Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Research Articles, Behavioral/Cognitive

Is Short Sleep Bad for the Brain? Brain Structure and Cognitive Function in Short Sleepers

Anders M. Fjell, Øystein Sørensen, Yunpeng Wang, Inge K. Amlien, William F. C. Baaré, David Bartrés-Faz, Carl-Johan Boraxbekk, Andreas M. Brandmaier, Ilja Demuth, Christian A. Drevon, Klaus P. Ebmeier, Paolo Ghisletta, Rogier Kievit, Simone Kühn, Kathrine Skak Madsen, Lars Nyberg, Cristina Solé-Padullés, Didac Vidal-Piñeiro, Gerd Wagner, Leiv Otto Watne and Kristine B. Walhovd
Journal of Neuroscience 12 July 2023, 43 (28) 5241-5250; https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2330-22.2023
Anders M. Fjell
1Center for Lifespan Changes in Brain and Cognition, University of Oslo, 0373 Oslo, Norway
2Computational Radiology and Artificial Intelligence, Department of Radiology and Nuclear Medicine, Oslo University Hospital, 0424 Oslo, Norway
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Øystein Sørensen
1Center for Lifespan Changes in Brain and Cognition, University of Oslo, 0373 Oslo, Norway
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Yunpeng Wang
1Center for Lifespan Changes in Brain and Cognition, University of Oslo, 0373 Oslo, Norway
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Inge K. Amlien
1Center for Lifespan Changes in Brain and Cognition, University of Oslo, 0373 Oslo, Norway
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Inge K. Amlien
William F. C. Baaré
3Danish Research Centre for Magnetic Resonance (DRCMR), Centre for Functional and Diagnostic Imaging and Research, Copenhagen University Hospital-Amager and Hvidovre, 2650 Hvidovre, Copenhagen, Denmark
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
David Bartrés-Faz
4Departament de Medicina, Facultat de Medicina i Ciències de la Salut, Universitat de Barcelona, and Institut de Neurociències, Universitat de Barcelona, Institut d'Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer (IDIBAPS), 08036 Barcelona, Spain
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Carl-Johan Boraxbekk
3Danish Research Centre for Magnetic Resonance (DRCMR), Centre for Functional and Diagnostic Imaging and Research, Copenhagen University Hospital-Amager and Hvidovre, 2650 Hvidovre, Copenhagen, Denmark
5Umeå Center for Functional Brain Imaging, Umeå University, 907 36 Umeå, Sweden
6Department of Radiation Sciences, Diagnostic Radiology, Umeå University, 907 36 Umeå, Sweden
7Institute of Sports Medicine Copenhagen (ISMC), Copenhagen University Hospital Bispebjerg, 2400 Copenhagen, Denmark
21Institute for Clinical Medicine, Faculty of Medical and Health Sciences, University of Copenhagen, 2020 Copenhagen, Denmark
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Andreas M. Brandmaier
14Center for Lifespan Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, 14195 Berlin, Germany
19Department of Psychology, MSB Medical School Berlin, Berlin, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Ilja Demuth
8Charité–Universitätsmedizin Berlin, corporate member of Freie Universität Berlin and Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Department of Endocrinology and Metabolic Diseases (including Division of Lipid Metabolism), Biology of Aging working group, Augustenburger Platz 1, 13353 Berlin, Germany
22Berlin Institute of Health at Charité – Universitätsmedizin Berlin, 10178 Berlin, Germany
26BCRT - Berlin Institute of Health Center for Regenerative Therapies, 13353 Berlin, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Christian A. Drevon
9Vitas AS, The Science Park, 0349 Oslo, Norway
10Department of Nutrition, Institute of Basic Medical Sciences, Faculty of Medicine, University of 0372 Oslo, Norway
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Christian A. Drevon
Klaus P. Ebmeier
11Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford OX3 7JX, United Kingdom
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Klaus P. Ebmeier
Paolo Ghisletta
12Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Geneva, 1205 Geneva, Switzerland
23UniDistance Suisse, 3900 Brig, Switzerland
24Swiss National Centre of Competence in Research LIVES, University of Geneva, 1205 Geneva, Switzerland
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Rogier Kievit
13Cognitive Neuroscience Department, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behavior, Radboud University Medical Center, 6525 GA Nijmegen, The Netherlands
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Simone Kühn
14Center for Lifespan Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, 14195 Berlin, Germany
15Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, 20251 Hamburg, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Kathrine Skak Madsen
3Danish Research Centre for Magnetic Resonance (DRCMR), Centre for Functional and Diagnostic Imaging and Research, Copenhagen University Hospital-Amager and Hvidovre, 2650 Hvidovre, Copenhagen, Denmark
16Radiography, Department of Technology, University College Copenhagen, 1799 Copenhagen, Denmark
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Lars Nyberg
1Center for Lifespan Changes in Brain and Cognition, University of Oslo, 0373 Oslo, Norway
5Umeå Center for Functional Brain Imaging, Umeå University, 907 36 Umeå, Sweden
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Cristina Solé-Padullés
4Departament de Medicina, Facultat de Medicina i Ciències de la Salut, Universitat de Barcelona, and Institut de Neurociències, Universitat de Barcelona, Institut d'Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer (IDIBAPS), 08036 Barcelona, Spain
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Didac Vidal-Piñeiro
1Center for Lifespan Changes in Brain and Cognition, University of Oslo, 0373 Oslo, Norway
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Didac Vidal-Piñeiro
Gerd Wagner
17Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Jena University Hospital, 07743 Jena, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Leiv Otto Watne
18Oslo Delirium Research Group, Oslo University Hospital, 0424 Oslo, Norway
20Department of Geriatric Medicine, Akershus University Hospital, 1478 Lørenskog, Norway
25Institute of Clinical Medicine, Campus Ahus, University of Oslo, 1478, Lørenskog, Norway
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Kristine B. Walhovd
1Center for Lifespan Changes in Brain and Cognition, University of Oslo, 0373 Oslo, Norway
2Computational Radiology and Artificial Intelligence, Department of Radiology and Nuclear Medicine, Oslo University Hospital, 0424 Oslo, Norway
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Many sleep less than recommended without experiencing daytime sleepiness. According to prevailing views, short sleep increases risk of lower brain health and cognitive function. Chronic mild sleep deprivation could cause undetected sleep debt, negatively affecting cognitive function and brain health. However, it is possible that some have less sleep need and are more resistant to negative effects of sleep loss. We investigated this using a cross-sectional and longitudinal sample of 47,029 participants of both sexes (20–89 years) from the Lifebrain consortium, Human Connectome project (HCP) and UK Biobank (UKB), with measures of self-reported sleep, including 51,295 MRIs of the brain and cognitive tests. A total of 740 participants who reported to sleep <6 h did not experience daytime sleepiness or sleep problems/disturbances interfering with falling or staying asleep. These short sleepers showed significantly larger regional brain volumes than both short sleepers with daytime sleepiness and sleep problems (n = 1742) and participants sleeping the recommended 7–8 h (n = 3886). However, both groups of short sleepers showed slightly lower general cognitive function (GCA), 0.16 and 0.19 SDs, respectively. Analyses using accelerometer-estimated sleep duration confirmed the findings, and the associations remained after controlling for body mass index, depression symptoms, income, and education. The results suggest that some people can cope with less sleep without obvious negative associations with brain morphometry and that sleepiness and sleep problems may be more related to brain structural differences than duration. However, the slightly lower performance on tests of general cognitive abilities warrants closer examination in natural settings.

SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Short habitual sleep is prevalent, with unknown consequences for brain health and cognitive performance. Here, we show that daytime sleepiness and sleep problems are more strongly related to regional brain volumes than sleep duration. However, participants sleeping ≤6 h had slightly lower scores on tests of general cognitive function (GCA). This indicates that sleep need is individual and that sleep duration per se is very weakly if at all related brain health, while daytime sleepiness and sleep problems may show somewhat stronger associations. The association between habitual short sleep and lower scores on tests of general cognitive abilities must be further scrutinized in natural settings.

  • brain
  • cognition
  • hippocampus
  • MRI
  • sleep
  • sleepiness

Introduction

Approximately 50% of people sleep less than the recommended 7–9 h (Hirshkowitz et al., 2015; Watson et al., 2015), with 6.5% sleeping less than six (Kocevska et al., 2021b), many of whom do not report excessive daytime sleepiness. Shorter than recommended sleep is believed to reduce brain health and cognitive performance (Zamore and Veasey, 2022); but guidelines do not take into account two factors. First, partly because of genetic differences (Dashti et al., 2019), sleep need varies. Even vulnerability to sleep loss is a heritable and stable trait, and several genetic polymorphisms are identified (Casale and Goel, 2021). Second, like other physiological systems, sleep need may not be fixed, but is expected to have the ability to adapt to changes in external circumstances (Horne, 2011). This may allow short sleep without sleepiness, reduced brain health and cognitive deficits. Crucially, however, the relationship between sleep duration, daytime sleepiness and neurocognitive function is complex (Horne, 2010). A subset of healthy adults sleeping 7–8 h without daytime sleepiness can still fall asleep within 6 min in the multiple sleep latency test (MSLT; Roehrs et al., 1996). This indicates a level of sleepiness similar to patients with primary sleep disorders, and it was suggested that this could represent accumulated sleep debt from chronically mild insufficient sleep (Roehrs et al., 1996). Lack of daytime sleepiness may result from insensitivity to sleep drive or renormalization in response to chronic sleep deprivation preventing feelings of sleepiness, rather than reflecting lower sleep need (Mander et al., 2017). With extended sleep deprivation, subjective sleepiness can return to baseline levels well before deficits in psychomotor vigilance tasks are normalized (Van Dongen et al., 2003). If such short sleepers are indeed suffering undetected sleep debt despite low levels of daytime sleepiness, neurocognitive deficits may be possible to detect by cognitive tests and brain MRIs. In contrast, if short sleep is due lower sleep need, then such participants are not likely to have poorer cognitive function or brain health.

Duration per se may be a less important indicator of insufficient sleep than daytime sleepiness and problems such as frequently having trouble falling or staying asleep. Short duration (≤6 h) combined with problems and insomnia was associated with higher risk of hypertension, whereas sleeping as short as ≤5 h without problems was not (Vgontzas et al., 2009). Daytime sleepiness was associated with thinner cortex (Carvalho et al., 2017), but sleep duration was not addressed.

Here, we compared cognitive function and brain volumes between short sleepers (≤6 h) with or without sleep problems and daytime sleepiness, and in participants sleeping the recommended 7−8 h. Important aspects of brain health can be measured by structural MRI, which is sensitive to aging (Walhovd et al., 2016) and disease (Fjell et al., 2014). Global brain volume is consistently related to higher general cognitive function (GCA; Walhovd et al., 2022), and atrophy in specific regions has been associated with reduced cognitive function (Gorbach et al., 2020). Brain morphometry and habitual sleep duration form an inverse U-shaped relationship, peaking at ∼7 h (Spira et al., 2016; Fjell et al., 2020b, 2021). We combined data from the Lifebrain consortium (Walhovd et al., 2018), UK Biobank (UKB; Miller et al., 2016), and the Human Connectome Project (HCP; Van Essen et al., 2013) in a mixed cross-sectional and longitudinal design, allowing us to target short (≤6 h) and normal (7−8 h) sleepers with or without daytime sleepiness and sleep problems.

Materials and Methods

Sample

The sample (see Table 1), described in more detail previously (Fjell et al., 2023), consisted of community-dwelling adults from multiple European countries and the United States. All participants gave written informed consent. The Lifebrain project (Walhovd et al., 2018) was approved by the Regional Committees for Medical and Health Research Ethics South East Norway, and substudies approved by the relevant national review boards. For UKB, ethical approval was obtained from the National Health Service National Research Ethics Service (Ref 11/NW/0382). The full sample consisted of 47,029 participants (20−89 years) with information about sleep duration and MRI of the brain and 8694 with general cognitive ability scores, calculated as a g-factor from the available cognitive tests in each sample (for details, see Walhovd et al., 2022). As the specific cognitive test varied across samples (see below), the g-factor was used to reduce the impact of test-specific variance on the results. For 3893 participants, longitudinal MRI examinations were available, yielding a total of 51,295 MRIs (mean follow-up interval 2.5 years, range 0.005–11.2, 26 811 female/24,509 male observations).

Lifebrain

Participants from major European brain studies: Berlin Study of Aging II (BASE II; Bertram et al., 2014; Gerstorf et al., 2016), the BETULA project (Nyberg et al., 2020), the Cambridge Center for Ageing and Neuroscience study (Cam-CAN; Shafto et al., 2014), Whitehall-II (WH-II; Filippini et al., 2014), and Center for Lifespan Changes in Brain and Cognition longitudinal studies (LCBC; Walhovd et al., 2016; Fjell et al., 2018).

UKB

UK Biobank is a national and international health resource open to all bona fide health researchers (https://www.ukbiobank.ac.uk/; Guggenheim et al., 2015), and includes MRI data from a subsample (https://www.ukbiobank.ac.uk/imaging-data/; Miller et al., 2016). The dataset released February 2020 was used.

HCP

HCP freely share data from 1143 young adults (ages 22–35) from families with twins and nontwin siblings, with 3T MRI and behavioral testing. The dataset used was the 1200 Subjects Release.

The general cognitive ability score was calculated as a g-factor per sample, using available tests. More details are given in (Walhovd et al., 2022), but the following test scores were included: the Practical Problems, Figural Analogies, and Letter Series tests (Düzel et al., 2016; Whitehall-II); Block Design test 7, a measure of immediate free recall of 16 enacted verb-noun sentences, a 30-item five-alternative forced choice vocabulary test, four measures of verbal fluency measured during 1 min, as well as a 26-item general knowledge test (Betula; Nilsson et al., 1997), the standard form of the Cattell Culture Fair, Scale 2 Form A (Cattell and Cattell, 1973), the Spot The Word task (Baddeley et al., 1993; CamCAN), Vocabulary and Matrix reasoning from WASI (Wechsler, 1999; LCBC), raw fluid intelligence score from the UKB Data-field 20 016 (UKB), Practical Problems, Figural Analogies, Letter Series (Düzel et al., 2016; BASE-II), Block design and/or vocabulary from WAIS-III (Wechsler, 1997) and National Adult Reading Test (NART; Nelson and Willison, 1991; Barcelona), and for HCP, Flanker, Dimensional Change Card Sort, Picture Sequence Memory, List Sorting and Pattern Comparison, and Picture Vocabulary and Reading Tests.

Classification of participants

Sleep information was available for baseline only for most of the participants. For the small number of participants for whom more than one observation about sleep was available, we used the average value across timepoints. For the HCP and the Lifebrain samples except Betula, sleep characteristics were measured by the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI; Buysse et al., 1989). For Betula, sleep characteristics were measured by The Karolinska Sleep Questionnaire (KSQ; Nordin et al., 2013; Westerlund et al., 2014). For UKB, sleep was measured through multiple questions.

Participants were classified in four groups, according to criteria specified in Table 2. Groups 1 and 2 consisted of short sleepers (≤6 h). Group 1 reported no daytime sleepiness and no sleep problems (“group 1: Short sleep and no sleep problems/sleepiness”), while group 2 reported daytime sleepiness and/or sleep problems (“group 2: Short sleep and sleep problems/sleepiness”). Group 3 and four consisted of participants sleeping the recommended 7−8 h. Like group 1, group 3 participants did not report daytime sleepiness and sleep problems (“group 3: Recommended sleep and no sleep problems/sleepiness”). Like group 2, group 4 participants reported daytime sleepiness and/or sleep problems (“group 4: Recommended sleep and sleep problems/sleepiness”). Since UKB did not include PSQI, the items used for group assignment were not identical for UKB and Lifebrain participants. Participants not satisfying the criteria were ungrouped and not included in the analyses. 19 Lifebrain participants were short sleepers (≤6 h) and experienced daytime sleepiness without reporting sleep problems. This shows that very few participants report to sleep ≤ 6 h and feel tired during the day unless they also have sleep problems. We considered this group to be too small for statistical comparisons. Of the total sample, 9611 participants were successfully classified into the predefined sleep groups. Of participants sleeping ≤6 h, 740 (6%) and 1742 (14%) were classified as belonging to group 1 or 2, respectively. Of participants sleeping 7−8 h, 3886 (12%) and 3243 (10%) were classified as belonging to group 3 or 4, respectively (see Table 3 for details).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Sample origins of the full sample

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2.

Definition of sleep groups

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 3.

Description of sleep groups

The same sleep categories were used across the age-range, although recommended sleep duration range is not identical for younger and older adults. This was done because mean sleep duration in this sample is relatively stable across the age-range (Fjell et al., 2023), and the recommended lower limit of sleep is the same across adulthood (Paruthi et al., 2016; Hirshkowitz et al., 2015). This led to the exclusion of young and middle-aged participants sleeping 8–9 h, which is within the recommended duration, and hence represent a risk of reducing the representativity of the “recommended sleep” groups. However, 75% of the sample reported to sleep <9 h, and so we believe this choice did not bias the results.

Accelerometer-derived sleep duration

We ran validation analyses using sleep duration quantified by accelerometer data for the UKB participants. Raw accelerometer data were downloaded from UK Biobank bulk data field 90 001. The UKB physical activity data includes a large sample of participants that wore wrist-worn accelerometers (Axivity AX3 wrist-worn triaxial accelerometers) for up to 7 consecutive days. Data were processed using the R-package GGIR v2.4.0 (van Hees et al., 2014, 2015, 2022), using the configuration provided previously (Jones et al., 2019). We excluded accelerometer-derived sleep duration data if any of the UKB derived data fields 90002, 90015, 90016, and 90017 indicated data quality issues. Further, we excluded data when the number of data recording errors, number of interrupted recording periods, or the duration of interrupted recording periods exceeded Q3 + 1.5⋅IQR of the sample. The GGIR computed variable SptDurationInSpt (Total sleep duration) was used as the accelerometer derived sleep duration measure.

Magnetic resonance imaging acquisition and analysis

Lifebrain MRI data originated from seven different scanners (for details, see Fjell et al., 2019; processed with FreeSurfer 6.0, https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/; Dale et al., 1999; Fischl et al., 2002; Reuter et al., 2012; Jovicich et al., 2013). Because FreeSurfer is almost fully automated, to avoid introducing possible site-specific biases, gross quality control measures were imposed and no manual editing was done. To assess the influence of scanner on volumetric estimates, seven participants were scanned on seven scanners across the consortium sites (for details, see Fjell et al., 2019). Using hippocampus as test-region, there was a significant main effect of scanner on volume (F = 4.13, p = 0.046), but the between-participant rank order was close perfectly retained between scanners, with a mean between-scanner Pearson correlation of r = 0.98 (range 0.94–1.00). Similar analyses of cortical regions also revealed close correspondence across scanners (Nyberg et al., 2023). Thus, including site as a random effect covariate in the analyses of hippocampal volume is likely sufficient to remove the influence of scanner differences.

UKB participants were scanned using three identical Siemens 3T Prisma scanners (https://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/ukbiobank/protocol/). FreeSurfer outputs (Alfaro-Almagro et al., 2018) and the volumetric scaling from T1 head image to standard space as proxy for Intracranial Volume (ICV) were used in the analyses, generated using publicly available tools, primarily based on FSL (FMRIB Software library, https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki). Details of the imaging protocol (http://biobank.ctsu.ox.ac.uk/crystal/refer.cgi?id=2367) and structural image processing are provided on the UK Biobank website (http://biobank.ctsu.ox.ac.uk/crystal/refer.cgi?id=1977).

HCP imaging data were collected and processed (https://www.humanconnectome.org/study/hcp-young-adult) as described previously (Glasser et al., 2013). Imaging data were collected at a customized Siemens 3T “Connectome Skyra” housed at Washington University in St. Louis, using a standard 32-channel Siemens receive head coil and a “body” transmission coil designed by Siemens specifically for the smaller space available using the special gradients of the WU-Minn and MGH-UCLA Connectome scanners. Images were processed using a custom combination of tools from FSL and FreeSurfer (Jenkinson et al., 2002, 2012; Fischl, 2012).

An overview of scanning parameters across all subsamples in the present study is given in Table 4.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 4.

MR Acquisition parameters

Statistical analyses and data availability

Analyses were run in R version 4.0.0 (R Core Team, 2020), by use of Generalized Additive Mixed Models (GAMM) using the packages “gamm4” version 0.2-26 (Wood and Scheipl, 2020) and “mgcv” version 1.8-28 (Wood, 2017). Advantages with GAMM are first that it represents a nonlinear statistical approach which does not require a priori specification of a polynomial functional form (Sørensen et al., 2021), which is important since the relationship between sleep and brain health may not be linear, and second that it is a mixed model, which means that all data can be included, it deals well with missing data, and the model is optimized to account for both between-subject and within-subject variance. For the main analyses, we ran the model below: mod<− gamm4(value ∼ s(age) + sex + site + sleep + Group + icv,random=∼(1|id), data=dat)

Value represents the brain volume or cognitive score of interest, sleep represents sleep duration, Group represents the different sleep groups, which were pairwise contrasted. All available data were used, and hence random intercepts were included. Separate analyses were run including the interaction term age x Group. Where appropriate, critical p-values were determined by use of the Benjamini–Hochberg procedure with a 5% false discovery rate to control for multiple comparisons.

As we did not specifically model follow-up time between examinations, the results represent the optimal fit to the longitudinal and cross-sectional observations, and should not be interpreted to signify change in brain volumes per se.

Analyses were run for 12 brain regions, the ventricles, total gray matter volume (TGV) and ICV. We tested for whole-brain effects by computing meta analytic estimates of standardized regression coefficients across the 12 regions, using the R package “metafor” (Viechtbauer, 2010). Data supporting the results of the current study are available from the PI of each substudy on request, given appropriate ethics and data protection approvals. Contact information can be obtained from the corresponding authors. UK Biobank data requests can be submitted to http://www.ukbiobank.ac.uk.

Results

Sleep duration versus sleepiness

For the Lifebrain and HCP samples, we defined “sleepiness” from the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) by using the one to four values from item PSQI8 (“trouble staying awake”). Sleepiness and sleep duration correlated r = −0.11 (p < 0.0001), meaning that short sleep on average was associated with slightly more sleepiness, with ∼1% explained variance. Sleep duration and sleepiness hence represents largely independent features.

Brain volumetric comparisons among short sleepers (group 1 vs 2)

Differences in brain volumes between the groups are shown in Table 5 and Figure 1. We performed a meta-analysis across all regions listed in Table 5 (for the ventricles, the sign of the regression coefficient was reversed). Group 2: Short sleep and sleep problems/sleepiness had significantly smaller brain volumes than group 1: Short sleep and no sleep problems/sleepiness (estimate = −0.0041 [CI: −0.0061, −0.0021], SE = 0.001, z = −4. 1, p < 0.0001). Regional comparisons revealed that the differences were most evident for the brain stem and pallidum.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 5.

Differences in brain volumes between groups of short sleepers

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Percent differences in brain volumes between groups. Graphical presentation of the numeric results of Tables 5 (left panel) and 6 (right panel). Left panel, Comparisons between “group 1: Short sleep and no sleep problems/sleepiness” and “group 2: Short sleep and sleep problems/sleepiness,” using group 1 as reference (estimate = 0, vertical line). Dots to the left represent smaller volumes of group 2. Lines are sorted from lowest to highest right CI limit. Middle panel, The same comparison as in the left panel, but group classification is based on agreement between self-reports and accelerometer. Lower CIs are trunctade at −4%. Right panel, Comparisons between “group 3: Recommended sleep and no sleep problems/sleepiness” and “group 4: Recommended sleep and sleep problems/sleepiness,” using group 3 as reference (estimate = 0, dashed line). Dots to the left represent smaller volumes of group 4. Error bars represent 95% CI.

Next, we tested whether each of the two groups of short (≤6 h) sleepers had different volumes from group 3: Recommended sleep and no sleep problems/sleepiness, reporting 7−8 h of sleep. Group 1: Short sleep and no sleep problems/sleepiness had overall larger volumes than group 3 (estimate = 0.0067 [CI: 0.0030, 0.0103], SE = 0.0019, z = −3.60, p < 0.001), driven by differences in brain stem, caudate, cerebellum white matter and cortex volumes. Group 2: Short sleep and sleep problems/sleepiness did not show an overall significant difference in volume compared with group 3 (estimate = −0.0007 [CI: −0.0019, 0.0034], SE = 0.001, z = −0.54, p = 0.59).

Brain volumetric comparisons among participants reporting recommended amount of sleep (group 3 vs 4)

Group 4: Recommended sleep and sleep problems/sleepiness showed smaller volumes for cerebellum WM, corpus callosum and pallidum, as well as ICV, compared with group 3: Recommended sleep and no sleep problems/sleepiness (Table 6; Fig. 1). However, the meta-analysis did not show a significant overall difference across regions (estimate = −0.0012 [CI: −0.0028, 0.0004], SE = 0.0008, z = −1.51, p = 0.13).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 6.

Differences in brain volumes between groups reporting recommended sleep

Comparisons among participants with daytime sleepiness (group 2 vs 4)

Finally, we compared group 2: Short sleep and sleep problems/sleepiness with group 4: Recommended sleep and sleep problems/sleepiness. The groups differed by 1.7 h in mean sleep duration, but the meta-analysis did not show a significant difference in brain volumes (estimate = 0.0022 [CI: −0.0002, 0.0046], SE = 0.0012, z = 1.80, p = 0.07).

Age interactions

Age was included as covariate in all analyses, but we also ran additional analyses including an interaction term age × sleep group to formally test whether effect of sleep group differed as a function of age. For no brain region or group contrast did the age × sleep group term survive correction for multiple comparisons.

Cognitive function

General cognitive function (GCA) scores were available for 8694 of the classified participants. GCA was calculated as the principal component of different cognitive scores available for each sample. There were no significant differences between the groups of short sleepers (group 2 vs 1; estimate = −0.077 SD, t = −1.55, p = 0.12, n = 2173). Among those sleeping 7−8 h, group 4: Recommended sleep and sleep problems/sleepiness showed significantly lower GCA than group 3: Recommended sleep and no sleep problems/sleepiness (estimate = −0.078 SD, t = 6.89, p < 5.89e−12, n = 6521). The effect size was however not larger than for the contrast between the two groups of short sleep, so the difference in significance was because of a substantially larger sample for the latter analysis. A significant difference was also seen between the two groups reporting daytime sleepiness (group 2 vs 4; estimate = −0.03 SD, t = −2.97, p = 0.003). Both group 1: Short sleep and no sleep problems/sleepiness (estimate = −0.16 SD, t = −3.80, p < 0.0002) and group 2: Short sleep and sleep problems/sleepiness (estimate = −0.19 SD, t = −9.94, p < 2.97e−09) showed significantly lower scores than group 3: Recommended sleep and no sleep problems/sleepiness.

Comparing covariates across groups

We compared BMI, depression, and education level across groups (Table 7). ANOVAs showed significant differences across groups for all variables. Comparing groups pairwise for each variable, using group 1: Short sleep and no sleep problems/sleepiness as reference, showed that group 2: Short sleep and sleep problems/sleepiness had higher BMI, more depression symptoms and lower education. Group 3: Recommended sleep and no sleep problems/sleepiness had lower BMI, while group 4: Recommended sleep and sleep problems/sleepiness had more depression symptoms.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 7.

BMI, depression scores, and education level

We repeated the main group analyses (group 1 vs 2 and group 3 vs 4) using each of the above covariates (Fig. 2; Table 8). This did not affect the main results. Thus, although there were differences between the short sleepers with versus without daytime sleepiness on several of the covariates, these could not explain the volumetric brain differences.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 8.

Comparing brain volumes while controlling for additional covariates

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Controlling for additional covariates. Regional brain volumes compared while controlling for additional covariates “group 2: Short sleep and sleep problems/sleepiness” compared with “group 1: Short sleep and no sleep problems/sleepiness.” Error bars represent 95%CI.

Validation analyses: accelerometer estimated sleep duration

We performed validation analyses on participants from UKB for whom accelerometer data were available. Although not always highly correlated, accelerometer-based estimates and self-reported sleep duration typically show convergent validity (Wrzus et al., 2012). Groups were re-computed based both on accelerometer-estimated sleep duration and on self-reports, i.e., we included only participants with both self-reported and accelerometer estimated sleep ≤6 h. This naturally reduced the sample size (group 1: Short sleep and no sleep problems/sleepiness n = 92, group 2: Short sleep and sleep problems/sleepiness n = 274). Group 2 still showed significantly smaller regional brain volumes (estimate = −0.014% [CI: −0.0215, −0.007], SE = 0.0037, z = −3.87, p < 0.001), driven especially by total gray matter, thalamus, putamen, pallidum and caudate, with substantially larger effect sizes compared with classifications based on self-report only (Fig. 1).

Discussion

Sleeping ≤6 h has been associated with smaller regional brain volumes (Fjell et al., 2022). Here, we find that individuals reporting short sleep but not sleep problems or daytime sleepiness show significantly larger volumes than both short sleepers with sleep problem and daytime sleepiness and those sleeping the recommended 7−8 h. One can speculate whether this contributes to explain why these short sleepers do not report feelings of daytime sleepiness. Still, both groups of short sleepers scored significantly lower on tests of general cognitive function. Although these effect sizes also were minute, this observation warrants further examinations.

Individual differences in sleep need

Short sleepers without daytime sleepiness may need less sleep than the average person, allowing them to sleep in a range that normally is associated with smaller volumes (Fjell et al., 2022). However, as discussed above, lack of daytime sleepiness may not be a good indication of sufficient sleep. Subjective sleepiness tends to normalize faster than performance on psychomotor vigilance tests after sleep deprivation (Van Dongen et al., 2003; Zamore and Veasey, 2022). Still, it is unclear whether vigilance deficits and sleepiness detected in the laboratory reflect real sleep debt (Horne 2010, 2011). Even participants not reporting daytime sleepiness can usually easily fall asleep in a sleep-inducing environment, such as during the multiple sleep-latency test. Sleepiness “unmasked” in the laboratory may be less relevant, as the “sleepiness” is small enough to go unnoticed in more stimulating environments, and one should be cautious inferring “sleep debt” in such cases (Horne, 2010). Hence, it seems clear that self-report measures, as used in the present study, answer different questions about daytime sleepiness than objective assessments (Pérez-Carbonell et al., 2022).

This discussion provides an important context for our findings. If the short sleepers, regardless of experienced daytime sleepiness and sleep problems, had lower regional brain volumes this could indicate undetected sleep debt. The finding of slightly larger volumes in the short sleepers without sleep problems and sleepiness does not necessarily mean that this group has lower sleep need, but is still in line with findings of substantial trait-like individual differences in multiple physiological aspects of sleep, such as sleep homeostasis and duration, which in magnitude have been reported to exceed even effects of 36 h of sleep deprivation (Tucker et al., 2007). Such trait variability may partly be accounted for by genetic differences (Linkowski, 1999; Landolt, 2011; Dashti et al., 2019). This explanation fits previous findings that daytime sleepiness is not solely caused by short sleep (Horne, 2010) and that increased duration does not necessarily cause longer sleep latency and less sleepiness even within individuals (Roehrs et al., 1996). It also concurs with the results from a recent study showing very modest effects of variations in within-participant sleep duration on subjective alertness (Vallat et al., 2022). Recent meta-analyses of twin studies found 38% (Madrid-Valero et al., 2020) and 46% (Kocevska et al., 2021a) of the variability in self-reported sleep duration to be explained by genetics, with GWAS estimates typically being around 10% (Garfield, 2021). Heritability of daytime sleepiness is reported to be 0.38–0.48 in twin studies (Carmelli et al., 2001; Desai et al., 2004; Watson et al., 2006) and 0.08–0.29 (Gottlieb et al., 2007; Lane et al., 2017) with GWAS. Importantly, the genetic overlap between sleep duration, daytime sleepiness, and vulnerability to sleep loss is modest. One study reported a genetic correlation between sleep duration and daytime sleepiness of 0.22 (Wang et al., 2019), and none of the loci associated with duration were associated with sleepiness. The low duration–sleepiness correlation in the present study is in accordance with this, and fits the interpretation that habitual sleep duration and sleepiness are partly independent, trait-like characteristics with different associations to brain health and cognitive function. Differences in sleep quality may further contribute to reduce the relationship between sleep duration and sleepiness. Without taking the individual differences perspective into account, large natural variability in aspects of sleep may lead to spurious, sample-dependent sleep-brain correlations without functional significance (Landolt, 2011).

Still, although short sleep was not associated with smaller regional brain volumes, the short sleepers scored lower on tests of general cognitive abilities. The effect sizes were equal to 2.9 and 2.4 IQ-points for the short sleepers with and without sleep problems and daytime sleepiness, respectively. This could indicate that these participants sleep less than optimal, in accordance with experimentally induced sleep deprivation yielding reduced cognitive function (Lowe et al., 2017). However, a meta-analysis did not find an effect of sleep deprivation on intelligence and reasoning measures (Lowe et al., 2017), which are the tests most similar to the present measures of cognitive function. In addition, most sleep deprivation experiments involve rapid and dramatic reductions in sleep duration, unlike natural variations in habitual sleep duration between people. A recent very large observational study found ∼7 h of sleep to be associated with the highest cognitive function, and <6 h to be associated with mildly lower performance (Coutrot et al., 2022). This fits the results of the present study. Still, we were surprised to find that there were no differences in general cognitive ability between the short sleepers with and without sleep problems and sleepiness, as we expected to see lower scores primarily in the first group. The direction of causality and the possible influence of third variables cannot be decided based on the present data, and must await experimental testing in naturalistic settings, involving modest changes in sleep duration lasting for prolonged periods to mimic everyday sleep duration–cognition relationships.

A complementary account for why the short sleepers in the present study did not show smaller regional brain volumes is that neurocognitive consequences of short sleep depend on adaptation. Sudden sleep deprivation beyond certain limits has negative effects on cognitive performance (Van Dongen et al., 2003; Killgore, 2010) and brain structure (Liu et al., 2014; Saletin et al., 2016; Voldsbekk et al., 2021; Zamore and Veasey, 2022). However, adaptation over time within these limits are unlikely to be harmful to health (Freidmann et al., 1977; Mullaney et al., 1977; Horne, 2011), and may account for the weak relationship between sleep duration and brain morphometry. Reductions in sleep duration can seemingly be obtained without increases in daytime sleepiness or reductions in cognitive performance (Horne and Wilkinson, 1985; Youngstedt et al., 2009). This suggests that sleep duration is adaptable in response to environmental conditions, in line with the present results that variations in sleep duration per se is of less importance for regional brain volume if daytime sleepiness is low. Thus, a combination of adaptation and genetic propensities may create less sleep need and protect from potentially negative consequences of short sleep.

As the present study is correlational and targets brain morphometry, we believe it is not warranted to make strong anatomic interpretations from the results. Still, the regions showing volumetric differences between the short sleepers with versus without sleepiness and sleep problems included the brain stem, which has a critical role in sleep regulation, especially in control of REM sleep (Siegel, 2022). Further, regions of the basal ganglia, especially pallidum and caudate, showed trends toward volumetric differences, and it is suggested that the basal ganglia may play an integral role in the sleep-wake cycle (Hasegawa et al., 2020), with basal ganglia GABAergic neurons possibly representing a functional hub for sleep control (Adamantidis et al., 2021). However, many brain regions are involved on sleep regulation, so we caution about specific neuroanatomical claims based on the present study.

Limitations

(1) Morphometric brain measures and general cognitive ability were used as measures of brain health and cognitive function. Other measures could have yielded different results. e.g., different cognitive domains likely show different relationships to sleep, and the same may be true for other indexes of brain health such as Aβ deposition (Fjell et al., 2020a). (2) The samples were not thoroughly screened for sleep disorders such as sleep apnea, which could affect the observed sleep – brain relationships. (3) The samples are not representative of the populations from which they are drawn, and other sleep-brain patterns may exist in other populations. Further, the majority of participants are white, while sleep loss and sleep problems have been shown to be more prevalent in the United States Black than White population (Jean-Louis et al., 2022). (4) Daytime sleepiness is measured as part of PSQI, but other instruments, like the Epworth Sleepiness Scale, may be more sensitive to excessive daytime sleepiness (Pérez-Carbonell et al., 2022). (5) The strict classification of participants was used to ensure that the groups were as homogenous as possible with regard to sleep variables, but caused most participants to be unclassified. For instance, participants who responded “once or twice a week” on the question of problems sleeping within 30 min, would not be included in the group without sleep problems. Items such as “nap during the day” may both reflect a cultural phenomenon as well as sleep done for pleasure, not necessarily because of sleepiness or sleep need, but would still be assessed as “daytime sleepiness or sleep problems.” As a consequence, only 20% of participants sleeping ≤6 h were classified. The remaining 80% had very slight to slight sleep problems or daytime sleepiness, hence not fitting either group. (6) Relationships between sleep and the brain may develop over long time. In the present analyses, sleep duration was for most participants measured at a single time point, making it impossible to distinguish stable from changing sleep patterns. Still, we have previously found good stability in self-reported sleep over time (Fjell et al., 2018; Tsiknia et al., 2023), and another study found current self-reported sleep quality to be as tightly related to brain characteristics as longitudinal measures of sleep (Sexton et al., 2017).

In conclusion, some people sleep ≤6 h without showing lower regional brain volumes, despite sleeping within a range where smaller regional brain volumes are expected. Hence, short sleep is not necessarily associated with negative structural brain outcomes. In contrast, short sleepers showed slightly lower general cognitive abilities, although the causality is unclear. At the same time, the present results suggest that there are large differences in sleep need because of genetic and environmental factors, making general recommendations about sleep duration problematic when it comes to brain health and general cognitive function.

Footnotes

  • The Lifebrain project is funded by the EU Horizon 2020 Grant Agreement Number 732592 (Lifebrain). In addition, the different substudies are supported by different sources. The Center for Lifespan Changes in Brain and Cognition (LCBC) is supported by European Research Council Grant Agreements 283634 and 725025 (to A.M.F.) and 313440 (to K.B.W.) as well as the Norwegian Research Council (to A.M.F., K.B.W.) and The National Association for Public Health's Dementia Research Program, Norway (A.M.F.). Betula is supported by a scholar grant from the Knut and Alice Wallenberg (KAW) Foundation (L.N.). Barcelona is partially supported by a Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness (MINECO) grant and ICREA Academia 2019 grants to D.B.-F. [grant number PSI2015-64227-R (AEI/FEDER, UE)] and by the Walnuts and Healthy Aging Study (http://www.clinicaltrials.gov; Grant NCT01634841) funded by the California Walnut Commission, Sacramento, California. BASE-II has been supported by the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research Grant Numbers 16SV5537, 16SV5837, 16SV5538, 16SV5536K, 01UW0808, 01UW0706, 01GL1716A, and 01GL1716B and the European Research Council Grant Agreement 677804 (to S.K.). Work on the Whitehall II Imaging Substudy was mainly funded by the Lifelong Health and Well-being Program Grant G1001354 from the United Kingdom Medical Research Council (“Predicting MRI Abnormalities with Longitudinal Data of the Whitehall II Substudy”; to K.P.E.). The Wellcome Center for Integrative Neuroimaging is supported by core funding from the Wellcome Trust Award 203139/Z/16/Z. Data were provided in part by the Human Connectome Project, WU-Minn Consortium (Principal Investigators: David Van Essen and Kamil Ugurbil; 1U54MH091657) funded by the 16 National Institutes of Health (NIH) and Centers that support the NIH Blueprint for Neuroscience Research, and by the McDonnell Center for Systems Neuroscience at Washington University. Part of the research was conducted using the United Kingdom Biobank resource under application number 32048.

  • C.A.D. is a cofounder, stockowner, board member, and consultant in the contract laboratory Vitas AS, performing personalized analyses of blood biomarkers. All other authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Anders M. Fjell at andersmf{at}psykologi.uio.no

SfN exclusive license.

References

  1. ↵
    1. Adamantidis A,
    2. Fort P,
    3. Luppi PH
    (2021) The neurophysiology of wakefulness and non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep. In: Sleep medicine textbook (Bassetti C, McNicholas W, Paunio T, and Peigneux P, eds), Edition 2. Regensburg: European Sleep Research Society.
  2. ↵
    1. Alfaro-Almagro F, et al
    . (2018) Image processing and quality control for the first 10,000 brain imaging datasets from UK Biobank. Neuroimage 166:400–424. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.10.034 pmid:29079522
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Baddeley A,
    2. Emslie H,
    3. Nimmo-smith I
    (1993) The spot-the-word test - a robust estimate of verbal intelligence based on lexical decision. Br J Clin Psychol 32:55–65. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8260.1993.tb01027.x pmid:8467274
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Bertram L,
    2. Böckenhoff A,
    3. Demuth I,
    4. Düzel S,
    5. Eckardt R,
    6. Li SC,
    7. Lindenberger U,
    8. Pawelec G,
    9. Siedler T,
    10. Wagner GG,
    11. Steinhagen-Thiessen E
    (2014) Cohort profile: the Berlin Aging Study II (BASE-II). Int J Epidemiol 43:703–712. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyt018 pmid:23505255
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Buysse DJ,
    2. Reynolds CF 3rd.,
    3. Monk TH,
    4. Berman SR,
    5. Kupfer DJ
    (1989) The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res 28:193–213. https://doi.org/10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4 pmid:2748771
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Carmelli D,
    2. Bliwise DL,
    3. Swan GE,
    4. Reed T
    (2001) A genetic analysis of the Epworth Sleepiness Scale in 1560 World War II male veteran twins in the NAS-NRC twin registry. J Sleep Res 10:53–58. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2869.2001.00241.x pmid:11285055
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Carvalho DZ,
    2. St Louis EK,
    3. Boeve BF,
    4. Mielke MM,
    5. Przybelski SA,
    6. Knopman DS,
    7. Machulda MM,
    8. Roberts RO,
    9. Geda YE,
    10. Petersen RC,
    11. Jack CR Jr.,
    12. Vemuri P
    (2017) Excessive daytime sleepiness and fatigue may indicate accelerated brain aging in cognitively normal late middle-aged and older adults. Sleep Med 32:236–243. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2016.08.023 pmid:28065685
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Casale CE,
    2. Goel N
    (2021) Genetic markers of differential vulnerability to sleep loss in adults. Genes (Basel) 12:1317. https://doi.org/10.3390/genes12091317 pmid:34573301
    OpenUrlPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Cattell RB,
    2. Cattell HEP
    (1973) Measuring intelligence with the culture fair tests. Champaign: The Institute for Personality and Ability Testing.
  10. ↵
    1. Coutrot A,
    2. Lazar AS,
    3. Richards M,
    4. Manley E,
    5. Wiener JM,
    6. Dalton RC,
    7. Hornberger M,
    8. Spiers HJ
    (2022) Reported sleep duration reveals segmentation of the adult life-course into three phases. Nat Commun 13:7697. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-022-34624-8 pmid:36509747
    OpenUrlPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Dale AM,
    2. Fischl B,
    3. Sereno MI
    (1999) Cortical surface-based analysis. I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage 9:179–194. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.1998.0395 pmid:9931268
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Dashti HS, et al
    . (2019) Genome-wide association study identifies genetic loci for self-reported habitual sleep duration supported by accelerometer-derived estimates. Nat Commun 10:1100. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-019-08917-4 pmid:30846698
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Desai AV,
    2. Cherkas LF,
    3. Spector TD,
    4. Williams AJ
    (2004) Genetic influences in self-reported symptoms of obstructive sleep apnoea and restless legs: a twin study. Twin Res 7:589–595. https://doi.org/10.1375/1369052042663841 pmid:15607009
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Düzel S,
    2. Voelkle MC,
    3. Düzel E,
    4. Gerstorf D,
    5. Drewelies J,
    6. Steinhagen-Thiessen E,
    7. Demuth I,
    8. Lindenberger U
    (2016) The Subjective Health Horizon Questionnaire (SHH-Q): assessing future time perspectives for facets of an active lifestyle. Gerontology 62:345–353. https://doi.org/10.1159/000441493 pmid:26820307
    OpenUrlPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Filippini N, et al
    . (2014) Study protocol: the Whitehall II imaging sub-study. BMC Psychiatry 14:159. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-244X-14-159 pmid:24885374
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Fischl B,
    2. Salat DH,
    3. Busa E,
    4. Albert M,
    5. Dieterich M,
    6. Haselgrove C,
    7. van der Kouwe A,
    8. Killiany R,
    9. Kennedy D,
    10. Klaveness S,
    11. Montillo A,
    12. Makris N,
    13. Rosen B,
    14. Dale AM
    (2002) Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron 33:341–355. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00569-x pmid:11832223
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Fischl B
    (2012) FreeSurfer. Neuroimage 62:774–781. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.021 pmid:22248573
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Fjell AM,
    2. McEvoy L,
    3. Holland D,
    4. Dale AM,
    5. Walhovd KB
    ; Initiative Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging (2014) What is normal in normal aging? Effects of aging, amyloid and Alzheimer's disease on the cerebral cortex and the hippocampus. Prog Neurobiol 117:20–40. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2014.02.004 pmid:24548606
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Fjell AM,
    2. Idland AV,
    3. Sala-Llonch R,
    4. Watne LO,
    5. Borza T,
    6. Brækhus A,
    7. Lona T,
    8. Zetterberg H,
    9. Blennow K,
    10. Wyller TB,
    11. Walhovd KB
    (2018) Neuroinflammation and tau interact with amyloid in predicting sleep problems in aging independently of atrophy. Cereb Cortex 28:2775–2785. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhx157 pmid:28655157
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Fjell AM,
    2. Sederevicius D,
    3. Sneve MH,
    4. de Lange AG,
    5. Bråthen AC,
    6. Idland AV,
    7. Watne LO,
    8. Wang Y,
    9. Reinbold C,
    10. Dobricic V,
    11. Kilpert F,
    12. Blennow K,
    13. Zetterbergj H,
    14. Hong S,
    15. Bertram L,
    16. Walhovd KB
    ; Initiative Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging (2020a) Self-reported sleep problems related to amyloid deposition in cortical regions with high HOMER1 gene expression. Cereb Cortex 30:2144–2156. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhz228 pmid:32142100
    OpenUrlPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Fjell AM, et al
    . (2020b) Self-reported sleep relates to hippocampal atrophy across the adult lifespan: results from the Lifebrain consortium. Sleep 43:zsz280.
    OpenUrl
  22. ↵
    1. Fjell AM, et al
    . (2021) Poor self-reported sleep is related to regional cortical thinning in aging but not memory decline-results from the Lifebrain consortium. Cereb Cortex 31:1953–1969. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhaa332 pmid:33236064
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Fjell AM, et al
    . (2023) No phenotypic or genotypic evidence for a link between sleep duration and brain atrophy. Nature Human Behavior, in press.
  24. ↵
    1. Freidmann J,
    2. Globus G,
    3. Huntley A,
    4. Mullaney D,
    5. Naitoh P,
    6. Johnson L
    (1977) Performance and mood during and after gradual sleep reduction. Psychophysiology 14:245–250. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1977.tb01169.x pmid:854553
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Garfield V
    (2021) Sleep duration: a review of genome-wide association studies (GWAS) in adults from 2007 to 2020. Sleep Med Rev 56:101413. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2020.101413 pmid:33338765
    OpenUrlPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Gerstorf D,
    2. Bertram L,
    3. Lindenberger U,
    4. Pawelec G,
    5. Demuth I,
    6. Steinhagen-Thiessen E,
    7. Wagner GG
    (2016) Editorial. Gerontology 62:311–315. https://doi.org/10.1159/000441495 pmid:26820471
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Glasser MF,
    2. Sotiropoulos SN,
    3. Wilson JA,
    4. Coalson TS,
    5. Fischl B,
    6. Andersson JL,
    7. Xu J,
    8. Jbabdi S,
    9. Webster M,
    10. Polimeni JR,
    11. Van Essen DC,
    12. Jenkinson M
    ; WU-Minn HCP Consortium (2013) The minimal preprocessing pipelines for the Human Connectome Project. Neuroimage 80:105–124. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.127 pmid:23668970
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    1. Gorbach T,
    2. Pudas S,
    3. Bartres-Faz D,
    4. Brandmaier AM,
    5. Düzel S,
    6. Henson RN,
    7. Idland AV,
    8. Lindenberger U,
    9. Macia Bros D,
    10. Mowinckel AM,
    11. Sole-Padulles C,
    12. Sorensen O,
    13. Walhovd KB,
    14. Watne LO,
    15. Westerhausen R,
    16. Fjell AM,
    17. Nyberg L
    (2020) Longitudinal association between hippocampus atrophy and episodic-memory decline in non-demented APOE epsilon4 carriers. Alzheimers Dement (Amst) 12:e12110. https://doi.org/10.1002/dad2.12110 pmid:33015312
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    1. Gottlieb DJ,
    2. O'Connor GT,
    3. Wilk JB
    (2007) Genome-wide association of sleep and circadian phenotypes. BMC Med Genet 8 [Suppl 1]:S9. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2350-8-S1-S9 pmid:17903308
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Guggenheim JA,
    2. Williams C
    ; UK Biobank Eye and Vision Consortium (2015) Role of educational exposure in the association between myopia and birth order. Jama Ophthalmol 133:1408–1414. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamaophthalmol.2015.3556 pmid:26448589
    OpenUrlPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Hasegawa H,
    2. Selway R,
    3. Gnoni V,
    4. Beniczky S,
    5. Williams SCR,
    6. Kryger M,
    7. Ferini-Strambi L,
    8. Goadsby P,
    9. Leschziner GD,
    10. Ashkan K,
    11. Rosenzweig I
    (2020) The subcortical belly of sleep: new possibilities in neuromodulation of basal ganglia? Sleep Med Rev 52:101317. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2020.101317 pmid:32446196
    OpenUrlPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. Hirshkowitz M,
    2. Whiton K,
    3. Albert SM,
    4. Alessi C,
    5. Bruni O,
    6. DonCarlos L,
    7. Hazen N,
    8. Herman J,
    9. Adams Hillard PJ,
    10. Katz ES,
    11. Kheirandish-Gozal L,
    12. Neubauer DN,
    13. O'Donnell AE,
    14. Ohayon M,
    15. Peever J,
    16. Rawding R,
    17. Sachdeva RC,
    18. Setters B,
    19. Vitiello MV,
    20. Ware JC
    (2015) National Sleep Foundation's updated sleep duration recommendations: final report. Sleep Health 1:233–243. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleh.2015.10.004 pmid:29073398
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Horne J
    (2010) Sleepiness as a need for sleep: when is enough, enough? Neurosci Biobehav Rev 34:108–118. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.07.009 pmid:19643131
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Horne J
    (2011) The end of sleep: 'sleep debt' versus biological adaptation of human sleep to waking needs. Biol Psychol 87:1–14.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Horne JA,
    2. Wilkinson S
    (1985) Chronic sleep reduction: daytime vigilance performance and EEG measures of sleepiness, with particular reference to “practice” effects. Psychophysiology 22:69–78. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1985.tb01562.x pmid:3975321
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Jean-Louis G,
    2. Grandner MA,
    3. Seixas AA
    (2022) Social determinants and health disparities affecting sleep. Lancet Neurol 21:864–865. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(22)00347-7 pmid:36115351
    OpenUrlPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Jenkinson M,
    2. Bannister P,
    3. Brady M,
    4. Smith S
    (2002) Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. Neuroimage 17:825–841. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1132
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    1. Jenkinson M,
    2. Beckmann CF,
    3. Behrens TE,
    4. Woolrich MW,
    5. Smith SM
    (2012) FSL. Neuroimage 62:782–790. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.015 pmid:21979382
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Jones SE, et al
    . (2019) Genetic studies of accelerometer-based sleep measures yield new insights into human sleep behaviour. Nat Commun 10:1585. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-019-09576-1 pmid:30952852
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Jovicich J, et al
    . (2013) Brain morphometry reproducibility in multi-center 3T MRI studies: a comparison of cross-sectional and longitudinal segmentations. Neuroimage 83:472–484. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.007 pmid:23668971
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. Killgore WD
    (2010) Effects of sleep deprivation on cognition. Prog Brain Res 185:105–129. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-444-53702-7.00007-5 pmid:21075236
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. Kocevska D,
    2. Barclay NL,
    3. Bramer WM,
    4. Gehrman PR,
    5. Van Someren EJW
    (2021a) Heritability of sleep duration and quality: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Med Rev 59:101448. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2021.101448 pmid:33636423
    OpenUrlPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Kocevska D, et al
    . (2021b) Sleep characteristics across the lifespan in 1.1 million people from The Netherlands, United Kingdom and United States: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Nat Hum Behav 5:113–122. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-00965-x pmid:33199855
    OpenUrlPubMed
  44. ↵
    1. Landolt HP
    (2011) Genetic determination of sleep EEG profiles in healthy humans. Prog Brain Res 193:51–61. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-444-53839-0.00004-1 pmid:21854955
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    1. Lane JM,
    2. Liang J,
    3. Vlasac I,
    4. Anderson SG,
    5. Bechtold DA,
    6. Bowden J,
    7. Emsley R,
    8. Gill S,
    9. Little MA,
    10. Luik AI,
    11. Loudon A,
    12. Scheer FA,
    13. Purcell SM,
    14. Kyle SD,
    15. Lawlor DA,
    16. Zhu X,
    17. Redline S,
    18. Ray DW,
    19. Rutter MK,
    20. Saxena R
    (2017) Genome-wide association analyses of sleep disturbance traits identify new loci and highlight shared genetics with neuropsychiatric and metabolic traits. Nat Genet 49:274–281. https://doi.org/10.1038/ng.3749 pmid:27992416
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    1. Linkowski P
    (1999) EEG sleep patterns in twins. J Sleep Res 8 [Suppl 1]:11–13. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2869.1999.00002.x pmid:10389101
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    1. Liu C,
    2. Kong XZ,
    3. Liu X,
    4. Zhou R,
    5. Wu B
    (2014) Long-term total sleep deprivation reduces thalamic gray matter volume in healthy men. Neuroreport 25:320–323. https://doi.org/10.1097/WNR.0000000000000091 pmid:24346259
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    1. Lowe CJ,
    2. Safati A,
    3. Hall PA
    (2017) The neurocognitive consequences of sleep restriction: a meta-analytic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 80:586–604. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.07.010 pmid:28757454
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    1. Madrid-Valero JJ,
    2. Rubio-Aparicio M,
    3. Gregory AM,
    4. Sánchez-Meca J,
    5. Ordoñana JR
    (2020) Twin studies of subjective sleep quality and sleep duration, and their behavioral correlates: systematic review and meta-analysis of heritability estimates. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 109:78–89. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.12.028 pmid:31899301
    OpenUrlPubMed
  50. ↵
    1. Mander BA,
    2. Winer JR,
    3. Walker MP
    (2017) Sleep and human aging. Neuron 94:19–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2017.02.004 pmid:28384471
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    1. Miller KL, et al
    . (2016) Multimodal population brain imaging in the UK Biobank prospective epidemiological study. Nat Neurosci 19:1523–1536. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4393 pmid:27643430
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    1. Mullaney DJ,
    2. Johnson LC,
    3. Naitoh JP,
    4. Friedmann JK,
    5. Globus GG
    (1977) Sleep during and after gradual sleep reduction. Psychophysiology 14:237–244. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1977.tb01168.x pmid:854552
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    1. Nelson H,
    2. Willison J
    (1991) The National Adult Reading Test (NART). Windsor: NFER-Nelson.
  54. ↵
    1. Nilsson LG,
    2. Bäckman L,
    3. Erngrund K,
    4. Nyberg L,
    5. Adolfsson R,
    6. Bucht G,
    7. Karlsson S,
    8. Widing M,
    9. Winblad B
    (1997) The Betula prospective cohort study: memory, health, and aging. Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 4:1–32. https://doi.org/10.1080/13825589708256633
    OpenUrl
  55. ↵
    1. Nordin M,
    2. Åkerstedt T,
    3. Nordin S
    (2013) Psychometric evaluation and normative data for the Karolinska Sleep Questionnaire. Sleep Biol Rhythms 11:216–226. https://doi.org/10.1111/sbr.12024
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  56. ↵
    1. Nyberg L,
    2. Boraxbekk CJ,
    3. Sörman DE,
    4. Hansson P,
    5. Herlitz A,
    6. Kauppi K,
    7. Ljungberg JK,
    8. Lövheim H,
    9. Lundquist A,
    10. Adolfsson AN,
    11. Oudin A,
    12. Pudas S,
    13. Rönnlund M,
    14. Stiernstedt M,
    15. Sundström A,
    16. Adolfsson R
    (2020) Biological and environmental predictors of heterogeneity in neurocognitive ageing: evidence from Betula and other longitudinal studies. Ageing Res Rev 64:101184. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.arr.2020.101184 pmid:32992046
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  57. ↵
    1. Nyberg L, et al
    . (2023) Individual differences in brain aging: heterogeneity in cortico-hippocampal but not caudate atrophy rates. Cereb Cortex 33:5075–5081.
    OpenUrl
  58. ↵
    1. Paruthi S,
    2. Brooks LJ,
    3. D'Ambrosio C,
    4. Hall WA,
    5. Kotagal S,
    6. Lloyd RM,
    7. Malow BA,
    8. Maski K,
    9. Nichols C,
    10. Quan SF,
    11. Rosen CL,
    12. Troester MM,
    13. Wise MS
    (2016) Consensus statement of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine on the recommended amount of sleep for healthy children: methodology and discussion. J Clin Sleep Med 12:1549–1561. https://doi.org/10.5664/jcsm.6288 pmid:27707447
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    1. Perez-Carbonell L,
    2. Mignot E,
    3. Leschziner G,
    4. Dauvilliers Y
    (2022) Understanding and approaching excessive daytime sleepiness. Lancet 400:1033–1046.
    OpenUrl
  60. ↵
    R Core Team (2020) R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available at: https://www.R-project.org/.
  61. ↵
    1. Reuter M,
    2. Schmansky NJ,
    3. Rosas HD,
    4. Fischl B
    (2012) Within-subject template estimation for unbiased longitudinal image analysis. Neuroimage 61:1402–1418. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.084 pmid:22430496
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  62. ↵
    1. Roehrs T,
    2. Shore E,
    3. Papineau K,
    4. Rosenthal L,
    5. Roth T
    (1996) A two-week sleep extension in sleepy normals. Sleep 19:576–582. pmid:8899937
    OpenUrlPubMed
  63. ↵
    1. Saletin JM,
    2. Goldstein-Piekarski AN,
    3. Greer SM,
    4. Stark S,
    5. Stark CE,
    6. Walker MP
    (2016) Human hippocampal structure: a novel biomarker predicting mnemonic vulnerability to, and recovery from, sleep deprivation. J Neurosci 36:2355–2363. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3466-15.2016 pmid:26911684
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  64. ↵
    1. Sexton CE,
    2. Zsoldos E,
    3. Filippini N,
    4. Griffanti L,
    5. Winkler A,
    6. Mahmood A,
    7. Allan CL,
    8. Topiwala A,
    9. Kyle SD,
    10. Spiegelhalder K,
    11. Singh-Manoux A,
    12. Kivimaki M,
    13. Mackay CE,
    14. Johansen-Berg H,
    15. Ebmeier KP
    (2017) Associations between self-reported sleep quality and white matter in community-dwelling older adults: a prospective cohort study. Hum Brain Mapp 38:5465–5473. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23739 pmid:28745016
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  65. ↵
    1. Shafto MA,
    2. Tyler LK,
    3. Dixon M,
    4. Taylor JR,
    5. Rowe JB,
    6. Cusack R,
    7. Calder AJ,
    8. Marslen-Wilson WD,
    9. Duncan J,
    10. Dalgleish T,
    11. Henson RN,
    12. Brayne C,
    13. Matthews FE
    ; Cam-CAN (2014) The Cambridge Centre for Ageing and Neuroscience (Cam-CAN) study protocol: a cross-sectional, lifespan, multidisciplinary examination of healthy cognitive ageing. BMC Neurol 14:204. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12883-014-0204-1 pmid:25412575
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  66. ↵
    1. Siegel JM
    (2022) Sleep function: an evolutionary perspective. Lancet Neurol 21:937–946. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(22)00210-1 pmid:36115365
    OpenUrlPubMed
  67. ↵
    1. Sørensen O,
    2. Walhovd KB,
    3. Fjell AM
    (2021) A recipe for accurate estimation of lifespan brain trajectories, distinguishing longitudinal and cohort effects. Neuroimage 226:117596. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117596 pmid:33248257
    OpenUrlPubMed
  68. ↵
    1. Spira AP,
    2. Gonzalez CE,
    3. Venkatraman VK,
    4. Wu MN,
    5. Pacheco J,
    6. Simonsick EM,
    7. Ferrucci L,
    8. Resnick SM
    (2016) Sleep duration and subsequent cortical thinning in cognitively normal older adults. Sleep 39:1121–1128. https://doi.org/10.5665/sleep.5768 pmid:26951390
    OpenUrlPubMed
  69. ↵
    1. Tsiknia AA,
    2. Parada H Jr.,
    3. Banks SJ,
    4. Reas ET
    (2023) Sleep quality and sleep duration predict brain microstructure among community-dwelling older adults. Neurobiol Aging 125:90–97. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2023.02.001 pmid:36871334
    OpenUrlPubMed
  70. ↵
    1. Tucker AM,
    2. Dinges DF,
    3. Van Dongen HP
    (2007) Trait interindividual differences in the sleep physiology of healthy young adults. J Sleep Res 16:170–180. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2869.2007.00594.x pmid:17542947
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  71. ↵
    1. Vallat R,
    2. Berry SE,
    3. Tsereteli N,
    4. Capdevila J,
    5. Khatib HA,
    6. Valdes AM,
    7. Delahanty LM,
    8. Drew DA,
    9. Chan AT,
    10. Wolf J,
    11. Franks PW,
    12. Spector TD,
    13. Walker MP
    (2022) How people wake up is associated with previous night's sleep together with physical activity and food intake. Nat Commun 13:7116. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-022-34503-2 pmid:36402781
    OpenUrlPubMed
  72. ↵
    1. Van Dongen HP,
    2. Maislin G,
    3. Mullington JM,
    4. Dinges DF
    (2003) The cumulative cost of additional wakefulness: dose-response effects on neurobehavioral functions and sleep physiology from chronic sleep restriction and total sleep deprivation. Sleep 26:117–126. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/26.2.117 pmid:12683469
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  73. ↵
    1. Van Essen DC,
    2. Smith SM,
    3. Barch DM,
    4. Behrens TE,
    5. Yacoub E,
    6. Ugurbil K
    ; WU-Minn HCP Consortium (2013) The WU-Minn Human Connectome Project: an overview. Neuroimage 80:62–79. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.041 pmid:23684880
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  74. ↵
    1. van Hees VT,
    2. Fang Z,
    3. Langford J,
    4. Assah F,
    5. Mohammad A,
    6. da Silva IC,
    7. Trenell MI,
    8. White T,
    9. Wareham NJ,
    10. Brage S
    (2014) Autocalibration of accelerometer data for free-living physical activity assessment using local gravity and temperature: an evaluation on four continents. J Appl Physiol (1985) 117:738–744. https://doi.org/10.1152/japplphysiol.00421.2014 pmid:25103964
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  75. ↵
    1. van Hees VT,
    2. Sabia S,
    3. Anderson KN,
    4. Denton SJ,
    5. Oliver J,
    6. Catt M,
    7. Abell JG,
    8. Kivimäki M,
    9. Trenell MI,
    10. Singh-Manoux A
    (2015) A novel, open access method to assess sleep duration using a wrist-worn accelerometer. PLoS One 10:e0142533. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0142533 pmid:26569414
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  76. ↵
    1. van Hees V,
    2. Fang Z,
    3. Zhao J,
    4. Heywood J,
    5. Mirkes E,
    6. Sabia S,
    7. Migueles J
    (2022) GGIR: raw accelerometer data analysis. R package version 2.8-2.
  77. ↵
    1. Vgontzas AN,
    2. Liao D,
    3. Bixler EO,
    4. Chrousos GP,
    5. Vela-Bueno A
    (2009) Insomnia with objective short sleep duration is associated with a high risk for hypertension. Sleep 32:491–497. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/32.4.491 pmid:19413143
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  78. ↵
    1. Viechtbauer W
    (2010) Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. J Stat Soft 36:1–48. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v036.i03
    OpenUrl
  79. ↵
    1. Voldsbekk I,
    2. Groote I,
    3. Zak N,
    4. Roelfs D,
    5. Geier O,
    6. Due-Tønnessen P,
    7. Løkken LL,
    8. Strømstad M,
    9. Blakstvedt TY,
    10. Kuiper YS,
    11. Elvsåshagen T,
    12. Westlye LT,
    13. Bjørnerud A,
    14. Maximov II
    (2021) Sleep and sleep deprivation differentially alter white matter microstructure: a mixed model design utilising advanced diffusion modelling. Neuroimage 226:117540. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117540 pmid:33186715
    OpenUrlPubMed
  80. ↵
    1. Walhovd KB, et al
    . (2016) Neurodevelopmental origins of lifespan changes in brain and cognition. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 113:9357–9362. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1524259113 pmid:27432992
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  81. ↵
    1. Walhovd KB,
    2. Fjell AM,
    3. Westerhausen R,
    4. Nyberg L,
    5. Ebmeier KP,
    6. Lindenberger U,
    7. Bartrés-Faz D,
    8. Baaré WFC,
    9. Siebner HR,
    10. Henson R,
    11. Drevon CA,
    12. Strømstad Knudsen GP,
    13. Ljøsne IB,
    14. Penninx BWJH,
    15. Ghisletta P,
    16. Rogeberg O,
    17. Tyler L,
    18. Bertram L
    ; Consortium Lifebrain (2018) Healthy minds 0-100 years: optimising the use of European brain imaging cohorts (“Lifebrain”). Eur Psychiatry 50:47–56. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2017.12.006 pmid:29449073
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  82. ↵
    1. Walhovd KB, et al
    . (2022) Education and income show heterogeneous relationships to lifespan brain and cognitive differences across European and US cohorts. Cereb Cortex 32:839–854.
    OpenUrl
  83. ↵
    1. Wang H, et al
    . (2019) Genome-wide association analysis of self-reported daytime sleepiness identifies 42 loci that suggest biological subtypes. Nat Commun 10:3503. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-019-11456-7 pmid:31409809
    OpenUrlPubMed
  84. ↵
    1. Watson NF,
    2. Goldberg J,
    3. Arguelles L,
    4. Buchwald D
    (2006) Genetic and environmental influences on insomnia, daytime sleepiness, and obesity in twins. Sleep 29:645–649. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/29.5.645 pmid:16774154
    OpenUrlPubMed
  85. ↵
    1. Watson NF,
    2. Badr MS,
    3. Belenky G,
    4. Bliwise DL,
    5. Buxton OM,
    6. Buysse D,
    7. Dinges DF,
    8. Gangwisch J,
    9. Grandner MA,
    10. Kushida C,
    11. Malhotra RK,
    12. Martin JL,
    13. Patel SR,
    14. Quan SF,
    15. Tasali E
    (2015) Recommended amount of sleep for a healthy adult: a joint consensus statement of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine and Sleep Research Society. Sleep 38:843–844. https://doi.org/10.5665/sleep.4716 pmid:26039963
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  86. ↵
    1. Wechsler D
    (1997) Wechsler adult intelligence scale (WAIS-III): administration and scoring manual, Ed 3. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.
  87. ↵
    1. Wechsler D
    (1999) Wchsler abbreviated scale of intelligence. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.
  88. ↵
    1. Westerlund A,
    2. Brandt L,
    3. Harlid R,
    4. Åkerstedt T,
    5. Lagerros YT
    (2014) Using the Karolinska Sleep Questionnaire to identify obstructive sleep apnea syndrome in a sleep clinic population. Clin Respir J 8:444–454. https://doi.org/10.1111/crj.12095 pmid:25396257
    OpenUrlPubMed
  89. ↵
    1. Wood SN
    (2017) Generalized additive models: an introduction with R, Ed 2. Boca Raton: Chapman and Hall/CRC.
  90. ↵
    1. Wood SN,
    2. Scheipl F
    (2020) 'gamm4': generalized additive mixed models using 'mgcv' and 'lme4'. R package version 0.2-6. Available at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=gamm4.
  91. ↵
    1. Wrzus C,
    2. Brandmaier AM,
    3. von Oertzen T,
    4. Müller V,
    5. Wagner GG,
    6. Riediger M
    (2012) A new approach for assessing sleep duration and postures from ambulatory accelerometry. PLoS One 7:e48089. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0048089 pmid:23110178
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  92. ↵
    1. Youngstedt SD,
    2. Kline CE,
    3. Zielinski MR,
    4. Kripke DF,
    5. Devlin TM,
    6. Bogan RK,
    7. Wilcox S,
    8. Hardin JW
    (2009) Tolerance of chronic 90-minute time-in-bed restriction in older long sleepers. Sleep 32:1467–1479. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/32.11.1467 pmid:19928386
    OpenUrlPubMed
  93. ↵
    1. Zamore Z,
    2. Veasey SC
    (2022) Neural consequences of chronic sleep disruption. Trends Neurosci 45:678–691. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2022.05.007
    OpenUrl
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 43 (28)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 43, Issue 28
12 Jul 2023
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Masthead (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Is Short Sleep Bad for the Brain? Brain Structure and Cognitive Function in Short Sleepers
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Is Short Sleep Bad for the Brain? Brain Structure and Cognitive Function in Short Sleepers
Anders M. Fjell, Øystein Sørensen, Yunpeng Wang, Inge K. Amlien, William F. C. Baaré, David Bartrés-Faz, Carl-Johan Boraxbekk, Andreas M. Brandmaier, Ilja Demuth, Christian A. Drevon, Klaus P. Ebmeier, Paolo Ghisletta, Rogier Kievit, Simone Kühn, Kathrine Skak Madsen, Lars Nyberg, Cristina Solé-Padullés, Didac Vidal-Piñeiro, Gerd Wagner, Leiv Otto Watne, Kristine B. Walhovd
Journal of Neuroscience 12 July 2023, 43 (28) 5241-5250; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2330-22.2023

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Is Short Sleep Bad for the Brain? Brain Structure and Cognitive Function in Short Sleepers
Anders M. Fjell, Øystein Sørensen, Yunpeng Wang, Inge K. Amlien, William F. C. Baaré, David Bartrés-Faz, Carl-Johan Boraxbekk, Andreas M. Brandmaier, Ilja Demuth, Christian A. Drevon, Klaus P. Ebmeier, Paolo Ghisletta, Rogier Kievit, Simone Kühn, Kathrine Skak Madsen, Lars Nyberg, Cristina Solé-Padullés, Didac Vidal-Piñeiro, Gerd Wagner, Leiv Otto Watne, Kristine B. Walhovd
Journal of Neuroscience 12 July 2023, 43 (28) 5241-5250; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2330-22.2023
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • brain
  • cognition
  • hippocampus
  • MRI
  • sleep
  • sleepiness

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Articles

  • Transcranial focused ultrasound modulates feedforward and feedback cortico-thalamo-cortical pathways by selectively activating excitatory neurons
  • Intracellular Spermine Is a Key Player in GSG1L’s Regulation of Calcium-Permeable AMPAR Channel Conductance and Recovery from Desensitization
  • Interindividual Variability in Memory Performance Is Related to Corticothalamic Networks during Memory Encoding and Retrieval
Show more Research Articles

Behavioral/Cognitive

  • Tracking Neural Correlates of Contextualized Meanings with Representational Similarity Analysis
  • Hemifield Specificity of Attention Response Functions during Multiple-Object Tracking
  • Interindividual Variability in Memory Performance Is Related to Corticothalamic Networks during Memory Encoding and Retrieval
Show more Behavioral/Cognitive
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Accessibility
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.