Abstract
The sensitivity of retinal cells is altered in background light to optimize the detection of contrast. For scotopic (rod) vision, substantial adaptation occurs in the first two cells, the rods and rod bipolar cells (RBCs), through sensitivity adjustments in rods and postsynaptic modulation of the transduction cascade in RBCs. To study the mechanisms mediating these components of adaptation, we made whole-cell, voltage-clamp recordings from retinal slices of mice from both sexes. Adaptation was assessed by fitting the Hill equation to response-intensity relationships with the parameters of half-maximal response (I1/2), Hill coefficient (n), and maximum response amplitude (Rmax). We show that rod sensitivity decreases in backgrounds according to the Weber–Fechner relation with an I1/2 of ∼50 R* s−1. The sensitivity of RBCs follows a near-identical function, indicating that changes in RBC sensitivity in backgrounds bright enough to adapt the rods are mostly derived from the rods themselves. Backgrounds too dim to adapt the rods can however alter n, relieving a synaptic nonlinearity likely through entry of Ca2+ into the RBCs. There is also a surprising decrease of Rmax, indicating that a step in RBC synaptic transduction is desensitized or that the transduction channels became reluctant to open. This effect is greatly reduced after dialysis of BAPTA at a membrane potential of +50 mV to impede Ca2+ entry. Thus the effects of background illumination in RBCs are in part the result of processes intrinsic to the photoreceptors and in part derive from additional Ca2+-dependent processes at the first synapse of vision.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Light adaptation adjusts the sensitivity of vision as ambient illumination changes. Adaptation for scotopic (rod) vision is known to occur partly in the rods and partly in the rest of the retina from presynaptic and postsynaptic mechanisms. We recorded light responses of rods and rod bipolar cells to identify different components of adaptation and study their mechanisms. We show that bipolar-cell sensitivity largely follows adaptation of the rods but that light too dim to adapt the rods produces a linearization of the bipolar-cell response and a surprising decrease in maximum response amplitude, both mediated by a change in intracellular Ca2+. These findings provide a new understanding of how the retina responds to changing illumination.
Introduction
The visual system can encode light over an immense range of illumination. We see stimuli producing single-photon responses in a minority of the rod photoreceptors (Hecht et al., 1941; van der Velden, 1946; Baylor et al., 1979), but as light intensity increases, adaptation within the rods and in circuits that carry rod signals extends the dynamic range of rod vision to at least seven orders of magnitude (Dunn et al., 2006; Tikidji-Hamburyan et al., 2015; Frederiksen et al., 2021).
Rod bipolar cells (RBCs) are depolarizing (ON center) bipolar cells, which are the first point of pooling of rod signals in the mammalian retina. They allow convergence of up to 100 rods (Tsukamoto et al., 2001). The responses of RBCs are produced by a signal transduction mechanism that influences the gating of the transient receptor potential melastatin channel 1 (TRPM1), through action of a metabotropic glutamate receptor, mGluR6 (Morgans et al., 2009). The light-dependent opening of TRPM1 channels requires the deactivation of Gαo (Nawy, 1999; Dhingra et al., 2000; Okawa et al., 2010b), a process that is accelerated by the regulator of G-protein-signaling proteins RGS7 and RGS11 (Cao et al., 2012). All these steps in the mGluR6 cascade are potential points of modulation during light adaptation. The light-dependent TRPM1 inward currents have been shown to be affected by changes in internal calcium (Ca2+) concentration (Shiells and Falk, 1999; Berntson et al., 2004; Nawy, 2004; Kaur and Nawy, 2012), although the mechanism of Ca2+ action and its role in setting the sensitivity of RBC responses with background light remain unclear.
To elucidate the processes responsible for adaptation of scotopic vision, we used whole-cell patch recordings in dark-adapted slices from rod photoreceptors and RBCs in darkness and in background light. Background intensities bright enough to adapt the rod photoreceptors caused changes in the sensitivity of RBCs that are mostly derived directly from rod adaptation. Light intensities too weak to produce significant adaptation in the rods can also altered the RBC response by mechanisms intrinsic to the bipolar cell. Dim backgrounds changed the sensitivity of the response to small differences in light intensity by linearizing the slope of the response-intensity curve, an effect likely mediated by entry of Ca2+. Low background intensities also produce an additional novel form of adaptation that reduced the RBC maximum response amplitude (Rmax), apparently by desensitizing some step in the RBC synaptic transduction cascade or by changing the conformation of the TRPM1 channels so that they become reluctant to open. This effect was also facilitated by calcium entry. These experiments provide new insight into the effects of exposure to background illumination on responses of retinal cells and the mechanisms of adaptation of scotopic vision.
Materials and Methods
Animals and animal care
This study was conducted in accordance with the recommendations of the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals of the National Institutes of Health and the Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology Statement for the Use of Animals in Ophthalmic and Vision Research. The animal use protocol was approved by the University of California, Los Angeles, Animal Research Committee (Protocol ARC-2014–005). The primary method of killing was cervical dislocation. C57BL/6J mice (Mus musculus) were purchased from The Jackson Laboratory and were not screened for the absence of the rd8 mutation (Chang et al., 2002). All mice used in this study were between 2 and 6 months of age from approximately equal numbers of both sexes and were reared under a 12 h dark/light cycle.
Solutions
Retinal slices were made in HEPES-buffered Ames' medium (Sigma-Aldrich) containing 2.38 g L−1 HEPES balanced with 0.875 g L−1 NaOH to give an osmolarity of 284 ± 1 mOsm at pH 7.35 ± 0.05. This Ames-HEPES was kept on ice and bubbled continuously with 100% O2. Bicarbonate-buffered Ames' medium (hereafter, referred to as buffered Ames' medium) was made from Ames' medium supplemented with 1.9 g L−1 NaHCO3 and equilibrated with 95% O2/5% CO2, pH 7.4. The electrode internal solution contained the following (in mm): 125 K-aspartate, 10 KCl, 10 HEPES, 5 N-methyl-glucamine-HEDTA, 0.5 CaCl2, 0.5 MgCl2, 1 ATP-Mg, 0.2 GTP-Tris, 2.5 NADPH; pH was adjusted to ∼7.3 with N-methyl-glucamine-OH, and the osmolarity was adjusted to ∼280 mOsm. For experiments investigating effects of calcium buffering on RBC adaptation, the internal solution additionally contained 10 mm BAPTA (catalog #A4926, Sigma-Aldrich). All other constituents were identical to those of the normal internal solution.
Dissection and slice preparation
Mice were dark adapted for 12–20 h before the start of the experiment. All experiments began in the morning as defined by the vivarium dark/light cycle. Dissections were performed under infrared illumination (λ ≥ 900 nm) with infrared image converters, either head mounted (ITT Industries) or dissecting-microscope mounted (B.E. Meyers). Following euthanasia, eyes from mice were enucleated, the anterior portion of the eye was cut, and the lens and cornea were removed. Eyecups were stored at 32°C in buffered Ames' medium in a light-tight container. Eyecups were bisected through the optic nerve head with a number 10 scalpel under the infrared-equipped dissection microscope (Carl Zeiss), and the retina was carefully removed from the retinal pigment epithelium with fine forceps. The isolated piece of retina was embedded in a low-temperature gelling agarose (3%; Sigma-Aldrich) in HEPES-buffered Ames' medium. Vertical retinal slices (200 μm in thickness) were cut in chilled, oxygenated Ames-HEPES with a vibrating microtome (VT-1000 S, Leica) and transferred either to a recording chamber or to the storage container for use later in the experiment. During recordings, the retinal slice was stabilized with a custom-made anchor of stainless steel (420 grade, polished), which was fastened to the recording chamber with a small amount of petroleum jelly. The slice was superfused with buffered Ames' medium at ∼4 ml min−1. The bath temperature was held at 36 ± 1°C by a temperature controller with feedback (catalog #TC-324B; Warner Instruments).
Physiologic recordings from rod photoreceptors and rod bipolar cells
Recordings from individual cells were made by whole-cell patch clamp from dark-adapted retinal slices as described previously (Arman and Sampath, 2010). Rods were visualized with illumination from an infrared light-emitting diode (LED; λ = 940 nm; Cairn Research) attached to the transmitted light path of the physiology microscope (Eclipse FN1, Nikon). Rod somata were identified by morphology and location in the outer nuclear layer (ONL), and RBC somata by morphology and location in the outermost portion of the inner nuclear layer as well as by their characteristic response to a flash. Some RBCs were filled with a fluorescent dye (100 μm; Alexa Fluor 750, λmax = ∼750 nm; Thermo Fisher Scientific) loaded in the recording pipette. Dye-filled cells were imaged following the recording with a Hamamatsu ORCAflash4.0LT+ (model C11440, Hamamatsu Photonics).
Filamented borosilicate-glass capillaries (BF120-69–10; Sutter Instruments) were pulled on the day of the experiment with a P-97 Flaming/Brown micropipette puller (Sutter Instruments) to a tip resistance in the bath medium of 15–19 MΩ for rods and 13–16 MΩ for RBCs. Cells were voltage clamped at holding potentials of −40 mV for rods and −60 mV or +50 mV for RBCs with an AxoPatch 200B patch-clamp amplifier (Molecular Devices). Series resistance of the recording pipette was compensated at 75–80% to prevent error in clamping potentials, and pipette capacitance was neutralized before break-in (Sherman et al., 1999). The patch seal was assessed after break-in, and recordings were terminated if the seal resistance was below ∼1 GΩ, or the access resistance exceeded ∼60 MΩ. All reported values of membrane potential have been corrected for liquid-junction potentials (Neher, 1992), which were estimated to be ∼10 mV for our recording solutions (Ingram et al., 2019).
Recordings were low-pass filtered at 5 kHz by the patch-clamp amplifier and digitized at 10 kHz with a 16-bit A/D converter (ITC18/USB18, HEKA Elektronik). Data were collected in MATLAB (R2018b, MathWorks) with the open-source software package Symphony Data Acquisition System (https://symphony-das.github.io). All off-line data visualization and analysis was performed with custom scripts and the Iris DVA framework for MATLAB (Khris Griffis, https://github.com/sampath-lab-ucla/IrisDVA). Further zero-phase shift digital filtering was performed off-line with a seventh-order Butterworth filter and the MATLAB FilterM C Mex package. Typical filtering bandwidths were 0–30 Hz, and any deviations from this value for specific experiments are listed in the corresponding figure legends and below in Results.
Light stimulation
Stimuli were delivered with a dual OptoLED light stimulation system (Cairn Research) through a custom-built optical pathway that feeds into the transmitted light path of the physiology microscope. The stimulus and background LEDs had peak wavelengths of 505 ± 5 nm and 405 ± 5 nm. Light sources were attenuated by absorptive neutral-density filters (Thorlabs). At the beginning of each experiment, the microscope field-stop aperture was focused at the level of the slice to provide uniform illumination and was reduced to limit the stimulation region to a spot ∼200 µm in diameter.
The intensities of the LEDs were measured with a calibrated photodiode (Gamma Scientific) through a photodiode amplifier (PDA200C, Thorlabs). Light intensities were calibrated as effective photons per square micrometer and adjusted for the absorption spectrum of rhodopsin (Govardovskii et al., 2000; Nymark et al., 2012). Stimulus intensities were then converted to light-activated rhodopsins per rod (R*) by accounting for the effective collecting area of a rod outer segment.
We estimated the effective collecting area of individual rods from the trial-to-trial variability in the responses to a fixed stimulus. Under the assumption that photon absorption obeys Poisson statistics, the mean number of photoisomerizations produced by the flash,
Response-intensity relationships
To calculate the normalized amplitude of the photoresponse to a given stimulus intensity, we correlated each response with a template generated from the average response across all flash intensities. We then took the amplitude relative to a baseline measured in the 200 ms before flash delivery (Sampath and Rieke, 2004). The amplitudes were scaled by the maximal response to the brightest flashes. This template-scaling procedure produced consistent estimates of the more variable dim-flash responses than measuring peak-current deflections. Response amplitudes were then related to flash intensities, Φ, with a Hill equation as follows:
To determine the effects of background light on maximum response amplitudes (Rmax), responses to saturating flash intensities were recorded in the presence of a variety of background light intensities. These responses were bracketed by saturating flash responses recorded in darkness. A line was fit with respect to time between the peaks of the flashes in darkness, and the predicted maximal response,
In this equation, ΔR = R0 − Rs, and Rs is the settling point of the maximal attenuation. ΔR can then be taken as a metric for the maximal suppression of Rmax by background light exposure (see Fig. 6).
Calculation of rod sensitivity
Rod sensitivity was measured from current responses to dim flashes of 505 nm light in whole-cell voltage clamp at a holding potential of −40 mV. Sensitivity (in pA R*−1) was calculated in darkness and in the presence of background light as the peak amplitude of the response divided by the flash intensity for two to three flash intensities in the linear range of the rod photoreceptor. Mean sensitivities were scaled by those in darkness to give
Experimental design and statistical analyses
All uncertainties were calculated by Monte Carlo simulations (bootstrap) with 10,000 replicates except for time-series data, which instead used 2000 simulations in the interest of reducing computation time. Uncertainty is expressed as 95% confidence intervals about the mean. To increase accuracy and mitigate errors arising from the nonparametric situation, confidence intervals were estimated by the BCa method (Efron, 1987).
In cases where fitting procedures were used, data were binned by logarithmic-spaced intervals, and fits were performed with a total least-squares method, also known as orthogonal regression, by the Total Least Squares Approach to Modeling Toolbox for MATLAB (Petráš and Bednárová, 2010). The fitting procedure was bootstrapped by resampling from the residuals of individual cells (Freedman, 1981; Efron and Tibshirani, 1986). The data were resampled, binned, and fit for 10,000 repetitions generating sampling distributions of model parameters. Uncertainty regions of the fitting parameters are presented as BCa 95% confidence intervals. Uncertainty regions of the regression lines are the 95% confidence intervals generated from each bootstrapped fit over an interpolating region and they were displayed as a shaded region surrounding the fit traces. Statistical significance of fitting parameters, where applicable, was determined from the BCa 95% confidence regions, which corresponds to a 5% α level (p < 0.05) (Efron and Tibshirani, 1986).
Statistical comparisons between BAPTA and control conditions for Rmax experiments were made by first assessing a one-way repeated-measures ANOVA by a custom bootstrap approach for unbalanced design in MATLAB. This custom algorithm is equivalent to the standard linear mixed-effects model, except that bootstrap replicates are calculated from the residuals as the fixed-effects estimator (Freedman, 1981). Post hoc analysis proceeded if the results of ANOVA indicated a significant effect, that is, p < 0.05. Pairwise testing was performed on all pairs by a custom bootstrap algorithm of Welch's t test for unequal variances (Welch, 1938, 1947). To account for multiple testing errors, all p values were adjusted for false discovery rate (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995). Sample sizes, N, are provided in Table 1 or in the Figure legends of corresponding experiments.
Results
Photoreceptor flash sensitivity is dependent on background light levels
Rod photoreceptors can detect single photons (Baylor et al., 1979), and rod-mediated vision operates over approximately seven orders of magnitude of light intensity. This requires substantial adaptation in rods and postrod retinal circuits. To identify rod photoreceptor contributions to adaptation under our experimental conditions, we used patch electrodes to record current responses to brief flashes of light that elicited no more than 20% of the maximal response in darkness and with increasing background light intensity (Fig. 1A, left). To measure flash sensitivities, the peak amplitudes of the current responses were divided by flash intensities (Fig. 1A, right). Sensitivity in background light was divided by sensitivity in darkness, and the resulting ratio was plotted as a function of background intensity (Fig. 1B). These changes in sensitivity agree with previous reports of background dependence of sensitivity in rod photoreceptors (Mendez et al., 2001; Makino et al., 2004; Woodruff et al., 2008; Chen et al., 2010; Morshedian and Fain, 2017), and we found a similar fit to the Weber–Fechner relation (Eq. 3). Our value of I0 = 53 (40, 120) R* s−1 agrees with previous measurements (Morshedian et al., 2018), although somewhat higher than reported by Mendez et al. (2001) or Dunn et al. (2006). Our data confirm a component of rod-pathway adaptation in the rods themselves that may be propagated through downstream circuitry.
Background light reduces nonlinearity of the rod bipolar light response
To characterize RBC adaptation, we measured current responses to brief flashes of light in slice preparations of dark-adapted retinas (Fig. 2). We identified RBC somata visually first by their location at the boundary of the inner nuclear layer and the outer plexiform layer, then by their characteristically large and rapid flash responses, and finally in a few experiments by filling cells with Alexa Fluor 750 added to the recording solution for morphologic verification at the end of the experiment (Fig. 2A). Response families to increasing flash intensities were recorded in a series of background intensities, and responses were averaged at each flash intensity and background (Fig. 2B, top to bottom). Flash intensity ranges used for each background are listed in Table 1.
We saw a clear reduction in maximal response amplitude with increasing background intensity, which was accompanied by an apparent acceleration of response decay. Further, we observed that the steepness of the relationship between flash intensity and response amplitude was reduced in dim backgrounds (Fig. 2C). It should be noted that unlike photoreceptors whose response decay can be adequately described by a first-order decay exponential (Chen et al., 2000), the decay of RBC flash responses is nonuniform across flash intensities and often displayed an oscillatory component. For this reason, we did not quantify a time constant of decay. Instead, we focused on the parameters of the relationship between response amplitude and flash intensity as measures of the effects of background light on RBC flash responses.
To characterize the properties of the RBC flash response, we fit a two-parameter Hill equation (Eq. 1) to normalized response amplitudes as a function of stimulus intensity. Response amplitudes for each cell were calculated from two to five repetitions of flash-intensity families that covered the dynamic range of the RBC response at every background light intensity (Table 1). Maximal responses decreased with increasing background light, falling from −320 (−410, −260) pA in darkness to −18 (−26, −11) pA in a steady background light of 600 R* s−1. To normalize response amplitudes to the range 0–1, amplitudes were scaled by the maximal response amplitude, Rmax, on a cell-to-cell basis (Fig. 2C). We generated response-intensity curves from fitted parameters (Fig. 2C, smooth lines) and 95% confidence intervals (shaded regions) and show them for a selection of background intensities in Figure 2C. In dim backgrounds (producing fewer than ∼2 R* s−1; Fig. 2C, blue curve), we observed a flattening of the response-intensity curve accompanied by almost no shift in the I1/2 parameter (Fig. 2C, compare black and blue curves). When the background intensity was increased to 50 R* s−1 (Fig. 2C, orange curve), a level at which rod sensitivity is reduced by half (Fig. 1B), the response-intensity curve was shifted to brighter intensities by about twofold while not appearing to flatten any further. In the brightest background intensities tested (600 R* s−1, Fig. 2C, green curve), the response-intensity curve shifted further to brighter intensities, reflecting an ∼10-fold decrease in sensitivity. Fitting parameters from all the backgrounds tested are given in Table 1.
The nonlinearity in the RBC response-intensity curve can be quantified by the Hill coefficient n (Sampath and Rieke, 2004). For rods, the response-intensity curve was best fit by Equation 1 with a Hill coefficient of one. For RBCs, the Hill coefficient was much larger in darkness. Fitting Equation 1 to the RBC responses in darkness yielded estimates of n = 1.7 (1.6, 1.9) and I1/2 = 1.3 (1.2, 1.4) R*. Increases in background intensities that were too dim to desensitize rods, that is, producing fewer than ∼3 R* s−1, markedly flattened the response-intensity curve, reducing n to 1.0 (0.89, 1.3) for a 2.8 R* s−1 background (p < 0.05 by 95% CI comparison). Note that over the same background regime, the dimmest flashes on average elicited a greater fractional response, and responses to near-saturating flashes were slightly compressed (Fig. 2C, compare blue to black plots). For visual comparison, n parameter fits are plotted against background intensities in Figure 3A. These results show that relief of nonlinearity occurs at background intensities too dim to elicit changes in rod sensitivity (Fig. 1B).
To estimate changes in RBC flash sensitivity apart from the change in nonlinearity, we took the inverse of the I1/2 parameter as a measure of sensitivity. We then scaled this value by the value of I1/2 in darkness and took this ratio as an estimate of
This rod adaptational influence appears at backgrounds brighter than those that produce the relief in nonlinearity. These observations support the hypothesis that the response-intensity nonlinearity is postsynaptic in origin (Sampath and Rieke, 2004; Okawa et al., 2010a) and that RBC sensitivity may be imparted by rods at brighter background light intensities. Together, our results suggest a mechanism of adaptation separate of rod adaptation that is intrinsic to RBCs (see below, Discussion).
Calcium modulates RBC nonlinearity
To study the effects of calcium on RBC adaptation, we filled the recording pipette with a solution containing 10 mm of the fast calcium chelator BAPTA (see above, Materials and Methods). BAPTA buffering has been shown to reduce transient peaks of the RBC light response and increase nonlinearity of the response-intensity curve (Berntson et al., 2004). We recorded flash response families from RBCs in darkness (Fig. 4A) and generated a response-intensity curve from nine cells (Fig. 4B). Confirming prior reports, we found a slight increase in sensitivity, that is, a reduction in I1/2 [I1/2 = 0.9 (0.79, 1.1) R*, p < 0.05 by 95% CI comparison], and a strong increase in nonlinearity [n = 2.3 (1.9, 2.4) R*, p < 0.05 by 95% CI comparison]. In contrast to previous reports with application of 10 mm BAPTA, we did not observe a significant difference in averaged maximal responses in darkness [−330 (−480, −230) pA] compared with control conditions [Rmax = −320 (−400,−260) pA]. This discrepancy may result from differences in recording solutions. Because Ca2+ entry during the light response is facilitated by TRPM1 channels in the RBC dendrite (Nawy, 2000), these results suggest that Ca2+ entry plays a role in adjusting the linearity of the response.
Background light decreases the RBC maximal response
In the whole-cell patch configuration, RBC flash responses decrease in amplitude over a time span of 1–2 min following break-in. The reason for this rundown is unknown. We found that by decreasing the size of the recording electrode to resistance values of ∼16 MΩ, we could extend the duration of responsiveness by more than a minute. We characterized the rundown in our slice preparations in preliminary experiments, where we repeatedly delivered saturating flashes in darkness. We found that the maximal response amplitude slowly decreased in a linear trend over time, in agreement with similar measurements made in dogfish ON bipolar cells (Shiells and Falk, 1999) and tiger salamander RBCs (Nawy, 2004), albeit on a faster time course. From these results, we devised a protocol to study maximal response amplitudes in mouse RBCs that mitigated the effects of rundown.
The experiment was conducted as shown in Figure 5A. To verify that Rmax amplitude attenuation was because of background light and not rundown, we recorded maximal responses in darkness and in background light from the same cell. A saturating flash was delivered in darkness (intensity 1), and the cell was allowed to recover to baseline. A background light was then turned on and held constant for 8–10 s before a second saturating flash was delivered (intensity 2); the brightness of the second flash was determined from response-intensity curves like those in Figure 2 and Table 1. The background light was turned off, and the cell was allowed another 8–10 s to adapt to darkness. A final saturating flash was then given (intensity 1) before terminating the experiment. Assuming a linear decrease of the maximal response, we fit a line to the peaks in the bracketing dark responses with respect to time (Fig. 5, dashed lines). Using the parameters of these linear fits, we calculated the expected maximal response peak at the time of the measured peak of the response in background light.
As the background light level was increased, the maximal response peak was smaller than predicted (compare peaks at stimulus 2 to corresponding dashed lines). This effect is especially evident for the brightest background of 300 R* s−1, where the initial response to the background light corresponded closely to the predicted maximum given by the dashed line, but the response in the presence of the background to a saturating flash fell far short of this prediction. A full flash series at this 300 R* s−1 background level is given in Figure 2B, where it is evident that the response saturates at a much smaller value of Rmax than in the absence of a background. The RBC in background light behaves as if steady illumination modulates a step in the RBC transduction cascade so that the number of Gαo available to be deactivated or the proportion of TRPM1 channels available to open decreases.
To characterize the relationship between background intensity and attenuation of the maximal response, we took the ratio of the recorded response peaks in background light, Rmax, as a fraction of the predicted maximum,
In brighter backgrounds, attenuation rapidly increased before tapering off around Rmax/
The decrease in Rmax in dim backgrounds requires Ca2+ entry
Because the decrease in Rmax could occur at backgrounds too dim to elicit significant adaptation of the rods (Fig. 1B), the process producing this effect must be occurring within the bipolar cells. To test for an effect of Ca2+ on the decrease in Rmax, we therefore recorded responses to saturating flashes in darkness and in backgrounds with the membrane potential clamped to +50 mV (Nawy, 1999) and with 10 mm BAPTA in the recording pipette (Fig. 5B). This combination minimizes changes in intracellular Ca2+ by decreasing Ca2+ entry and increasing intracellular buffering. Current deflections are outward oriented because the holding potential was near the reversal potential of Na+ (Nernst potential of +56 mV for our solutions). The outward current was probably carried predominantly by K+ efflux.
Using the same protocol and analysis as described earlier (Fig. 5A), we found that the average of peak responses met or slightly exceeded predicted maxima in background levels less than ∼10 R* s−1 (Fig. 5B), indicating that Rmax was unaffected at these dim background intensities (p = 0.29). Consistent with previous reports (Berntson et al., 2004), we found that inclusion of BAPTA in the recording pipette together with a holding potential of +50 mV nearly abolished the rapid transient decay at the onset of the background step. Brighter backgrounds still elicited a slower component of decay that reached a plateau of 45 (35, 56)% of the maximal response peak in darkness. In control conditions at a background intensity producing ∼300 R* s−1, the plateau reached 27 (21, 35)% of the maximal response peak in darkness. Hence, BAPTA significantly decreased the decay of the step response (p = 0.049).
To compare the effects of Ca2+ on the suppression of Rmax, we calculated Rmax/
The decrease in Rmax is not produced by a change in single-channel conductance
One possible explanation of the effect of background light is a change in the single-channel conductance of the TRPM1 channel. To determine whether TRPM1 conductance is affected by background light, we estimated single-channel currents with nonstationary noise analysis (Sampath and Rieke, 2004; Hartveit and Veruki, 2007) in darkness and in background light (Fig. 7A). We found no differences in single-channel currents at the backgrounds tested (levels below 35 R* s−1, ANOVA p = 0.56). Single-channel currents were estimated to be 0.27 (0.24, 0.34) pA in darkness and between 0.10 and 0.52 pA across all backgrounds tested (Fig. 7B). These values agree with previous reports in darkness (0.27 pA in Sampath and Rieke, 2004). From parabolic fits (Hartveit and Veruki, 2007); we estimated the log-fold change in the number of open channels from the steady state before the flash and from the falling phase of the maximal response (Fig. 7C). We found that background light produced a significant reduction of log-fold change in open channels [−2.6 (−3.3, −1.8) in 24 R* s−1 compared with darkness; p = 0.0001]. Thus, brighter background light likely leads to a reduction in the number of channels that are available to open but does not affect the single-channel conductance.
Discussion
We have studied mechanisms of adaptation in mouse retina by making patch-clamp recordings from rods and RBCs in retinal slices in darkness and in background illumination. We characterized changes in RBC responses from the three parameters of the Hill equation, that is, flash sensitivity from I1/2 (intensity at half-maximal response), response-intensity nonlinearity from n, and maximal response amplitude from Rmax. Our experiments indicate three separate effects of background light on RBCs, namely, a decrease in sensitivity reflected in the increase of I1/2 (Fig. 3B), a reduction in the slope of the response-intensity curve reflected by a decrease in n (Fig. 2C), and a surprising decrease in the maximum amplitude of response Rmax. These three effects are likely to be at least in part independent. The decrease in RBC sensitivity in background light occurs only in light bright enough to adapt the rods and is derived directly from the decrease in rod sensitivity (Fig. 3B). The change in the slope of the response-intensity curve occurs at intensities too dim to produce adaptation in rods and is already complete in a background intensity of ∼2-5 Rh* rod−1 s−1 (Fig. 3A). The decrease in Rmax also occurs at intensities too dim to produce adaptation in the rods but continues to be significant in a range of light intensities brighter than those responsible for the change in n (Figs. 5, 6). The effect on Rmax indicates that sustained illumination desensitizes the RBC transduction cascade or produces a change in the conformation of the TRPM1 channels so that they somehow become reluctant to open (Bean, 1989) without a change in their unitary conductance (Fig. 7). Modulation of n and Rmax are both likely to be produced by regulation of some step in the RBC transduction cascade by Ca2+ as both are affected by holding the membrane potential at a positive value and/or dialysis of BAPTA (Figs. 4, 5).
Modulation of I1/2
The sensitivity of RBCs could not be measured as for rods from small-amplitude responses because this method assumes linearity—true for rods but not for dark-adapted RBCs or in dim background light (Figs. 2C, 3A). On the assumption that the inverse of I1/2 is a measure of the sensitivity of the cell (Sf), we compared the change in relative sensitivity in background light for rods and RBCs and show that these relationships are indistinguishable (Fig. 3B). Although RBCs pool rod signals and are more sensitive than single rods, the relative changes in sensitivity produced by background light are practically the same. We conclude that the adaptation of RBC sensitivity is derived directly from adaptation of sensitivity in rods without further synaptic modulation. Our finding for rods agrees with previous measurements of Dunn et al. (2007) for cones, who demonstrated a similar relationship between cones and cone bipolar cells.
Modulation of n
Previous experiments have shown that the response-intensity curve for dark-adapted RBCs is nonlinear, with a Hill coefficient of 1.4–1.7 (Field and Rieke, 2002; Berntson et al., 2004; Sampath and Rieke, 2004). This steepness of the response-intensity curve in darkness allows the synapse to distinguish more easily between small responses that are probably noise from larger responses that are more likely to be driven by light (Van Rossum and Smith, 1998). In the presence of background light, this nonlinearity disappears (Figs. 2C, 3A; Sampath and Rieke, 2004). In brighter illumination, it may be less important to distinguish small-amplitude signals and more useful to preserve a greater range of responsiveness across a larger range of stimuli.
The nonlinearity in dark-adapted RBCs cannot derive from the signals of rods as rod response amplitude increases linearly with stimulus intensity (Field and Rieke, 2002); the Hill coefficient n for rods is close to 1.0. The nonlinearity in dark-adapted RBCs must therefore arise from some feature of synaptic transmission. Our experiments demonstrate that the nonlinearity decreases in background light but becomes even steeper when the solution of the recording pipette contains the Ca2+ buffer BAPTA (Fig. 4; Berntson et al., 2004), suggesting the following hypothesis. In dark-adapted RBCs, glutamate release from rods activates the RBC signal-transduction cascade holding TRPM1 channels mostly closed (Sampath and Rieke, 2004). TRPM1 channels are nonselective cationic and permeable to Ca2+ (Oancea and Wicks, 2011), but as they are closed in darkness, the free-Ca2+ concentration in RBC dendrites should be low. In background light as the channels start to open, the free-Ca2+ concentration would rise, and this increase in Ca2+ may reduce nonlinearity of the response-intensity relationship. If RBCs are dialyzed with the nanomolar-affinity Ca2+ chelator BAPTA from the recording pipette, the buffering of Ca2+ in the RBC may further reduce the effective concentration of Ca2+ in the vicinity of the synapse, and the nonlinearity could then increase (Fig. 4). Although BAPTA would not alter the steady-state concentration of Ca2+ throughout the RBC, it could alter local and time-dependent changes in concentration. We hypothesize that Ca2+ has some effect on the synaptic transduction cascade of the RBC, perhaps near the site of Ca2+ entry in the vicinity of the TRPM1 channels.
Ultimately the dark glutamate release rate sets the extent of RBC nonlinearity. Although postsynaptic mGluR6 receptors are not saturated in darkness (Sampath and Rieke, 2004), their relative occupancy may play a critical role in saturation of the signaling cascade. Previous work has shown that mGluR dimers display a nonlinear increase in transduction efficiency when both subunits bind glutamate (Levitz et al., 2016). A possible explanation for RBC nonlinearity is that in darkness synaptic glutamate rests at a level where the mGluR6 dimers are straddling the singly and doubly liganded state. This interpretation is supported by the linearization of the rod bipolar-cell response-intensity relationship by weak background light (Fig. 3) or by application of a low concentration of a high-affinity antagonist of the mGluR6 receptors (Sampath and Rieke, 2004).
Modulation of Rmax
We show that Rmax, the maximum amplitude of the RBC flash response, is markedly decreased by background light even at intensities too dim to produce adaptation in rods. Moreover, this effect is greatly reduced when the Ca2+ concentration is prevented from changing by a combination of infusion of BAPTA and a holding potential of +50 mV. An increase in Ca2+ in RBC dendrites appears to be producing some change in the transduction cascade or in the TRPM1 channels so that the channels are prevented from opening to their maximal extent but without changing their single-channel conductance (Fig. 7). There could be a Ca2+-dependent effect on Gαo that prevents it from deactivating as much as in a dark-adapted cell or an alteration of the rate of production or destruction of the second messenger of the cascade, whose identity is presently unknown. Alternatively, the TRPM1 channels could transiently enter a conformational state in which they are reluctant to open, much as Bean (1989) showed for neuronal Ca2+ channels modulated by Gβγ.
As in previous reports, we also found that RBC responses to bright light had a characteristic transient peak followed by a rapid sag toward a plateau, which was also eliminated by impeding Ca2+ entry (Fig. 5; Berntson et al., 2004). This effect seems to be produced by Ca2+, perhaps by binding to the TRPM1 channels (Berntson et al., 2004). Ca2+ entering the RBC during presentation of steady illumination could first inhibit opening of the TRPM1 channels as Berntson et al. (2004) suggested, then cause the channels to move slowly into a conformational state that prevents them from opening to their fullest extent. If so, the entry and departure from this state must occur rapidly to explain the time course of the changes we have observed (Fig. 5A). Such effects might occur with a Ca2+-binding site near the channel pore, as has been observed with other members of the TRPM family (Winkler et al., 2017). Further studies of this effect may give additional details about its mechanism and its relationship to the production of the transient peak and rapid decay of the response.
Adaptation to background light in rod bipolar cells
Adaptation is known to operate on many time scales to provide a robust scaling of sensitivity with changing background light. Adaptational effects studied here were characterized on fast time scales following the delivery of background light, but additional mechanisms may contribute to adaptation on longer time scales of 10+ min. These include potential sensitizing effects of cGMP through protein kinase G (Snellman and Nawy, 2004) but also desensitizing effects independent of cGMP by Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II (Walters et al., 1998; Shiells and Falk, 2000) and the Ca2+-dependent phosphatase calcineurin (Snellman and Nawy, 2002). How these mechanisms collectively operate to provide a seamless representation of light intensity as luminance increases remains an open question.
Our experiments provide a comprehensive investigation of the fast effects of background light on mammalian RBCs. These cells synapse onto AII amacrine cells, which convey the rod signals to cone bipolar cells and then to ganglion cells (Fain and Sampath, 2018). It seems likely that rod signals undergo further adaptation downstream in the retina because adaptation of scotopic vision can occur at light intensities even dimmer than those we have used in our experiments (Rushton, 1965), probably in cells pooling an even larger number of rod responses (Dunn and Rieke, 2008). Our work on RBCs should provide the basis of further investigation at more proximal sites in the retina, to discover how adaptation proceeds for rod signals over the whole range of illumination of scotopic vision.
Footnotes
This work was supported by National Institutes of Health–National Eye Institute Grants EY17606 (A.P.S.), EY29817 (A.P.S.), and EY0331 and NEI Core Grant EY00331 (UCLA), and Research to Prevent Blindness Unrestricted Funds to the University of California, Los Angeles Department of Ophthalmology. We thank Prof. Gordon L Fain for help with manuscript preparation.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Alapakkam P. Sampath at asampath{at}jsei.ucla.edu