Abstract
The identifiable target effect refers to the preference for helping identified victims and punishing identifiable perpetrators compared with equivalent but unidentifiable counterparts. The identifiable target effect is often attributed to the heightened moral emotions evoked by identified targets. However, the specific neurocognitive processes that mediate and/or modulate this effect remain largely unknown. Here, we combined a third-party punishment game with brain imaging and computational modeling to unravel the neurocomputational underpinnings of the identifiable transgressor effect. Human participants (males and females) acted as bystanders and punished identified or anonymous wrongdoers. Participants were more punitive toward identified wrongdoers than anonymous wrongdoers because they took a vicarious perspective of victims and adopted lower reference points of inequity (i.e., more stringent norms) in the identified context than in the unidentified context. Accordingly, there were larger activity of the ventral anterior insula, more distinct multivariate neural patterns in the dorsal anterior insula and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, and lower strength between ventral anterior insula and dorsolateral PFC and between dorsal anterior insula and ventral striatum connectivity in response to identified transgressors than anonymous transgressors. These findings implicate the interplay of expectancy violations, emotions, and self-interest in the identifiability effect. Last, individual differences in the identifiability effect were associated with empathic concern/social dominance orientation, activity in the precuneus/cuneus and temporo-parietal junction, and intrinsic functional connectivity of the dorsolateral PFC. Together, our work is the first to uncover the neurocomputational processes mediating identifiable transgressor effect and to characterize psychophysiological profiles modulating the effect.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT The identifiable target effect, more help to identified victims or stronger punishment to identifiable perpetrators, is common in daily life. We examined the neurocomputational mechanisms mediating/modulating the identifiability effect on third-party punishment by bridging literature from economics and cognitive neuroscience. Our findings reveal that identifiable transgressor effect is mediated by lower reference points of inequity (i.e., more stringent norms), which might be associated with a stronger involvement of the emotion processes and a weaker engagement of the analytic/deliberate processes. Furthermore, personality traits, altered brain activity, and intrinsic functional connectivity contribute to the individual variance in the identifiability effect. Overall, our study advances the understanding of the identifiability effect by shedding light on its component processes and modulating factors.
- computational modeling
- emotions
- expectancy violations
- fMRI
- identifiable transgressor effect
- reference points
Introduction
Prosocial behaviors are influenced by the identifiability of the target, with individuals more inclined to help specific, identified victims and punish identifiable wrongdoers compared with unidentifiable counterparts (Jenni and Loewenstein, 1997; Small and Loewenstein, 2003). The identifiability effect on human altruism has been attributed to higher emotional responses to identifiable targets (Slovic, 2010; Genevsky et al., 2013). However, altruistic behaviors recruit multiple psychological subcomponents, while it is unclear how identifiability interacts with those subcomponents to impact overt behaviors. Additionally, previous studies have primarily focused on altruistic helping and have overlooked the theoretical and practical significance of how identifiability impacts altruistic punishment (Small and Loewenstein, 2005). Moreover, individual differences in identifiability effects have yet to be characterized. To address these gaps, our study combined computational modeling and brain imaging techniques with a third-party punishment (TPP) game to unravel neurocognitive processes that mediate and modulate the effects of identifiability.
Using computational models, we aimed to uncover specific cognitive processes underlying the effect of identifiability on TPP. First, it has been revealed that third-party bystanders engage in punishment to reduce inequity between transgressors and victims, without directly comparing themselves to either party (Zhong et al., 2016). This implies that bystanders adopt an impartial perspective rather than an egocentric perspective. An alternative hypothesis posits that bystanders adopt a vicarious perspective, focusing only on the disadvantage of victims and disregarding that of transgressors (FeldmanHall et al., 2014). Second, the effect of identifiability may manifest through its impact on inequity sensitivity or reference points. Social contexts can influence costly punishment by altering the degree of inequity aversion, known as inequity sensitivity (Wright et al., 2011). Alternatively, individuals form context-specific beliefs about what is expected, which then serve as behavioral reference points. Social contexts inducing higher reference points of inequity lead to less strict equity norms, resulting in reduced costly punishment (Sanfey, 2009; Chang and Sanfey, 2013; Sanfey et al., 2014). In short, our computational models quantitatively tested competing hypotheses on third-party's perspectives (egocentric, impartial, or vicarious) and potential ways (inequity sensitivity vs reference points) through which identifiability influenced TPP decisions.
Our study also aimed to uncover neural pathways related to the impact of identifiability on TPP. Previous studies have identified two interacting systems underlying costly punishment. First, deviations from social norms are detected as prediction errors, potentially encoded in the dorsal anterior insula (dAI) and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) (Chang and Sanfey, 2013; Xiang et al., 2013). These prediction error signals may drive aversive feelings toward norm violations encoded in the ventral AI (vAI) (Bellucci et al., 2018). These processes might drive a cognitive heuristic or a motivation to punish transgressors (Civai, 2013; Feng et al., 2015; Bellucci et al., 2018). Second, costly punishment involves cognitive control processes mediated by the dorsolateral PFC (dlPFC), which may regulate emotional responses in favor of self-interest (Sanfey et al., 2003; Harlé and Sanfey, 2012). Importantly, activity of these systems to inequity is context-sensitive (Wright et al., 2011; Chang and Sanfey, 2013), providing a plausible neural mechanism mediating the effect of identifiability. Specifically, higher inequity sensitivity or lower reference points induced by identified (vs unidentified) context might be associated with higher activity in the vAI, dAI, and dACC as well as lower activity in the dlPFC and/or stronger connectivity between the dlPFC and the AI/dACC.
Last, the importance of characterizing individual differences is increasingly recognized in cognitive neuroscience (Elliott et al., 2021). Thus, we examined the associations between identifiability effects and intrinsic features previously implicated in altruistic behaviors, including empathic concern and social dominance orientation (Pratto et al., 1994; FeldmanHall et al., 2015), as well as intrinsic functional connectivity features of the dlPFC (Nash and Knoch, 2016). We also investigated the correlations between identifiability effects and evoked brain responses (Wright et al., 2011). This approach would characterize psychophysiological profiles modulating identifiability effects and provide external validation of model parameters.
Materials and Methods
Participants
Twenty-seven students (15 females) (mean age ± SD = 22.89 ± 2.31 years) participated in the study for monetary compensation. The sample size was based on previous fMRI studies on the similar topic (Chang and Sanfey, 2013; Genevsky et al., 2013; Feng et al., 2016) and resource constraints. All participants were right-handed, had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, and had no neurologic or psychiatric history. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants. The study was conducted according to the ethical guidelines and principles of the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the local ethical committee.
Experimental procedure and task
There were three sessions for this study (Fig. 1A). Participants were first invited to the laboratory for a screening session a week before the fMRI scanning and completed psychological surveys/questionnaires, including the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (Davis, 1980) and social dominance orientation (SDO) scale (Sidanius and Pratto, 2001). Interpersonal Reactivity Index is a self-administered questionnaire consisting of 28 items, which are scored in a 5 point scale from “does not describe me well” to “describes me very well.” Interpersonal Reactivity Index consists of four subscales with 7 items per subscale: empathic concern, perspective taking, fantasy, and personal distressing. Higher scores on a subscale indicate greater levels of corresponding empathic ability. In the current study, we focused on empathic concern, a general tendency to experience feelings of others, as it has been closely associated with context-sensitive altruistic behaviors (FeldmanHall et al., 2015; Vekaria et al., 2017). SDO scale is a widely used self-report questionnaire that measures an individual's preference for inequity among social groups. SDO includes 16 items and is scored on a 7 point scale from “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree.” The negative associations between SDO and empathy/altruistic behaviors have been consistently established (Pratto et al., 1994; Weiß et al., 2020).
Second, participants returned the following week for the fMRI scanning session to play a TPP game (Fig. 1B). As third-party decision-makers (Player C), participants observed how a sum of money (12 money units [MUs]) was allocated between several pairs of other players (A and B). In particular, participants were told that these people (Players A and B) were participating in a previous study, in which they jointly earned a bonus (12 MUs) by completing another task. One person from each pair was randomly chosen as Player A (dictator) and was asked to allocate the jointly earned money, whereas the other Player B (recipient) had to accept Player A's allocation (Kahneman et al., 1986). On each trial, participants were given 6 MUs and had a chance to reduce A's payoff as a punishment by altruistically spending their own money: each MU spent reduced 2 MUs from Player A's payoff (Fehr and Fischbacher, 2004; Bernhard et al., 2006). Notably, terms such as “fairness,” “punish,” or “sanction” were avoided in the instructions, and participants were told that they had the chance to assign “deduction points” to the proposers (Fehr and Fischbacher, 2004).
Participants made their decisions under both identified and unidentified contexts (Fig. 1C,D). In the identified context, the decisions of Player A were presented along with his/her photograph, and participants were told that the person shown in the photograph made the offer (Genevsky et al., 2013). That is, a specific dictator making the offer was chosen and presented to the participant. In the unidentified context, decisions of Player A were presented along with a silhouette. Participants were instructed that Player A in this context has not been determined yet, but one of Players A would be randomly chosen from a sample of Players A having made the offer. Accordingly, the dictator responsible for the allocation was unspecific in this context. Notably, information about victims (i.e., Player B) was not presented in either identified or unidentified context.
On each trial of the TPP game, information (face or silhouette) and allocation of Player A were presented constantly for 4 s (Fig. 1C,D). Afterward, a jitter was presented (1-7 s) and followed by the response phase (3 s). During this phase, participants needed to decide how many MUs to spend to decrease Player A's payoff using 1 of the 4 possible choices (a hollow circle was under each choice): 0, 2, 4, or 6 MUs. Decisions were made through a response box, with associations between buttons and decisions being counterbalanced across participants. Once participants' response was registered, feedback indicating their decision with a filled circle under the corresponding choice was displayed onscreen until the end of the decision period. Each trial ended with a second jitter (1-7 s). Stimulus presentation and behavioral data collection were implemented by using Psychtoolbox-3 (http://psychtoolbox.org/).
Before fMRI scanning, the experimental paradigm was explained to the participants, and they played 4 trials (1 trial of 6:6 splits and 1 trial of 12:0 splits for both identified and unidentified contexts) of the game to get familiar with the task. Afterward, participants completed two 9 min runs of the TPP game (270 scans every 2 s). Each run consisted of 40 trials: 5 trials of 6:6 splits, 5 trials of 7:5, 3 trials of 10:2 splits, 3 trials of 11:1, and 4 trials of 12:0 splits for both identified and unidentified contexts. All conditions were randomly presented on a trial-by-trial basis.
Finally, in the postscan session, participants completed a survey. Participants were asked to rate the same splits (fair: 6:6, 7:5; unfair: 10:2, 11:1, 12:0) observed under both contexts (identified, unidentified) during the experiment on the following 7 point Likert scales: “How much responsibility did you feel to reduce A's money?” (Responsibility: 1 = not at all, 9 = absolutely), “To what extent did you feel that A's allocations were fair?” (Fairness: 1 = absolutely unfair, 9 = absolutely fair), “How excited did you feel” (Emotional arousal ratings: 1 = very calm, 9 = very excited), and “How pleased did you feel” (Emotional valence ratings: 1 = very unpleasant, 9 = very pleasant). Self-reported data from 1 participant were not collected because of errors in the arrangement process.
Data acquisition
Imaging was performed on a 3 T Siemens Trio scanner equipped with a 12 channel head coil at Imaging Center for Brain Research in Beijing Normal University. A T2-weighted gradient-EPI sequence was used to acquire functional images: TR/TE = 2000 ms/30 ms, flip angle = 90°, number of axial slices = 33, slices thickness = 3.5 mm, gap between slices = 0.7 mm, matrix size = 64 × 64, and FOV = 224 mm × 224 mm. Before the fMRI scanning of the TPP game, participants completed a 5 min resting-state fMRI scanning. During resting-state fMRI scanning, participants were instructed to close their eyes, keep still, remain awake, and not think about anything systematically. The resting state scanning consisted of 150 contiguous EPI volumes. The fMRI scanning for the TPP game consisted of 540 EPI volumes in total (2 runs, 270 EPI volumes per run). High-resolution anatomic images covering the entire brain were obtained by applying MPRAGE sequence: TR/TE = 2530 ms/3.39 ms, flip angle = 7°, number of slices = 144, slices thickness = 1.33 mm, matrix size = 256 × 256, FOV = 256 mm × 256 mm.
Statistical analysis: behavioral data
Linear mixed model
Participants' TPP decisions were analyzed using a linear mixed model implemented in the R statistical package (version 3.6.1) with the lmerTest package (Kuznetsova et al., 2017). Specifically, participants' TPP decisions were modeled with within-subjects variables of Inequity (computed as differences between dictators and recipients according to the dictators' offers) and Identifiability (identified vs unidentified) as fixed variables and subjects as a random variable with varying intercepts and slopes. p values were determined by likelihood ratio tests between the full model with the effect in question against the model without the effect in question (Brown, 2021).
Computational modeling
To uncover the cognitive underpinnings of participants' TPP decisions, we modeled their choice behaviors within the framework of the inequity aversion model (Fehr and Schmidt, 1999). This widely used model assumes that participants consider both monetary payoffs and inequity when making decisions. In the current study, we used different variants of the inequity aversion model to formally test hypotheses regarding participants' perspectives (egocentric, impartial, or vicarious) in the TPP task (Zhong et al., 2016), as well as potential mechanisms (reference points vs sensitivity to inequity) by which identifiability influenced TPP decisions (Wright et al., 2011; Zhong et al., 2016).
First, an egocentric model (M1) of inequity aversion assumes that people are averse to outcome inequity between self and others only. In this model, consider 3 players indicated by i ∈ [1, 2, 3] for P1 the dictator, P2 the recipient, and P3 the third party (i.e., participants); let d indicate the corresponding TPP decision made, M = [m1, m2, m3] denotes the vector of monetary payoffs of all 3 players, and θ represents a set of parameters in each model. The utility (U) of choice could be represented as follows (Fehr and Schmidt, 1999):
Second, an impartial model of inequity aversion assumes that third party takes an impartial perspective to reduce general inequity between P1 and P2 with the consideration of his own payoff (Zhong et al., 2016). In other words, a participant cares about his own payoff as well as is averse to general inequity between P1 and P2 but is not directly involved in inequity comparison with P1 or P2 as assumed in the egocentric model. According to this model, the utility of choice could be represented as follows:
Two variants of the impartial model were used to investigate two potential mechanisms by which identifiability might influence TPP decisions. First, the scaling impartial model (M2) assumes that participants assign different weights to inequity based on identifiability. In other words, identifiability is hypothesized to impact TPP decisions by affecting individuals' sensitivity to inequity (e.g., Wright et al., 2011). In the scaling model, the utility of choice takes the following forms for identified and unidentified contexts, respectively:
Alternatively, extensive research has documented the influential role of reference points in human decision-making, with reference-dependent preferences being a fundamental concept in prospect theory (Tversky and Kahneman, 1991). In light of this, the additive impartial model (M3) posits that identifiability modifies TPP decisions by modulating reference points of inequity. In other words, participants may hold different prior expectations regarding typical allocations by dictators in identified and unidentified contexts, which serve as reference points for assessing inequity in those respective contexts (e.g., Sanfey, 2009; Chang and Sanfey, 2013). A notable distinction between scaling and additive models lies in the relationship between the effects of identifiability and the level of inequity. In the scaling model, the effects of identifiability increase proportionally with higher levels of inequity. However, in the additive model, the impact of identifiability on punishment remains constant across different levels of inequity as long as they exceed the reference points (Jenkins et al., 2018). Within the additive model, context-specific reference points of inequity are modeled as additive shifting parameters (Zhong et al., 2016; Jenkins et al., 2018), as opposed to being fixed at an equal split between dictators and victims (i.e., 0). Accordingly, the utility of choice takes the following forms:
Third, a vicarious model of inequity aversion assumes that participants take a vicarious perspective with P2 and only disliked the disadvantageous inequity of P2 with P1. Accordingly, third-party's TPP decisions are driven by reducing the vicarious disadvantageous inequity of P2 with the consideration of his own payoff. Similar to impartial models, the vicarious model assumes that participants are not directly involved in inequity comparison with P1 or P2. A crucial distinction between impartial and vicarious models lies in participants' attitude toward the condition where P2 has more than P1. In impartial models, participants dislike both P1 having more than P2 and P2 having more than P1; hence, any disparities between P1 and P2 result in disutility. In vicarious models, however, participants only object to P1 having more than P2 and remain indifferent to P2 having more than P1. This distinction is behaviorally significant since participants' TPP decisions could lead to a scenario where P2 has more than P1.
Similar to impartial models, two variants of vicarious model were constructed to examine whether identifiability modulated sensitivity to inequity or reference points of inequity. In the scaling vicarious model (M4), it is assumed that identifiability affects TPP decisions through impacting sensitivity to inequity, and the utility of choice takes the following forms:
In the additive vicarious model (M5), it is assumed that identifiability affects TPP decisions through impacting reference points of inequity, and the utility of choice takes the following forms:
Finally, based on the utility of each choice, we used the softmax function to evaluate the probability (P) of each choice as follows:
Parameter estimation
Parameter estimation was conducted using hierarchical Bayesian analysis, which enables more stable and precise estimation compared with maximum likelihood estimation (Ahn et al., 2013). The hierarchical Bayesian analysis was implemented with the RStan package in R. The RStan package depends on an interface to R provided by Stan (Carpenter et al., 2017), which adopts Markov Chain Monte Carlo sampling methods to execute fully Bayesian inference (see also Ahn et al., 2017). Following the approach in the hBayesDM package (Ahn et al., 2017), it was assumed that individual-level parameters were drawn from group-level normal distribution Normal (μ0, σ0) where μ0 and σ0 refer to group-level mean and SD, in other words, the hyper parameters of individual-level parameters. Weakly informative priors were adopted for the priors of the group-level normal means and SDs: μ0, Normal (0, 10); and σ0, half-Cauchy (0, 5). This was to minimize the impact of priors on the posterior distributions. It should be noted that closely similar results of parameter estimates were revealed with an even more weakly informative hyper-priors (μ0, Normal (0, 100) and σ0, half-Cauchy (0, 10)). The ranges of parameters were set based on theoretical/empirical plausibility as well as previous studies using similar models. For the parameters of inequity aversion (α or δ) and the softmax inverse temperature (λ), the value 0, as the lower bound, was set based on theoretical plausibility. Negative values of inequity aversion parameters are implausible, as they would assume that participants preferred inequity between self and others or between others in the framework of inequity aversion model. Likewise, negative values of softmax inverse temperature are implausible, as they would assume that participants preferred options with lower utility than those with higher utility. The upper bound (i.e., 10) of these parameters was set based on previous studies using similar models, which have either explicitly set similar ranges or empirically demonstrated that parameter estimates are within the range of 0-10 (Wright et al., 2011; Xiang et al., 2013; Zhong et al., 2016). In accordance, our data also showed that all α/δ/λ estimates are <10 (see also Results). For the reference points of inequity (η), we have mentioned above that the range between 0 (participants expected the dictator to equally split the money units) and 12 (participants expected the dictator(s) take all money units) includes all plausible values. The constraint was implemented with inverse probit transform following the approach of the hBayesDM package (Ahn et al., 2017). Models were defined in a Stan file, which was then compiled in the R environment. We fit each model with three Markov Chain Monte Carlo chains, each of which contains 2000 iterations after 2000 iterations for warm-up. Parameter estimates of the winning model (i.e., M5, the additive vicarious model, see also Results) were used for the comparisons between contexts, as well as correlational analyses with self-reported ratings, personality traits, and neuroimaging measures. The details of these analyses are illustrated in Materials and Methods and Results.
Model comparison
The leave-one-out information criterion (LOOIC) and widely applicable information criterion (WAIC) were used as the criteria for model comparison, as recommended in recent studies (Vehtari et al., 2017). Both LOOIC and WAIC represent the estimation of out-of-sample pointwise predictive accuracy using the posterior simulations. LOOIC uses leave-one-out cross-validation, and WAIC is based on the series expansion of leave-one-out cross-validation (Vehtari et al., 2017). By convention, a lower LOOIC or WAIC indicates better prediction accuracy of candidate models; thus, the model with the lowest WAIC and LOOIC is the winning one. In general, a 10 point difference in LOOIC or WAIC between two models can demonstrate superiority (Burnham and Anderson, 2004). The LOOIC and WAIC of all candidate models were computed using the “loo” package in R.
Based on the differences in LOOIC/WAIC value between each candidate model and the winning model, it is also possible to obtain LOOIC/WAIC weights. These weights indicate the probability of being the best model for each model, given the data and the set of candidate models. Previous studies have provided detailed information on the calculation of LOOIC/WAIC weights (Wagenmakers and Farrell, 2004; Park et al., 2021).
Model validation
To further validate our model's performance, model validation was implemented. First, we correlated the average amounts of TPP between actual behavioral results and model predictions across participants. Second, we examined the correlation between the average amounts of TPP in actual behavioral results and model predictions across trials.
Parameter recovery
We ran parameter recovery analyses to assess how accurate a model estimates true parameter values from the simulated choice data generated from the true parameter values (Palminteri et al., 2017; Wilson and Collins, 2019). For this purpose, the estimated posterior means from actual data were used as the true parameter values, which provided plausible combinations of parameter values for parameter recovery analysis (see also Park et al., 2002). Afterward, simulated choice data were generated by using the true parameter values (for 27 subjects and 80 trials per subject). Finally, we used the simulated choice data to estimate the parameters, and then evaluated the model performance by calculating correlations between the true and predicted parameter values. The simulation was implemented 50 times for each participant. The means and SDs of the Pearson correlation coefficients between actual and simulated parameter estimates were reported.
Statistical analysis: fMRI data
Univariate analysis
Neuroimaging data analyses were performed with SPM 12 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm12/). Preprocessing of functional data included slice timing, realignment through rigid-body registration to correct for head motion, normalization to MNI space, interpolation of voxel sizes to 2 × 2 × 2 mm and smoothing (8 mm FWHM kernel).
A two-level GLM was used to analyze the functional data. For the first level, boxcar regressors were defined for each participant and for each epoch of the time course. The regressors modeled the BOLD response to the epoch of both split phase (4 s) and response phase (3 s). Two regressors were created for the split phase: one for the identified context and the other for the unidentified context, with the levels of inequity (i.e., 0, 2, 8, 10, 12) as a parametric modulator. Another regressor was created for the response phase across contexts. These regressors were convolutions between respective boxcar stimulus function with the canonical hemodynamic impulse-response function (Büchel et al., 1998). The GLM also modeled first-order temporal autocorrelations in the residual. For the second level, parametric modulations of inequity were compared between identified and unidentified contexts with a paired t test to examine the interaction between Inequity and Identifiability. To investigate the neural correlates of individual differences in changes of reference points between the identified and unidentified contexts, as estimated from the winning model (additive vicarious model, M5, as described in Results), participant-specific parameter values were incorporated as a covariate at the second level (Wright et al., 2011). These parameter values were included in the analysis of the interaction contrast, which compared the effects of inequity between the identified and unidentified contexts. The main effects of Inequity and Identifiability were also assessed.
For false positive control, we used whole-brain cluster correction with a cluster-defining threshold of p < 0.001 and a family-wise error corrected threshold of p < 0.05 unless otherwise stated (Woo et al., 2014; Eklund et al., 2016). A small volume correction was also conducted within ROIs, focusing on those brain areas consistently implicated in costly punishment in light of recent meta-analyses (Gabay et al., 2014; Feng et al., 2015, 2021a). A small volume of interest was created to include these regions in both hemispheres according to the automated anatomic labeling (aal) atlas (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002): aal insula ROIs were used for insula; aal Cingulum_Ant ROIs were used for ACC; mPFC was constructed as a combination of the separate aal ROIs Frontal_Sup_Medial, Frontal_Med_Orb, and rectus; dlPFC was generated with aal ROIs Frontal_Mid and Frontal_Sup; aal caudate ROIs were used for caudate; and aal putamen ROIs were used for putamen (for similar ROI constructions, see also Strobel et al., 2011; Corradi-Dell'Acqua et al., 2013; Feng et al., 2017). Statistical significance was established through either whole-brain cluster-level inference or small volume correction and was stated explicitly in Results.
Multivoxel pattern analysis (MVPA)
To examine brain regions that were differentially involved in the dissociation of fair and unfair events in the identified and unidentified contexts, we performed the MVPA implemented in The Decoding Toolbox (Hebart et al., 2014). For each subject, we estimated a GLM in which trials of four conditions (i.e., fair offers in the identified context, unfair offers in the identified context, fair offers in the unidentified context, unfair offers in the unidentified context) were individually modeled. The estimated β maps were then fed to a support vector machine classifier with a fixed cost parameter (c = 1) and searchlight decoding scheme (Kriegeskorte et al., 2006; Corradi-Dell'Acqua et al., 2011). For each voxel in the individual participants' brain image in the native space, a sphere with a radius of four voxels was defined. For images associated with each condition in each run, the parameter estimates for each of the N voxels in a given sphere were then extracted to represent an N-dimensional pattern vector. Pattern vectors in one run (training set) were used to train the support vector machine to discriminate between fair and unfair offers separately for identified and unidentified contexts, and the performance of the classifier was measured on the other independent run (test set). The d′ was computed as a metric of the sensitivity of the classifier to discriminate fair and unfair offers in the test set (Corradi-Dell'Acqua et al., 2011, 2016). The procedure was repeated twice, with each run being the test set once. The mean d′ across two folds was calculated and assigned to the central voxel of the sphere. The classification was conducted for each voxel, resulting in a 3D d′ map for each context and each subject. At the group level, the classification performance was compared between identified and unidentified contexts, and the statistical significance was determined using a permutation test (N = 5000) implemented with the SnPM toolbox of SPM (Corradi-Dell'Acqua et al., 2011, 2014). The MVPA focused on classifying unfair and fair offers rather than decoding the magnitude of inequity, because of the relatively small and unbalanced number of trials available for each level of inequity.
The MVPA and univariate analyses share the use of mean activation, but MVPA also leverages voxel-level variability, even if a single voxel does not significantly change across conditions. In this study, both univariate and MVPAs were reported for several reasons. First, univariate analysis has been the standard and conventional approach in the literature. Reporting the results of univariate analyses allows for direct comparisons with previous findings and facilitates future meta-analytic studies. Second, MVPA and univariate approaches are sensitive to different variances, noise sources, and different types of effects (Davis et al., 2014). As a result, the two approaches can be used in a complementary manner to provide a more comprehensive understanding of the data (see also Weaverdyck et al., 2020).
Connectivity analysis
The activation and MVPAs identified the involvement of left vAI, dAI, and dACC in the identifiability effect (see also Results). We examined whether these regions worked together with other brain regions to underlie the identifiability effect, with an analysis of psychophysiological interaction (PPI) (Friston et al., 1997) using the revealed areas as ROIs. Specifically, we used the generalized PPI toolbox (http://www.nitrc.org/projects/gppi, version 13.1) (McLaren et al., 2012) with fMRI signal time courses individually extracted from a given ROI as the seeding signals. These seeding signals were then deconvolved with the canonical HRF, resulting in estimates of underlying neuronal activity. Subsequently, the interactions of these estimated neuronal time-series and vectors representing each of the onsets for each regressor and corresponding parametric modulators as in the univariate activation analysis were computed. Last, these interaction terms were reconvolved with the HRF and entered into a new GLM along with the vectors for the onsets of each event (i.e., the psychological terms). Group-level analysis of the PPI data was similar to that of the activation data, except that the β values used were derived from the PPI regressors.
Statistical analysis: correlation analyses
To explore the individual differences in the identifiability effect on TPP, we first computed the Pearson correlation coefficient (r) of behavioral data (e.g., changes in punishment across contexts) with personality traits closely related to altruistic behaviors, including empathic concern and SDO.
We also took a intrinsic functional connectivity (FC) approach to examine the relationship between the intrinsic functional connectivity characteristics of the dlPFC (see also Results) and individual differences in the identifiability effects of TPP. This approach was inspired by the exciting advances in human brain functional connectomics research revealing the cognitive/behavioral significance of the resting-state brain functions. Indeed, resting-state fMRI is the workhorse to examine the neural basis of individual differences (e.g., Feng et al., 2021b, 2018a,b; Lu et al., 2019). A major reason for its widespread adoption is its minimal requirements (Dubois and Adolphs, 2016). As a task-independent approach, resting-state fMRI is free from confounds associated with ongoing task demand and different experimental designs across studies (Kable and Levy, 2015; Nash and Knoch, 2016), so it is well suitable for quantifying individual differences. In contrast, the task-dependent approach (e.g., task fMRI) was usually designed to identify group effects that allows inferences about the functions of the “average human brain,” which treats between-subjects variance as noises (Elliott et al., 2021). Here we focused on the dlPFC as a ROI because this region has been causally linked to the implementation of altruistic behaviors (Knoch et al., 2006; Ruff et al., 2013).
A whole-brain voxel-wise FC matrix for each participant was obtained by computing the Pearson correlation coefficient (r) between the time series of each pair of brain voxels in the brain. The FC matrix was then binarized, based on which the degree of a voxel was defined as the number of connections (edges) that connected the given voxel with other voxels (Rubinov and Sporns, 2010). Nodes (e.g., voxels or regions) with a higher degree are interacting with more other nodes in the network and are more likely to serve as hubs in large-scale brain networks. Notably, degree measures have higher reliability than other nodal centrality metrics (Wang et al., 2011) and are closely associated with measures of brain physiology (Liang et al., 2013). Therefore, the degree is a widely used metric in the functional connectivity literature to indicate the extent to which a given node represents a center for information integration (Sporns et al., 2007; Zuo et al., 2012).
Statistical analysis: mediation analyses
Based on correlational analyses (see also Results), mediation analyses were implemented to explore whether the relationship between personality traits and identifiability effects of TPP was mediated by the reference points of inequity estimated from the winning model (i.e., M5, the additive vicarious model, see also Results). Specifically, empathic concern/SDO scores were used as the independent variable, reference points of inequity as the mediator variable, and changes in TPP as the dependent variable.
By convention, the indirect effect is produced by the effect of the independent variable on the mediator variable and the effect of the mediator variable on the dependent variable after controlling the independent variable (MacKinnon et al., 2007). Bootstrapping procedures were implemented with mediation package in R (3.6.1) to confirm the statistical significance of the specific indirect effect. Specifically, we generated a 95% CI using 5000 samplings. If a 95% CI fails to contain zero, the indirect effect is interpreted as statistically significant (p < 0.05).
Code availability
The results and the analysis code are available at https://github.com/SummerTian00/identifiability-on-third-party-punishment. Because of the large size of the raw neuroimaging data, they will be available from the corresponding author on request.
Results
Modulations of identifiability on TPP
The linear mixed model on participants' TPP decisions revealed a main effect of Inequity, indicating that participants increased their punishment as a function of inequity levels (χ2(1) = 35.97, p = 2.00 × 10−9). Notably, an Inequity × Identifiability interaction was significant (χ2(1) = 5.74, p = 0.017), indicating that the increase of punishment with inequity levels was more pronounced in the identified context compared with the unidentified context (Fig. 2A). Last, the main effect of Identifiability was not significant (χ2(1) = 0.33, p = 0.56).
The linear mixed model analysis on self-reported feelings revealed significant main effects of Inequity (valence: χ2(1) = 56.67, p = 5.16 × 10−14, arousal: χ2(1) = 43.55, p = 4.13 × 10−11, responsibility: χ2(1) = 46.59, p = 8.77 × 10−12, and fairness: χ2(1) = 68.10, p = 2.00 × 10−16). However, there were no significant main effects of Identifiability (valence: χ2(1) = 3.46, p = 0.063, arousal: χ2(1) = 6.26, p = 0.012, responsibility: χ2(1) = 0.61, p = 0.43, and fairness: χ2(1) = 0.38, p = 0.54) or Inequity × Identifiability interaction (valence: χ2(1) = 0.08, p = 0.78, arousal: χ2(1) = 0.07, p = 0.79, responsibility: χ2(1) = 1.83, p = 0.18, and fairness: χ2(1) = 0.24, p = 0.63) on these post-ratings, except for the main effect of Identifiability on arousal feelings. This indicates that participants reported feeling more aroused in the identified context compared with the unidentified context.
Association of context-dependent punishment with subjective feelings and personality traits
Although our results demonstrated the influence of identifiability at the group level, participants differed widely on the extent to which the identifiability of transgressors impacts TPP (Fig. 2B). To account for the individual differences in the context-dependent TPP, associations between punishment decisions and subjective feelings and personality traits were assessed.
First, the correlation analyses revealed that context-dependent punishment, measured as the differential effect of Inequity between identified context and unidentified context, was associated with context-dependent self-reports of arousal (r = 0.81, p = 5.25 × 10−7, Fig. 2C) and responsibility (r = 0.74, p = 1.70 × 10−5, Fig. 2D). That is, larger changes in the feelings of arousal and responsibility across contexts were associated with larger adjustments in punishment.
Second, the context-dependent TPP was negatively associated with scores in empathic concern (r = −0.62, p = 0.001, Fig. 2E) and positively associated with scores in social dominance orientation (r = 0.55, p = 0.003, Fig. 2F). In other words, participants scoring higher in empathic concern were less sensitive to the identified and unidentified contexts, whereas participants scoring higher in social dominance orientation were more sensitive to the identifiability of transgressors.
Computational mechanisms underlying the context-dependent punishment
The additive vicarious model (M5, LOOIC = 1725.72, WAIC = 1682.55) of inequity aversion had the lowest scores for both LOOIC and WAIC compared with other models, including the egocentric model (M1, LOOIC = 2541.18, WAIC = 2617.00), the scaling impartial model (M2, LOOIC = 2497.75, WAIC = 2467.11), the additive impartial model (M3, LOOIC = 1793.38, WAIC = 1746.78), and the scaling vicarious model (M4, LOOIC = 2401.82, WAIC = 2365.49). Therefore, the additive vicarious model (M5) was the winning model. In addition, both LOOIC and WAIC weights indicated the superiority of the additive vicarious model (M5, both weights were >0.9999) compared with the other models (both weights were <0.0001). The winning model was further validated with two subsequent analyses. First, TPP decisions from model prediction and behavioral data showed similar fluctuations across trials (mean correlation coefficient ± SD, 0.93 ± 0.01). Furthermore, we found strong correlation between model prediction and actual behavior across participants (mean correlation coefficient ± SD, 0.99 ± 0.003). These results provided robust evidence that our model well predicted participants' behaviors. Second, the recovery analysis revealed that the correspondence between the true and recovered parameters was very high (mean correlation coefficient ± SD for ηi: 0.98 ± 0.02; ηu: 0.98 ± 0.01; δ: 0.94 ± 0.02; λ: 0.86 ± 0.05).
These results indicated that participants' TPP decisions were made from a vicarious perspective rather than an egocentric or impartial perspective and that identifiability modulated punishment behaviors by altering the reference points of inequity. Therefore, we used parameter estimation for this winning model (M5, the additive vicarious model) in subsequent analyses. Participants had lower reference points of inequity in the identified (mean ηi ± SD: 3.43 ± 3.12, range, 0.22-9.28) compared with the unidentified (mean ηu ± SD: 4.68 ± 3.48, range, 0.14-11.29) context (t(26) = 2.44, p = 0.022, paired t test, Fig. 3A). For the inequity sensitivity parameter δ, the mean ± SD was 0.84 ± 0.29 (range, 0.39-1.69); for the inverse temperature parameter λ, the mean ± SD was 2.56 ± 0.86 (range, 0.99-3.92).
The differences in the reference points between identified and unidentified contexts were associated with both context-dependent subjective feelings (arousal: r = −0.84, p = 1.12 × 10−7, Fig. 3D; responsibility: r = −0.77, p = 4.00 × 10−6, Fig. 3E) and personality traits (empathic concern: r = 0.53, p = 0.004, Fig. 3F; social dominance orientation: r = −0.52, p = 0.005, Fig. 3G).
Last, the mediation analyses revealed that the impact of both empathic concern (indirect effect = −0.01, 95% CI = [−0.020, −0.0007], p = 0.026, Fig. 3B) and social dominance orientation (indirect effect = 0.004, 95% CI = [0.0004, 0.01], p = 0.018, Fig. 3C) on context-dependent TPP were mediated by the differences in reference points of inequity.
Identifiability modulates activity and functional couplings of the left vAI
The parametric modulations of inequity revealed that left vAI showed stronger correlation with the extent of inequity in the identified context than in the unidentified context (peak MNI coordinates: x, y, z = −34, 18, −16 mm, whole-brain corrected at the cluster level, Fig. 4A,B).
The PPI analysis revealed reliable context-dependent functional connectivity for the left vAI as the seed region. In particular, the functional couplings of left vAI with the right dlPFC showed weaker correlation with the extent of inequity in the identified context than the unidentified context (peak MNI coordinates: x, y, z = 20, 48, 24 mm, small-volume corrected at the cluster level, Fig. 4C). Post hoc analysis revealed that the effect was driven by the reduced vAI-dlPFC connectivity in the identified context (t(26) = −4.72, p = 7.00 × 10−5, one-sample t test) rather than the enhanced connectivity in the unidentified context (t(26) = 0.93, p = 0.36, one-sample t test) compared with the baseline (Fig. 4D).
Identifiability modulates multivariate neural patterns in the left dAI and dACC
The MVPA revealed the left dAI (peak MNI coordinates: x, y, z = −26, 26, 14 mm, whole-brain corrected at the cluster level, Fig. 5A,B) and dACC (peak MNI coordinates: x, y, z = 6, 12, 34 mm, whole-brain corrected at the cluster level, Fig. 5C,D), in which spatial patterns of brain activity differentiating fair and unfair offers were more robust and distinct in the identified context than the unidentified context.
The PPI analysis revealed context-dependent functional connectivity for the left dAI as the seed region with the ventral striatum (VS, peak MNI coordinates: x, y, z = −8, 12, −4 mm, whole-brain corrected at the cluster level, Fig. 6A), such that the dAI-VS connectivity was attenuated in the identified context (t(26) = −4.38, p = 1.80 × 10−4, one-sample t test) but enhanced in the unidentified context (t(26) = 3.15, p = 0.004, one-sample t test) compared with the baseline (Fig. 6B). In addition, the exploratory correlational analyses revealed that in the identified context the dAI-VS connectivity strength was negatively correlated with empathic concern (r = −0.46, p = 0.016) but positively correlated with social dominance orientation (r = 0.46, p = 0.017). In the unidentified context, the dAI-VS connectivity strength was positively correlated with reference points of inequity (r = 0.38, p = 0.048) and negatively correlated with the effect of inequity level on punishment amounts (r = −0.45, p = 0.019).
Neural correlates of individual differences in identifiability effects
We first investigated whether individual differences in the effects of identifiability on reference points could be explained by alterations in brain responses to inequity between the identified and unidentified contexts. The findings demonstrated a positive association between changes in reference points and activity in the precuneus/cuneus (peak MNI coordinates: x, y, z = 4, −82, 24 mm) and right temporoparietal junction (rTPJ, peak MNI coordinates: x, y, z = 46, −46, 18 mm) across participants (Fig. 7A), with a cluster-defining threshold of p < 0.005 and a family-wise error whole-brain corrected threshold of p < 0.05 at cluster level. Specifically, the larger responses of these regions to inequity in unidentified (vs identified) context, the higher reference points of inequity in the unidentified than identified context.
We further examined individual differences in the effects of identifiability from the perspective of the resting-state functional connectivity. The correlation analyses revealed that the degree (computed as the number of connections that connected the given voxel with other voxels) of the dlPFC was associated with both identifiability effects on punishment behavior (r = 0.46, p = 0.016, Fig. 7B) and reference points of inequity (r = −0.42, p = 0.029, Fig. 7C). That is, the more connections the dlPFC maintained at resting state, the larger amounts of TPP and the lower reference points of inequity in the identified than unidentified context.
Main effect of brain activations
Main effect of inequity
The univariate activation analysis (whole-brain corrected at the cluster level) revealed that activations of the following brain regions were increased with the inequity of offers: AI (covering both ventral and dorsal subregions), dACC, dlPFC, middle cingulate cortex, striatum, inferior parietal lobules, middle occipital cortex, and cerebrum. In contrast, brain activity in the supramarginal gyrus was decreased as a function of inequity.
Main effect of context
The univariate activation analysis (whole-brain corrected at the cluster level) revealed that brain activations in the following regions were enhanced in the identified context than in the unidentified context: amygdala, fusiform gyrus, middle occipital cortex, rectus, and dorsomedial PFC. In contrast, brain activations in the superior temporal gyrus and angular gyrus were higher in the unidentified context than in the identified context.
Discussion
Our findings revealed the identifiable transgressor effect, with participants showing greater punitive behavior toward identified wrongdoers. The computational modeling results suggested that the identifiability effect was driven by lower reference points of inequity in the identified context. Our neuroimaging results revealed that the degree of norm violations was associated with higher responses in the vAI and lower connectivity strength between vAI and dlPFC in the identified than unidentified context. Likewise, multivariate neural patterns in the dAI and dACC manifested more differentiated coding of fair and unfair events in the identified context, and the connectivity strength between dAI and VS was lower in the identified context. Last, individual differences in the identifiability effect could be explained by personality traits, altered activity in the precuneus/cuneus and rTPJ, and intrinsic functional connectivity properties of the dlPFC. Overall, our study provides the first characterization of the neurocomputational underpinnings of the identifiability effect on TPP in terms of mediating processes and modulating factors.
The identifiable other effect has been attributed to greater emotional responses evoked by identified targets (Small et al., 2007; Slovic, 2010). For instance, previous research has shown that moral emotions, such as empathy and anger, mediate the effect of identifiability (Small and Loewenstein, 2005; Kogut, 2011). People's tendency to help identifiable victims is eliminated when they are instructed or primed to think deliberatively (Small et al., 2007). Our results support this idea, revealing that the identifiability effect was linked to self-reported affect arousal and responsibility. However, our study goes beyond previous research by providing a more nuanced understanding of the cognitive processes underlying the identifiability effect.
Participants cared only about the disadvantage of victims, but not that of perpetrators. These findings suggest that third-party participants adopted the perspective of victims instead of acting as impartial judges between victims and perpetrators. Thus, TPP may be more driven by retaliatory purposes than impartial justice (Jordan et al., 2016; Stallen et al., 2018; Gummerum et al., 2022). Importantly, the identifiability effect is mediated by changes in participants' reference points of inequity rather than the weights assigned to inequity aversion. In accordance, previous studies have demonstrated that participants' reference points of inequity are malleable across social contexts (Chang and Sanfey, 2013). For example, informing participants about the average offer impacts the costly punishment by altering reference points (Bohnet and Zeckhauser, 2004). In the identified context, adopting lower reference points (i.e., more stringent norms), participants might experience larger violations of expectations and stronger negative emotions in response to norm violations (Chang and Sanfey, 2013; Xiang et al., 2013). This, in turn, led to higher punishment to identifiable transgressors. These computational findings were complemented by our neuroimaging results, which provided evidence implicating how these processes are implemented in human brain.
When transgressors were identified (vs unidentified), the extent of norm violations showed increased association with responses of the vAI, but decreased association with functional coupling between vAI and dlPFC. The vAI is associated with emotional processing (Chang and Sanfey, 2013; Bellucci et al., 2018), contributing to encoding the visceral experience of negative feelings elicited by norm violations (Zhou et al., 2014; Feng et al., 2016). Previous meta-analyses have demonstrated consistent involvement of dlPFC in costly punishment (Gabay et al., 2014; Feng et al., 2015), with higher activation associated with lower costly punishment (Harlé and Sanfey, 2012). This suggests that the dlPFC may play a role in regulating affective processes by actively maintaining task rules and goals online, namely, accumulating money (Sanfey et al., 2003; Harlé and Sanfey, 2012). Considering this perspective, the decreased vAI-dlPFC connectivity to increasing inequity levels in the identified context could be indicative of heightened emotion-related thinking and reduced analytical thinking (Kogut, 2011). That is, the active maintenance of task goals mediated by the dlPFC might be particularly disrupted when participants observed that an identified dictator proposed increasingly unfair offers.
The identifiability of transgressors amplified neural representations of norm violations in the dAI and dACC while decreasing connectivity between the dAI and VS. The dAI and dACC are involved in tracking deviations from expectations (Seeley et al., 2007; Menon and Uddin, 2010). Unfair offers are encoded in the dAI and dACC as prediction errors (Wu et al., 2016; Luo et al., 2018), triggering the enforcement of social norms by punishing transgressors (Montague and Lohrenz, 2007; Chang and Sanfey, 2013; Xiang et al., 2013). The dAI and dACC likely encode context-dependent norm violations by integrating various information, including negative emotions and self-interest, conveyed by interconnected regions, such as the amygdala and VS (Craig, 2002). The VS is associated with representing concerns for self-interest during altruistic decisions (Hu et al., 2017). For instance, individuals with a strong self-interest inclination tend to donate less to charity and exhibit heightened VS activation when receiving money for themselves (Brosch et al., 2011). Additionally, the VS activity tends to be stronger for advantageous inequity than equal outcomes (Fliessbach et al., 2012). In light of previous findings, the heightened dAI-VS connectivity to escalating inequity levels in the unidentified context may reflect an increased self-interest motive to maximize personal gains or a greater concern for the cost of punishment. These self-interested concerns could be particularly pronounced for unfair allocations, where participants face a conflict between self-interest and equity norms. This interpretation is reinforced by our findings that stronger dAI-VS connectivity is linked to reduced inequity effects on punitive behavior and higher reference points in the unidentified context. Conversely, in the identified context, participants adhere to a stricter equity norm and concern less about self-interest when facing unfair allocations. This corresponds to the reduced dAI-VS connectivity to increasing inequity levels in the identified context. Overall, the context-dependent neural patterns in the dAI/dACC and dAI-VS connectivity suggest that participants enhance self-interest concerns in the unidentified context but reduce them in the identified context when confronted with a trade-off between self-interest and equity norms. These findings complement computational modeling results, suggesting that the unidentified (vs identified) context may be associated with a greater emphasis on self-interest, resulting in less stringent evaluations of norm violations and a more lenient equity norm.
Last, the identifiability effect on TPP was modulated by personality traits, activity in the precuneus/cuneus and rTPJ, and degree of the dlPFC. The associations between altruistic behaviors and empathic concern (FeldmanHall et al., 2015) or SDO (Pratto et al., 1994) have been well established, while our study is the first to demonstrate that they modulate the extent to which people's altruistic punishment is subject to contextual effects. The precuneus/cuneus and rTPJ represent key nodes of the metalizing system, which has been widely recognized to engage in perspective taking and intention inference (Frith and Frith, 2006). The relationship between the identifiability effect and altered metalizing-related activity generally aligns with the established role of modeling others' mind in costly punishment (Wright et al., 2011). Furthermore, the association between the degree of dlPFC and the identifiability effect aligns with two lines of research. First, previous studies have established the causal role of dlPFC in human altruism (Ruff et al., 2013). Second, brain regions frequently engaged by a given task exhibit intrinsic functional organization in the absence of an explicit task, which might underlie the individual differences in responding to task demands (Raichle, 2006).
Several limitations should be acknowledged. First, future studies should consider using larger and equal numbers of trials across different inequity levels to more accurately decode the magnitude of inequity. Second, the functional interplay between vAI and dAI, and how affective or cognitive processes, respectively, mediated by these regions collectively contribute to context-dependent costly punishment await further investigation. Third, examining how the amygdala mediates context-dependent norm detection and negative emotions represents an intriguing avenue for future research, because of the association between amygdala and TPP severity (Stallen et al., 2018; Civai et al., 2019). Fourth, our study did not collect additional behavioral measures to assist the interpretation of the reference point parameter. Nevertheless, the validity of this parameter has been demonstrated in previous studies, where it captured experimentally manipulated changes in participants' prior expectations of allocation (Herz and Taubinsky, 2013; Xiang et al., 2013). Furthermore, our findings contribute external validity to the reference point parameter through its correlations with punishment behaviors, personality traits, and neuroimaging measures, which align with the interpretation of this parameter as reference points. Last, our discussion of the roles of the revealed brain regions is substantiated by corresponding brain-behavior correlations and evidence from existing literature. However, it is important to recognize that the fMRI findings primarily signify the engagement of these regions in the identifiable target effect. The interpretations we provide are based on the available evidence and should be viewed as plausible hypotheses. They provide a foundation on which future research can build, using more refined techniques or experimental designs to unravel the precise functions of these brain regions.
In conclusion, our study offers a more nuanced understanding of the identifiable transgressor effect by uncovering mediating processes and characterizing modulating factors. These results help to broaden our understanding of the context- and person-dependent altruistic behaviors.
Footnotes
This work was supported by National Natural Science Foundation of China 32271126, 31920103009, and 32020103008; Natural Science Foundation of Guangdong Province 2021A1515010746; Major Project of National Social Science Foundation 20&ZD153; and Shenzhen-Hong Kong Institute of Brain Science–Shenzhen Fundamental Research Institutions 2023SHIBS0003.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Chunliang Feng at chunliang.feng{at}m.scnu.edu.cn or Yue-Jia Luo at luoyj{at}bnu.edu.cn