to performance at the end of adaptation. We averaged the MPE of the last five adaptation blocks of each participant and subtracted the average MPE of the readaptation phase (MPE re-/adaptation change). We again performed within and between permutation tests with this value.
To investigate to what extent the three measures (MPE change adaptation, MPE change baseline/washout, and FFC final adaptation) reflect the same underlying factor, we calculated Pearson's correlation between two measures each across participants.
Results
All participants performed reaches from a middle position to final targets. Opposing force fields perturbed reaches during the (re-)adaptation phase of the experiment. Figure 2 depicts reaching trajectories from middle (-right) to final target of relevant blocks of all participants. At the end of the adaptation phase, participants in the active sequence groups made straighter reaches between the middle and final targets than at the beginning of adaptation, when the force field had just been introduced. Moreover, when the force field was removed, strong after-effects were evident as curving of reach trajectories in the direction of the former force fields, because of participants being prepared to counter the force field they had previously encountered. Adaptation and after-effects indicate that participants adapted their reaches to the respective force fields.
Unimanual groups versus active bimanual sequence group
First, we asked whether a prior movement with the opposite arm can be used as an effective cue for learning force field specific adaptation. In addition, we investigated the degree of adaptation compared with same arm prior movements as a cue. To this end, we compared MPE changes and FFC final adaptation to zero (no performance change) within the control, unimanual, and bimanual sequence group as well as across groups.
MPE change during the adaptation phase was not significant in the control group (p = 0.5887; Fig. 3C). Control participants did not improve in counteracting the forces over the course of the adaptation phase. In contrast, performance of both the unimanual (p = 1.5e-05) and the active bimanual sequence group (p = 0.0029) improved over the adaptation phase. Improvement was greater in both sequence groups compared with the control group (
In addition, MPE baseline/washout changes were present in all groups (
To confirm our findings with a measure of feedforward adaptation, we compared FFC final adaptation. The same pattern as MPE changes in the adaptation phase was revealed: FFC final adaptation was different from zero in both sequence groups but not in the control group (
To investigate whether this difference in adaptation occurs during early learning in the adaptation phase, we performed a mixed model with the between groups factor group (unimanual vs bimanual sequence) and within group factor block (blocks 7-16). We found main effects for group (F(1,30) = 6.86, p = 0.14) and block (F(9,270) = 47.48, p < 0.001) as well as an interaction between group and block (F(9,270) = 5.49, p = 0.001; Fig. 3F). Participants in the unimanual sequence group had a steeper learning curve than participants in the bimanual sequence group during early learning. This result indicates that linking of movements over body parts might be slower than linking movements within one body part.
Active versus visual versus passive bimanual sequence group
To investigate whether the perception of specific opposite arm movements without active execution allows motor adaptation, we tested for effects of adaptation (MPE changes and FFC final adaptation) in each group separately as well as across groups. MPE changes in the adaptation phase were significant for the visual (p = 0.0234) and the passive (p = 0.0137) bimanual sequence group (Fig. 4C). Moreover, the active group improved their performance to a greater extent than the visual (p = 6.7e-06) and the passive (p = 3.6e-05) group. Finally, the change in performance was not different between the visual and passive group (p = 0.1422). These results suggest that, while prior perception of movement in one sensory modality does allow some force field specific adaptation, it is considerably less effective than an active prior movement.
Similarily, MPE changes from the baseline to the washout phase were evident in all groups (
The overall result pattern was equivalent for FFC final adaptation (Fig. 4E). All groups showed some compensation (
Transfer
Finally, we asked whether any potential hidden learning in the visual/passive conditions could be transferred to an actively performed bimanual motor sequence. To this end, we compared the MPE from the end of adaptation to the MPE during the readaptation phase. We observed performance changes in the passive (p = 0.0103) and active (p = 0.002) bimanual sequence groups, but not in the visual group (p = 0.0571; Fig. 4F). Participants in the active bimanual sequence group performed worse during the readaptation phase than at the end of the adaptation phase. This result is likely caused by the washout phase in between the adaptation and readaptation phase, in which participants readapted to an environment without force fields. Participants in the passive group, however, were able to reduce their MPE in the readaptation blocks compared with the last adaptation blocks. It is an open question whether this result is because of a motor learning mechanism or because of the need of the participants to stabilize their arm movements more when both arms have to be actively controlled. Comparisons between groups revealed a difference in MPE re-/adaptation change between the active and visual group (p = 7e-05) as well as between the active and passive group (p = 4.4e-06). The MPE was not different between the visual and passive groups (p = 0.1823). Overall, the transfer results further support the conclusion that initial learning was limited in the visual and passive group, making it difficult for transfer of learning to occur.
Correlation between measures
In order to investigate whether dynamics were similar across our different measures, we calculated a correlation between them. While some studies have shown that MPE change adaptation can be influenced by generic strategies, such as by making the reaching limb more stiff (e.g., Howard et al., 2012), all correlations between MPE change adaptation, MPE change baseline/washout, and FFC final adaptation across participants were high and in the expected direction (Table 1). This suggests that all variables measured the same motor mechanism. In our study, participants were more restricted in their movements than in similar studies because of the exoskeleton robot, which may have made it more difficult for them to use generic strategies, such as increased stiffness.
Strategies reported in questionnaire
Participants who (according to their reports) were able to correctly predict force field direction based on the cue's position or the prior movement were excluded from analysis (4 of 80). To get some qualitative insights what participants focused on during the experiment, they were asked to fill out a questionnaire at the end of the experiment. We asked them about strategies used to get the timing correct as well as strategies used to move quickly through the force field. Most participants reported to not having used any strategy at all. Other answers were more detailed. Many described a lot of switching and testing of strategies over the course of the experiment. Some participants focused on fast reaction times, speed, precision, or thinking of a specific rhythm. Others tried to use more or less force, to react faster to sensed perturbations, or to built up more or less tension in the arm before encountering the force field. Some participants also used a certain strategy only for a specific final target direction. Most participants noted that their tried strategies did not work sufficiently. A few participants sometimes intentionally deviated from trying to perform a completely straight line reach for some final target directions at some point during the experiment. However, they did not modify their bias to one side based on the position of the cue; therefore, their intentionally curved trajectories did not match the experienced force field direction in a systematic way. None of the strategies described by participants was problematic for the experiment nor objectively beneficial or detrimental.
Discussion
Fine-tuning movements to reduce discrepancies between motor command and sensory feedback are a key mechanism of motor learning. In this study, we investigated whether linking sequential movements of two arms can support motor adaptation to opposing force fields. We report two main findings. First, we found that prior movements of the opposite arm enabled adaptation to opposing force fields. This finding demonstrates that learning of a movement can be influenced by a linked movement of the other arm. Specifically, if there are consistent relations between kinematics of different arm movements in a motion sequence, the motor system can take advantage of this information to adjust movements accordingly. Second, visual and proprioceptive opposite-arm signals in the absence of an active movement were significantly less effective than active reaches, highlighting that actively using both arms is a key requirement in linking sequential, bimanual movements strongly. In addition, we replicated previous findings showing that active same-arm prior movements facilitate adaptation, whereas stationary visual cues, though indicative of force field direction, do not (e.g., Howard et al., 2012).
Unimanual versus bimanual sequence learning
Our findings indicate that formation of separate motor memories in a force field interference task is not only possible when distinct perturbations are encountered in unimanual movement sequences, but also in active, bimanual movement sequences. Notably, the bimanual movement chains were less effective than unimanual chains, evident in both, the smaller reduction of trajectory curvature as measured with the MPE in the adaptation phase and smaller forces applied against the expected perturbation as indicated by the FFC measure. Furthermore, comparison of the adaptation curves for unimanual and bimanual sequences during early learning indicated that adaptation in the bimanual context was not only weaker but also slower than in the unimanual context. Thus, chained movements of a single limb seem to be more readily linked and represented together than movements across limbs. Several factors may explain this difference.
First, if single movement elements of a sequence are difficult to perform, it is more likely that they are represented separately as discrete actions (Rand and Stelmach, 2000). This is also reflected by an increased response time when alternating between different hands in bimanual serial reaction time tasks (Trapp et al., 2012; Bhakuni and Mutha, 2015). Using different limbs within one sequence adds complexity, requiring more coordination and attention to execute the movement sequence according to the movement plan (Gálvez-García et al., 2014). The increased difficulty might inhibit the linking of bimanual sequences (Kennedy et al., 2021). As a result, movements might be preferentially represented discretely.
Second, linking the movements of two different body parts might be harder because of the organization and structure of the brain. Neural patterns pertaining to a bimanual movement sequence are spread out over both hemispheres because of the lateralization of the motor cortex (Gerloff and Andres, 2002). Thus, a wider and bihemispheric network of brain modules is involved in bimanual compared with unimanual movement sequences, which might, accordingly, be more difficult to establish and maintain (Noble et al., 2014).
Third, neural crosstalk might interfere with learning of bimanual tasks (Swinnen, 2002; Kennedy et al., 2021). Neural crosstalk occurs during bimanual movements when neural signals designated to muscles in one arm are also sent to homologous muscles in the other arm (Cardoso de Oliveira, 2002). Interference emerges when additional, conflicting signals are received in close temporal proximity. In our active bimanual sequence group, left and right hand reaches had to be made directly following one another; thus, the related neural signals may have resulted in interference, hampering adaptation.
The neural and computational mechanisms that mediate the linking of movements are not fully understood. Often, repeated movement sequences are assumed to be represented together in one common motor chunk that is executed holistically as a cohesive unit (Diedrichsen and Kornysheva, 2015). However, recent findings in macaque dorsal premotor and primary motor cortex during reaching sequences have suggested that movements are linked successfully when the second reach is prepared without disrupting the concurrent execution of the first (Zimnik and Churchland, 2021). Thus, at least at a motor cortical level, sequence segments can be prepared and executed independently. According to those results, it is critical that the preparatory state for a subsequent movement in a sequence is established during the prior movement. In our paradigm, the two used force fields were associated with distinct prior movements to a given target. Accordingly, a unique, linked prior movement did not only need to facilitate the selection and preparation of the correct subsequent reaching direction, but also its correct execution, that is, counteracting the force fields in our study. This adaptation to two different environment dynamics is assumed to require the switching of internal models which each predict the expected sensory feedback following the executed motor commands for their specific context (Imamizu et al., 2007). In our task, the prior movement seems to indeed activate the correct internal model for the subsequent reach after repetitive force-field exposure. On a physiological level, the primary motor cortex is also thought to be involved during forming and retrieving of internal models (Kadota et al., 2014). More specifically, preparatory neural states and population dynamics in motor cortex are altered to modify motor commands to adapt to changed environment dynamics (Sun et al., 2022). A more abstract representation of a learned movement sequence seems to be stored in circuits involving basal ganglia (Mizes et al., 2022), SMA and pre-SMA (Diedrichsen and Kornysheva, 2015). In addition, the cerebellum plays an important role in associative sensorimotor learning (Calame et al., 2021; Avraham et al., 2022) as well as in timing and coordination of different limbs (Diedrichsen et al., 2007). It is not clear, however, whether the differences in the ability to link unimanual and bimanual sequences lie primarily in cortical or such subcortical regions. The answer to this question requires the use of neuroimaging techniques.
Sensory information as a substitute for active movement in sequence learning
In our study, providing prior visual or proprioceptive feedback of the opposite-arm movement did not enhance adaptation of the moving arm to the same extent as an active prior opposite-arm movement. This suggests that sensory information on its own does not provide a substitute for active movement in bimanual sequence learning. This finding contrasts with prior research in which visual and passive prior same-arm movements were effective cues for adaptation to opposing force fields (Howard et al., 2012). The contrast could be explained by sensory information of the same limb being weighted differently than information of another limb during sensorimotor integration processes. Sensorimotor integration is the ability to extract relevant sensory inputs to create informed motor outputs (Wolpert et al., 1995). Sensory information received from a same-arm visual or passive “movement” directly affects the state estimation of the arm and thus the internal model and motor command of the next reach, allowing force field specific adaption. Sensory changes in another limb, however, do not bear the same relevance for the execution of a reach and may not be integrated computationally within a bimanual motor sequence. In addition, in the visual bimanual sequence group, participants may not have represented the red cursor as their left arm. In line with this explanation, visual cues which cannot be directly related to the state of the moving arm have not led to adaptation in previous research, for instance, spatially static visual cues or field-specific cursor/background colors (Cothros et al., 2009; Howard et al., 2012, 2013).
Although active engagement seems to be necessary for strong linking to occur across arms, some participants in the visual and passive bimanual sequence group were able to reduce their movement error over the course of the adaptation phase, indicating that interindividual differences exist in whether and how cues can be used for motor adaptation. One factor that could influence individual adaptation is the attention given to the perceptual information of the opposite arm. However, in our debriefing, 6 of 24 participants indicated that they had primarily attended to the color switch of the middle targets, rather than anticipating the end of the visual or proprioceptive left hand movement. Despite this strategy, these participants' performance was not appreciably different from that of other participants. In addition, manipulating attention or restricting eye movements has not been shown to change adaptation performance in similar experimental conditions (Howard et al., 2012). Thus, which factors may drive the adaptation differences between participants remains unclear. We also observed a difference between visual and passive prior movement in the washout effect where the adaptation was greater on average in the passive compared with the visual group. This might indicate that some modality-specific differences in the use of sensory information exist. Insufficient power to detect smaller effect sizes might have prevented us from finding this effect in the other two measures.
In addition, we asked whether participants could transfer any learning obtained in a setting where the prior movement was only visual or passive to an active bimanual movement. This question was motivated by sports and rehabilitation practices, where a similar transfer would be highly desirable to support motor learning processes. In our experiment, however, learning was, for the most part, absent when only visual or passive-proprioceptive information of the prior opposite-arm movement were available. A transfer of overt improved performance to an active bimanual sequence was therefore impossible.
Surprisingly, there was immediate improvement in performance in the passive bimanual sequence group once they actively performed the task. The reduction of the MPE in the readaptation phase could, however, originate not from force field specific adaptation but from increased muscle cocontraction in the right arm. The increased stiffness would result in smaller MPEs without true learning of the force fields. Cocontraction of the right arm might be more pronounced in the readaptation than the adaptation phase because participants had to control their left arm movement in addition to their right arm during the active readaptation. Together, we cannot draw strong conclusions about transfer ability from linked sensory/active movements to active motor sequences.
Motor adaptation versus sequence learning
Motor theories usually distinguish between motor adaptation and skill learning. Motor adaptation entails a trial-by-trial change evoked by a mismatch between expected and received feedback and is thus a recalibration process (Wolpert et al., 2011). In contrast, skill learning entails the creation of a new movement pattern (Diedrichsen and Kornysheva, 2015). It is currently unknown whether findings about motor adaptation generalize to motor skill learning. In our view, it is crucial for complex bilateral motor behavior and learning to adjust part of a movement sequence according to kinematic parameters of the same sequence; this appears to us to be equivalent in multilimb adaptation and skill learning. Our present results suggest, for example, that a novice juggler will improve over time by using kinematic information from one arm to adjust movements of the other more and more. Bimanual tasks with concurrent movements of the arms have already shown that internal models of arm movements encompass not only kinematic information about the relevant arm but also about the opposite arm to allow smooth compensation and flexible interaction between them (Yokoi et al., 2011). In our study, we provide first evidence that the selection process of internal models is influenced by prior movements of another limb and thereby contribute to a mechanistic understanding of complex bilateral motor behavior. Our finding is relevant for motor learning in both rehabilitation and sport settings. For example, in rehabilitation of stroke, activating a specific motor memory of the affected hand (e.g., reaching for a cup) could be cued and facilitated by a prior opposite-arm movement. Similarly, in sports, deliberately using movement sequences to differentiate between otherwise interfering moves (e.g., twisting once or twice in gymnastics) could be especially beneficial. These exciting prospects await future research.
Footnotes
We thank Pei-Cheng Shih for valuable discussion, advice, and help with programming and setting up the experiment; Elisabeth Kaminski for valuable discussion; Philipp Kaniuth for useful comments on an earlier version of the manuscript; and Lisa Franke for help with data collection.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Magdalena Gippert at gippert{at}cbs.mpg.de