Abstract
Adult twin neuroimaging studies have revealed that cortical thickness (CT) and surface area (SA) are differentially influenced by genetic information, leading to their spatially distinct genetic patterning and topography. However, the postnatal origins of the genetic topography of CT and SA remain unclear, given the dramatic cortical development from neonates to adults. To fill this critical gap, this study unprecedentedly explored how genetic information differentially regulates the spatial topography of CT and SA in the neonatal brain by leveraging brain magnetic resonance (MR) images from 202 twin neonates with minimal influence by the complicated postnatal environmental factors. We capitalized on infant-dedicated computational tools and a data-driven spectral clustering method to parcellate the cerebral cortex into a set of distinct regions purely according to the genetic correlation of cortical vertices in terms of CT and SA, respectively, and accordingly created the first genetically informed cortical parcellation maps of neonatal brains. Both genetic parcellation maps exhibit bilaterally symmetric and hierarchical patterns, but distinct spatial layouts. For CT, regions with closer genetic relationships demonstrate an anterior-posterior (A-P) division, while for SA, regions with greater genetic proximity are typically within the same lobe. Certain genetically informed regions exhibit strong similarities between neonates and adults, with the most striking similarities in the medial surface in terms of SA, despite their overall substantial differences in genetic parcellation maps. These results greatly advance our understanding of the development of genetic influences on the spatial patterning of cortical morphology.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Genetic influences on cortical thickness (CT) and surface area (SA) are complex and could evolve throughout the lifespan. However, studies revealing distinct genetic topography of CT and SA have been limited to adults. Using brain structural magnetic resonance (MR) images of twins, we unprecedentedly discovered the distinct genetically-informed parcellation maps of CT and SA in neonatal brains, respectively. Each genetic parcellation map comprises a distinct spatial layout of cortical regions, where vertices within the same region share high genetic correlation. These genetic parcellation maps of CT and SA of neonates largely differ from those of adults, despite their highly remarkable similarities in the medial cortex of SA. These discoveries provide important insights into the genetic organization of the early cerebral cortex development.
Introduction
Cortical thickness (CT) and surface area (SA) are two essential morphologic features of the cerebral cortex and have been widely adopted in neuroimaging studies (Sowell et al., 2004; Hazlett et al., 2011; Wierenga et al., 2014; Dubois and Dehaene-Lambertz, 2015; Lyall et al., 2015; F. Wang et al., 2019b). Although both CT and SA have complex nonlinear and dynamic development, especially during early postnatal stages (Knickmeyer et al., 2008; Li et al., 2013, 2014a), each has its own distinct spatiotemporal patterns (Raznahan et al., 2011; Storsve et al., 2014; Wierenga et al., 2014; Bethlehem et al., 2022). Specifically, CT increases rapidly after birth, reaches the peak at around 14 months of age (F. Wang et al., 2019b), and then gradually decreases thereafter (Bethlehem et al., 2022). By contrast, SA expands to 69% of adult size by two years of age (Lyall et al., 2015), peaks at ∼11 years of age (Bethlehem et al., 2022), and turns out to be the principal factor that drives the growth of the cerebral cortex after CT reaching its peak (Wierenga et al., 2014). The early development of CT and SA has critical influences on behavioral and cognitive abilities as well as neurodevelopmental and neuropsychiatric disorders (Stiles and Jernigan, 2010; Brown and Jernigan, 2012; Ecker et al., 2014; Gerrits et al., 2016; Haring et al., 2016; Li et al., 2016; Vijayakumar et al., 2016). Discovering the underlying mechanisms that drive the distinct early development of CT and SA is critical for better understanding of healthy and disordered brains.
Intrinsic genetic mechanisms are widely perceived as one of the major determinants of the patterns of CT and SA (Panizzon et al., 2009; Winkler et al., 2010; Eyler et al., 2012; Jha et al., 2018; Strike et al., 2019). Previous studies have reported that the distinct genetic influences on CT and SA change from neonates to adults and exhibit regionally heterogeneous patterns (Panizzon et al., 2009; Winkler et al., 2010; Eyler et al., 2012; Schmitt et al., 2014; Jha et al., 2018; Strike et al., 2019; Teeuw et al., 2019; K. Xia et al., 2022). For example, the genetic influence on the total SA, whether in neonatal stage or in adulthood, is strong and significant. In contrast, the genetic influence on the average CT is weak and insignificant during the neonatal stage, although it increases during adulthood (Panizzon et al., 2009; Jha et al., 2018). Previous studies also revealed that the heritability estimates of CT (neonates: 0.01–0.52, adults: 0.06–0.73) and SA (neonates: 0.01–0.76, adults: 0.17–0.76) vary across cortical regions and age groups (Winkler et al., 2010; Jha et al., 2018). With reference to the above, genetic-based region-specific patterns of CT and SA may provide important information to many brain disorders. Nowadays, a series of studies on how genetic factors influence the formation of region-specific patterns of CT and SA have been performed using structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) of adult twin brains (Rubenstein et al., 1999; Panizzon et al., 2009; Chen et al., 2011, 2013; Schlaggar, 2011). The distinct genetic-based cortical parcellation maps of CT and SA in adults and their different organizing principles thus have been unveiled (Chen et al., 2011, 2013), providing us valuable references for understanding cortical regional specialization and potential genetic vulnerabilities associated with brain disorders. However, it remains unknown how genetic factors regulate the initial spatial patterns of CT and SA after birth, and whether they are preserved through postnatal development or evolve with age.
To fill this knowledge gap, we aimed to discover the initial genetically-regulated spatial patterns of CT and SA by taking advantage of neonatal twins. The motivation is that CT and SA in the neonatal brains are mainly genetically determined and minimally affected by the complicated postnatal environmental factors and thus are ideal candidates for discovering the postnatal origins of genetically influenced patterns. Furthermore, existing studies have demonstrated the genetic hierarchy of cortical arealization in animals (O'Leary et al., 2007) and the hierarchical transcriptional architecture of the human brain regions (Kang et al., 2011). Consistent with these findings, researchers also unveiled the hierarchical cortical genetic parcellation maps of adult CT and SA (Chen et al., 2013). We thus reasonably speculate that the cortical genetic parcellation maps of neonatal CT and SA should also be hierarchical.
To this end, brain structural MRI scans from 202 same-sex neonatal twins from the University of North Carolina (UNC) Early Brain Development Study, including monozygotic (MZ) twins and dizygotic (DZ) twins, were used in this study. The classical twin analysis model (Maes, 2005; Neale and Cardon, 2013), also termed as ACE model, was adopted to estimate the genetic and environmental influences on the variances of CT and SA, and to compute the genetic correlations between different vertices on the cortical surface. Accordingly, we leveraged the data-driven spectral clustering (Ng et al., 2001; Von Luxburg, 2007) method to generate genetic parcellation maps of CT and SA in neonatal brains. Since vertices within the same region are maximally genetically correlated, the resulting parcellation maps well represent the genetically regulated topography of CT and SA in neonates. Then, we calculated the pair-wise genetic similarity matrices among regions of discovered genetic parcellations of CT and SA, respectively. Based on these two genetic similarity matrices, we adopted the dendrogram to further explore the hierarchical genetic organization among regions of genetic parcellations of CT and SA, respectively.
Materials and Methods
Participants
The Institutional Review Boards of the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill and Duke University Medical Center (DUMC) approved this study. Pregnant mothers, who have provided written informed consent for this research, were recruited during their second trimesters of pregnancy from prenatal diagnostic clinics at UNC Hospitals and DUMC. Subjects with abnormal fetal ultrasounds or mothers with major medical diseases or psychiatric illnesses were excluded from this study. Subjects included in this study had no congenital anomaly, significant medical illness, or MRI abnormality (Gilmore et al., 2012). Structural MR images from 202 neonates, including 112 DZ twins and 90 MZ twins with same sex, were adopted in this study. The detailed demographic information is shown in Table 1.
MRI acquisition
All T1-weighted and T2-weighted images were collected on a Siemens head-only 3T scanner. Acquired with a 3D magnetization-prepared rapid gradient echo (MPRAGE) sequence, the acquisition parameters of T1-weighted images (160 sagittal slices) are: TR/TE = 1,820/4.38 ms, flip angle = 7°, inversion time = 1,100 ms, and resolution = 1 × 1 × 1 mm3. T2-weighted images (70 transverse slices) were acquired with the turbo spin-echo (TSE) sequences and acquisition parameters are: TR/TE = 7,380/119 ms, flip angle = 150° and resolution =1.25 × 1.25 × 1.95 mm3.
Image processing and cortical surface reconstruction
All T1-weighted and T2-weighted MR images were processed using an infant-dedicated computational pipeline (http://www.ibeat.cloud/) described previously (Li et al., 2014a, 2015, 2019; L. Wang et al., 2018, 2023). The processing procedure contains the following main steps: (1) correction of intensity inhomogeneity of both T1-weighted and T2-weighted MR images using the N3 method (Sled et al., 1998); (2) linear alignment of each T2-weighted image onto its corresponding T1-weighted image using FLIRT (Smith et al., 2004); (3) removal of brain skull, brainstem, and cerebellum using a deep learning-based method (Zhang et al., 2019); (4) segmentation of brain tissues into white matter, gray matter and cerebrospinal fluid through a deep learning-based segmentation method (L. Wang et al., 2018); (5) separation of each brain into left and right hemispheres with noncortical regions filled with white matter.
After that, cortical surfaces of each MRI scan were reconstructed by the following steps: (1) correction of topological defects of white matter using a learning-based topological correction method (Sun et al., 2019); (2) reconstruction of the inner cortical surface of each hemisphere by tessellating the corrected white matter as a triangular mesh; (3) reconstruction of the outer cortical surface of each hemisphere by deforming the inner cortical surface outwards while avoiding mesh self-intersection (Li et al., 2012, 2014a); and generation of the middle cortical surface as the geometric average of the inner and outer cortical surfaces; (4) mapping the inner cortical surface onto a standard sphere while minimizing the geometric distortion (Fischl et al., 1999). To enable the vertex-wise intersubject comparison and analysis of CT and SA, we further registered each mapped spherical surface onto the age-matched template in the UNC 4D Neonatal and Infant Cortical Surface Atlases (Wu et al., 2019) using Spherical Demons (Yeo et al., 2009) and accordingly resampled each surface to a 163,842 standard mesh tessellation to build the vertex-to-vertex cortical correspondences across subjects. For each vertex, CT was computed as the minimum distance between the inner and outer surfaces, while SA was computed as one-third the sum of areas of all triangles associated with that vertex on the resampled middle cortical surface. The 163,842 vertex-wise CT and SA maps were then smoothed using iterative nearest neighbor averaging with 2560 iterations as previously described (Chen et al., 2013; F. Wang et al., 2019b). To reduce the computational cost of discovering the genetic regions, we further resampled CT and SA maps to have 2562 vertices on each hemisphere.
Twin analysis
The common model in twin analysis is the standard ACE model, which assumes that the variation in a phenotype is caused by additive genetic effects (A), common environmental effects (C), and individual-specific environmental effects (E; Maes, 2005; Neale and Cardon, 2013). In the univariate ACE model, the genotypes of MZ twins are identical and thus share 100% of their genes, while DZ twins share 50% of their genes. The common environmental effects among members of a twin pair are equal, despite their zygosity. As for the individual-specific environmental effects, they are independent across members of any twin pairs.
The bivariate ACE model is an extension of the univariate ACE model (Maes, 2005; Neale and Cardon, 2013). It can not only decompose the phenotypic variance into genetic and environmental variances, but also decompose the covariance between phenotypes into genetic and environmental covariances. Therefore, the genetic and environmental correlations between phenotypes can be estimated according to the genetic and environmental covariances. Of note, genetic correlations reflect the degree to which two phenotypes share genetic variance based on twin data. Mathematically, the genetic correlation between two phenotypes is calculated as the genetic covariance of two phenotypes divided by the square root of the product of their separate genetic variances (Neale and Cardon, 2013).
Since we aimed to, respectively, obtain the genetic correlations of CT and SA between any two vertices on the cortical surface, we adopted the standard bivariate ACE model, as in previous studies of genetic influence on CT (Panizzon et al., 2009; Rimol et al., 2010; Chen et al., 2013).
To avoid additional confounding sex effects within twins, we focused on twin pairs with the same sex. Thus, the adopted 202 subjects in this study are all same-sex twins. Before model fitting, the mean CT and total SA of the whole cerebral cortex were, respectively, regressed out from the CT and SA at each vertex through the linear regression as in (Chen et al., 2012, 2013; Jha et al., 2018).
Discovering genetic parcellations with spectral clustering
To discover the genetic parcellations of CT and SA in neonates, after obtaining their genetic correlation matrices, we leveraged the spectral clustering method (Ng et al., 2002; Von Luxburg, 2007) to group cortical vertices into a set of distinct regions. The main motivation of using spectral clustering is that it is flexible to discover clusters with arbitrary shapes according to the realistic distribution of data similarity (Meila, 2016). Specifically, spectral clustering transforms the data similarity matrix into eigenspace and represents the similarity matrix with top k decomposed eigenvectors, thus can better capture the distributions of the data similarity for identifying clusters. Given a similarity matrix
In this study,
To determine an appropriate region number of genetic parcellation, we applied the widely used silhouette coefficients (Rousseeuw, 1987) adopted in many previous studies (Chen et al., 2012, 2013; F. Wang et al., 2019a,b; J. Xia et al., 2019). The silhouette coefficients of the vertex i denoted as
Since the previous adult twin studies (Chen et al., 2013) determined 12 as the appropriate region number under genetic influence, we set the region number k ranging from 2 to 15 in this study for comparison. Then the average silhouette coefficient was used to determine the most appropriate region number to further investigate the genetic parcellation maps in neonates. Finally, we explored the genetic relatedness between discovered distinct regions and constructed a dendrogram accordingly.
Results
Genetic parcellation maps of neonatal CT and SA
We first identified the genetic parcellation maps of CT and SA on both left and right hemispheres (Figs. 1, 2), with region numbers k increasing from 2 to 15, respectively. As can be seen, the discovered genetic parcellation maps of both CT and SA appear to be bilaterally symmetric across most regions, although we did not impose any constraint for hemispheric symmetry. Moreover, with the region number increasing, newly emerged regions largely follow the previously formed region boundaries and yield the meaningful hierarchical genetic parcellations of CT and SA. Meanwhile, the discovered genetic parcellation maps of CT differ remarkably from SA.
As for CT (Fig. 1), when the region number k equals to 2, the unveiled two-region basic genetic parcellation exhibits an anterior-posterior (A-P) division, which separates the frontal and insular cortices from other cortices. When increasing the region number to 4, the medial parietal-occipital boundary, indicated by yellow arrows, emerges and is well preserved till the 15-region partition. Besides, the boundary of the lateral occipital cortex, indicated by black arrows, appears when region number is three and exists until the region number reaches 15.
Regarding SA (Fig. 2), the identified two-region basic genetic parcellation roughly demonstrates an A-P division on the lateral surface, but a lateral-medial division on the medial surface. The lateral occipital boundary indicated by magenta arrows consistently exists with region numbers from 5 to 15. Moreover, the medial occipital boundary indicated by green arrows also appears when the region number is 5 and is well preserved until the region reaches 15. Meanwhile, the boundary, which separates the frontal and nonfrontal parts and is indicated by blue arrows, is consistently well-preserved from 2 to 15 regions.
Then we estimated the average silhouette coefficients of genetic parcellation maps with different region numbers to help discover the appropriate region number. Accordingly, the appropriate region number was determined based on two criteria: (1) having a relatively high value of silhouette coefficient, which indicates better quality of a parcellation; (2) having relatively stable silhouette coefficients across the neighboring region numbers. The silhouette coefficient of CT (Fig. 3) increases from
The parcellation maps with 12 regions of CT and SA in neonates (Figs. 4A, 5A) approximately correspond to the structurally or functionally meaningful regions. With approximate names shown below the parcellation map, discovered genetic regions of CT approximately correspond to: (1) superior parietal cortex, (2) precuneus and cingulate, (3) medial occipital cortex, (4) anteromedial temporal cortex, (5) perisylvian, inferior parietal and lateral temporal cortex, (6) middle precentral and postcentral, (7) lateral occipital cortex, (8) superior precentral and postcentral, (9) superior frontal cortex, (10) anterior frontal cortex, (11) anterior insula, and (12) dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. The genetic parcellation map of CT in neonates (Fig. 4A) is largely different from that in adults (Fig. 4B), except for a few regions in the posterior and medial surfaces. For example, region 1 (superior parietal cortex), region 3 (medial occipital cortex), region 4 (anteromedial temporal cortex), region 7 (lateral occipital cortex), and region 10 (anterior frontal cortex) in neonates are roughly consistent with region 2, region 6, region 9, region 5, and region 7 of adults, respectively.
The discovered genetic regions of SA in neonates (Fig. 5A) approximately correspond to: (1) superior parietal cortex, (2) primary somatosensory, (3) inferior parietal and posterior perisylvian, (4) precuneus, (5) dorsomedial frontal cortex, (6) anteromedial temporal cortex, (7) middle temporal cortex, (8) lateral temporal-occipital junction, (9) medial occipital cortex, (10) medial orbitofrontal and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, (11) anterior insula and ventral orbitofrontal, and (12) premotor and primary motor. Strikingly, genetic parcellation maps of SA in neonates (Fig. 5A) and adults (Fig. 5B) seem to resemble each other in many regions, especially for regions in the medial surface. For example, region 1 (superior parietal cortex), region 12 (premotor and primary motor), region 4 (precuneus), region 5 (dorsomedial frontal cortex), region 6 (anteromedial temporal cortex), and region 9 (medial occipital cortex) in neonates, respectively, correspond well to region 10, region 1, region 11, region 3, region 8, and region 12 in adults (Fig. 5B).
Hierarchical organization of genetic regions
After discovering the above genetically distinct regions, we tried to reveal the hierarchical organization between these regions. First, to visualize the genetic proximity between these regions, we built their pair-wise genetic similarity matrix, with each row and column corresponding to the sorted region number. Specifically, based on the genetic correlation matrix obtained from twin analysis, each cell in the pair-wise genetic similarity matrix is the average of genetic correlations within and between regions as described previously (Chen et al., 2011, 2013). Then, based on the pair-wise genetic similarity matrix of CT or SA, we performed the hierarchical clustering method to group the genetic regions into a multilevel cluster tree or dendrogram to represent the genetic organization among regions. Since the dendrogram shows the hierarchical relationship between regions, it can help us to make a direct comparison of the hierarchical organizations of CT and SA. The color bars of heatmaps represent the mean genetic correlations within and between regions of CT (SA), where higher values indicate closer genetic relatedness.
As can be observed from the dendrograms (Fig. 6A,B), which are, respectively, derived from the pair-wise genetic similarity matrix of CT and SA, the hierarchical relationship between genetic regions of CT differs remarkably from that of SA. Specifically, genetic correlations of SA (Fig. 6B) between regions within the same lobe are higher than those from different lobes, except for the precuneus cortex (region 4), dorsolateral frontal cortex (region 5), and medial occipital cortex (region 9). Regions that are genetically more similar in SA can be roughly grouped as: (1) parietal lobe, including the superior parietal cortex, primary somatosensory cortex, inferior parietal and posterior perisylvian, as well as precuneus cortex (regions 1–4); (2) temporal lobe, including the anteromedial temporal cortex, middle temporal cortex and the lateral temporal-occipital junction (regions 6–8); (3) frontal lobe, including the medial orbitofrontal and dorsolateral prefrontal, anterior insula and ventral orbitofrontal, and premotor and primary motor cortex (regions 10–12).
The genetic organization of CT (Fig. 6A) among regions differs remarkably from that of SA (Fig. 6B). The organization among genetic regions of CT (Fig. 6A) can be roughly divided into frontal and nonfrontal parts, which can also be considered as an A-P division. Specifically, the frontal parts approximately contain the superior precentral and postcentral (region 8), superior frontal cortex (region 9), anterior frontal cortex (region 10), anterior insula (region 11), and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (region 12). While the nonfrontal parts cover the superior parietal cortex (region 1), precuneus and cingulate (region 2), medial occipital cortex (region 3), anteromedial temporal cortex (region 4), perisylvian, inferior parietal and lateral temporal cortex (region 5), middle precentral and postcentral (region 6), and the lateral occipital cortex (region 7). Meanwhile, in the nonfrontal parts, regions with strong genetic correlation are typically spatially adjacent, but come from different lobes, e.g., regions 1 and 2, regions 3 and 4, and regions 5 and 6.
Discussion
To our knowledge, this is the first study to comprehensively explore the early postnatal genetic topography of CT and SA using neonatal twins. We found that the genetic parcellations of both CT and SA are bilaterally symmetric, and the discovered regions largely correspond to existing structural or functional parcels (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002; Desikan et al., 2006; Gordon et al., 2016). Of note, we performed the spectral clustering purely based on the genetic correlation matrix without any restrictions for hemispheric symmetry or anatomic knowledge, thus the naturally-formed bilaterally symmetric regions may reflect the intrinsic constraint of genetic regulation on cortical regionalization (formation of structurally and functionally distinct cortical regions) of both hemispheres, consistent with the observation in previous adult research (Wilkinson et al., 1989; Fraser et al., 1990; Puelles and Rubenstein, 2003; Tomancak et al., 2007).
We found that when the region number is 2, the basic genetic parcellations of CT and SA are similar to some extent on the lateral surface, as both roughly exhibit an anterior-posterior (A-P) division. As initial patterns of CT and SA are mainly determined by genetic factors before birth, these findings suggest that genetic influence in forming the initial patterns of CT and SA may be similar on the lateral surface on a coarse scale. Such results are generally consistent with previous studies that significant genetic overlap exists between CT and SA in infants (Jha et al., 2018). Studies in rodent models show that the initiation of cortical regionalization is predominantly driven by intrinsic genetic mechanisms involving morphogen gradients along the A-P, dorsal-ventral (D-V), and medial-lateral (M-L) axes of the developing cortex (O'Leary et al., 2007; Sansom and Livesey, 2009). These morphogens, which play critical roles in neurogenesis and progenitor cell proliferation (Tiberi et al., 2012), may serve as the common factors affecting the development of both CT and SA. Unlike in neonates, very little genetic correlation is found between CT and SA in adults (Chen et al., 2013). Moreover, in adults, the basic genetic regions of SA follow an A-P division, while the basic genetic regions of CT follow a D-V division (Chen et al., 2013). A possible explanation is that CT and SA may share similar genetic influences in neonates on a coarse scale, which subsequently diverge over time, leading to differences in basic genetic patterns between neonates and adults (K. Xia et al., 2022).
When the region number is 12, the genetic parcellations of CT and SA demonstrate significantly distinct patterns. As in the radial unit hypothesis of cerebral cortex development (Rakic, 1988, 2000), SA is determined by the number of radial columns, whereas CT is under the influence of the number of cells within a column. The two different processes may be manipulated by their unique regulatory genes and thus leading to distinct genetic patterns between CT and SA. In fact, a large-scale human study reveals that CT and SA are associated with largely distinct common genetic variants (Grasby et al., 2020). For example, SA is associated with common genetic variants affecting the Wnt signaling pathway (Nusse and Varmus, 1992; Nusse, 2005), which regulates progenitor cell expansion. On the contrary, CT is associated with common genetic variants affecting genes functioning in neural differentiation, migration, and myelination.
Moreover, since the patterns of CT and SA in neonates are mainly genetically determined and inevitably suffer from complicated postnatal environmental influence during development, the similar patterns of SA and different patterns of CT between neonates and adults may suggest that SA is under strong genetic influence, while CT has weaker genetic influences and is more likely influenced by environmental factors (Jha et al., 2018, 2019). Previous studies have revealed that SA is mainly determined by the shape of cortical folding (Mota and Herculano-Houzel, 2015; Garcia et al., 2018), which features the greatest development during the third trimester of pregnancy and has been well formed at term birth, especially for the primary and secondary cortical folds (Armstrong et al., 1995; White et al., 2010; Li et al., 2014b). Although SA undergoes more growth than CT from birth to adulthood, the SA expansion of the medial surface is less than the lateral surface (Li et al., 2013). Therefore, SA may intrinsically suffer from less postnatal environmental influences, especially for the medial surface, and keep relatively stable genetic patterns across the lifespan. Furthermore, though the peaking time of the average CT is around 14 months of age (F. Wang et al., 2019b), rather earlier than the total SA peaking at around 11 years of age (Bethlehem et al., 2022), CT still shows age-related changes across the lifespan and tends to thin after reaching its peak (Wierenga et al., 2014; Bethlehem et al., 2022). This processing is driven by age-related biological procedures, e.g., synaptic pruning (Stiles and Jernigan, 2010; Lyall et al., 2015) and myelination (Natu et al., 2019), which are reported to be influenced by environmental factors (Tooley et al., 2021). Moreover, compared with SA, CT is more sensitive (Hutton et al., 2009; Winkler et al., 2012) and vulnerable to postnatal environmental influences, likely leading to different genetic topography between neonates and adults.
These findings are also consistent with previous studies of genetic influences on CT and SA at different developmental stages. For example, in a neonatal twin study, genetic influences on the total SA (0.78) are significant and stronger than that on the average CT (0.29; Jha et al., 2018). Meanwhile, with twins between zero and two years of age, researchers found relatively stronger genetic influences on SA (0.59 in neonates, 0.74 in one-year olds, and 0.73 in two-year olds) than on CT (0.48 in neonates, 0.37 in one-year olds, and 0.44 in two-year olds) across all three examined age groups (K. Xia et al., 2022). Additionally, studies with participants between three and 20 years of age indicated that shared environmental influences (socioeconomic status) are larger on changes of CT than on changes of SA (Piccolo et al., 2016).
We also found that the hierarchical organization of CT among the 12 regions is different from that of SA. Roughly organized according to four cortical lobes, the discovered genetic regions of SA within the same lobe are genetically more correlated than regions across lobes, consistent with the organization principle of SA in adults (Chen et al., 2013). This suggests relatively little postnatal environmental influences on the genetic organization principle of SA. Nevertheless, the organization of CT in neonates does not always follow this principle. Specifically, for CT in neonates, clusters with closer genetic correlation roughly correspond to an A-P division, which separates the cerebral cortex into frontal and nonfrontal parts. Moreover, in the nonfrontal parts, regions with strong genetic correlations are typically from different lobes. According to the previous study on the developmental topography of CT during infancy (F. Wang et al., 2019b), this potentially reflects the differential maturation patterns of CT. For example, most regions in nonfrontal cortices reach their peaks of CT during the second postnatal year, while many regions in the frontal cortex show a monotonical increase of CT during the first two postnatal years, e.g., regions 8, 9, and 12 in Figure 4A. Previous adult studies also suggested that regions with close genetic similarity in CT tend to have similar maturational timing (Chen et al., 2013). Taken together, these findings suggest CT and SA have distinct organizational principles for genetic similarity among discovered regions since birth, which differentially evolve from neonates to adults.
It is also worth noting that the A-P division of the genetic organization of CT in neonates in our study aligns with prior studies investigating the evolution of the cerebral cortex. For example, previous studies discovered that both genetic correlation and structural covariance of CT in adults exhibited an A-P pattern (Valk et al., 2020). A comparable organization of structural covariance of CT in macaques is also present, potentially indicating that the A-P division is phylogenetically conserved during evolution. Further studies of the genetic organization of CT in macaques are needed to confirm this.
Although the revealed genetic parcellations of CT and SA in neonates are neurobiologically meaningful, some limitations in our study should be noted. First, our study only contains neonatal twins, thus further studies with twin data from different developing age groups is needed to fill the gap of genetic parcellations of CT and SA between neonates and adults. Second, genetic factors affect human cortical structures, which can be characterized not only by CT and SA, but also by other cortical features, e.g., cortical folding, gyrification, and functional connectivity. Future studies might be performed on how genetic factors influence other cortical features to complement the research on genetically influenced cortical parcellations during early brain development. Third, since many neurodevelopmental disorders are rooted during infancy, future research should be performed to explore whether genetic parcellations can help identify early biomarkers for early interventions.
In summary, this study has three contributions. First, leveraging the neonatal twins, for the first time, we revealed the postnatal origins of the genetic topography of CT and SA. The genetically informed parcellation maps of CT and SA are distinct, but both tend to be hierarchically organized and bilaterally symmetric and correspond well to existing structurally or functionally meaningful regions. Second, by comparing our revealed genetic parcellations in neonates with those in adults, we found their larger differences in most regions, but also striking similarities in many regions in terms of surface area, especially on the medial surface. Third, we showed that CT and SA have distinct organizational principles of genetic similarity among regions in the generated parcellation maps since birth, which differentially evolve from neonates to adults. These results help us better understand how genetic factors shape the initial postnatal organizational patterns of CT and SA and provide important references for future studies of the genetic contribution to the development of CT and SA. Our generated genetically informed cortical parcellation maps of CT and SA will be publicly available on our website.
Footnotes
This work was partially supported by National Institutes of Health Grants MH116225, MH117943, MH123202, MH116527, MH070890, and HD053000.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Gang Li at gang_li{at}med.unc.edu