Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Research Articles, Neurobiology of Disease

Prolactin-Releasing Peptide Contributes to Stress-Related Mood Disorders and Inhibits Sleep/Mood Regulatory Melanin-Concentrating Hormone Neurons in Rats

Szilvia Vas, Rege S. Papp, Katalin Könczöl, Emese Bogáthy, Noémi Papp, Csaba Ádori, Máté Durst, Klaudia Sípos, Klementina Ocskay, Imre Farkas, Flóra Bálint, Szilamér Ferenci, Bibiána Török, Anita Kovács, Evelin Szabó, Dóra Zelena, Krisztina J. Kovács, Anna Földes, Erzsébet Kató, László Köles, György Bagdy, Miklós Palkovits and Zsuzsanna E. Tóth
Journal of Neuroscience 1 February 2023, 43 (5) 846-862; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2139-21.2022
Szilvia Vas
1Department of Pharmacodynamics, Semmelweis University, Budapest, 1089, Hungary
2MTA-SE Neuropsychopharmacology and Neurochemistry Research Group, Semmelweis University, Budapest, 1089, Hungary
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Rege S. Papp
3Human Brain Tissue Bank and Laboratory, Semmelweis University, Budapest, 1094, Hungary
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Katalin Könczöl
4Laboratory of Neuroendocrinology and In Situ Hybridization, Department of Anatomy, Histology and Embryology, Semmelweis University, Budapest, 1094, Hungary
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Emese Bogáthy
1Department of Pharmacodynamics, Semmelweis University, Budapest, 1089, Hungary
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Noémi Papp
1Department of Pharmacodynamics, Semmelweis University, Budapest, 1089, Hungary
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Csaba Ádori
5Department of Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, 17177, Sweden
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Máté Durst
4Laboratory of Neuroendocrinology and In Situ Hybridization, Department of Anatomy, Histology and Embryology, Semmelweis University, Budapest, 1094, Hungary
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Klaudia Sípos
4Laboratory of Neuroendocrinology and In Situ Hybridization, Department of Anatomy, Histology and Embryology, Semmelweis University, Budapest, 1094, Hungary
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Klementina Ocskay
4Laboratory of Neuroendocrinology and In Situ Hybridization, Department of Anatomy, Histology and Embryology, Semmelweis University, Budapest, 1094, Hungary
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Imre Farkas
6Laboratory of Reproductive Neurobiology, Institute of Experimental Medicine, Eötvös Loránd Research Network, Budapest, 1083, Hungary
7Laboratory of Endocrine Neurobiology, Institute of Experimental Medicine, Eötvös Loránd Research Network, Budapest, 1083, Hungary
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Imre Farkas
Flóra Bálint
7Laboratory of Endocrine Neurobiology, Institute of Experimental Medicine, Eötvös Loránd Research Network, Budapest, 1083, Hungary
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Szilamér Ferenci
8Laboratory of Molecular Neuroendocrinology, Institute of Experimental Medicine, Eötvös Loránd Research Network, Budapest, 1083, Hungary
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Bibiána Török
9Laboratory of Behavioral and Stress Studies, Institute of Experimental Medicine, Eötvös Loránd Research Network, Budapest, 1083, Hungary
10Institute of Physiology, Medical School, University of Pécs, Centre for Neuroscience, Szentágothai Research Center, Pécs, 7624, Hungary
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Anita Kovács
10Institute of Physiology, Medical School, University of Pécs, Centre for Neuroscience, Szentágothai Research Center, Pécs, 7624, Hungary
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Evelin Szabó
10Institute of Physiology, Medical School, University of Pécs, Centre for Neuroscience, Szentágothai Research Center, Pécs, 7624, Hungary
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Dóra Zelena
9Laboratory of Behavioral and Stress Studies, Institute of Experimental Medicine, Eötvös Loránd Research Network, Budapest, 1083, Hungary
10Institute of Physiology, Medical School, University of Pécs, Centre for Neuroscience, Szentágothai Research Center, Pécs, 7624, Hungary
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Dóra Zelena
Krisztina J. Kovács
8Laboratory of Molecular Neuroendocrinology, Institute of Experimental Medicine, Eötvös Loránd Research Network, Budapest, 1083, Hungary
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Anna Földes
11Department of Oral Biology, Semmelweis University, Budapest, 1089, Hungary
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Erzsébet Kató
12Department of Pharmacology and Pharmacotherapy, Semmelweis University, Budapest, 1089, Hungary
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
László Köles
11Department of Oral Biology, Semmelweis University, Budapest, 1089, Hungary
12Department of Pharmacology and Pharmacotherapy, Semmelweis University, Budapest, 1089, Hungary
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
György Bagdy
1Department of Pharmacodynamics, Semmelweis University, Budapest, 1089, Hungary
2MTA-SE Neuropsychopharmacology and Neurochemistry Research Group, Semmelweis University, Budapest, 1089, Hungary
13NAP2-SE New Antidepressant Target Research Group, Budapest, 1085, Hungary
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Miklós Palkovits
3Human Brain Tissue Bank and Laboratory, Semmelweis University, Budapest, 1094, Hungary
4Laboratory of Neuroendocrinology and In Situ Hybridization, Department of Anatomy, Histology and Embryology, Semmelweis University, Budapest, 1094, Hungary
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Zsuzsanna E. Tóth
4Laboratory of Neuroendocrinology and In Situ Hybridization, Department of Anatomy, Histology and Embryology, Semmelweis University, Budapest, 1094, Hungary
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Stress disorders impair sleep and quality of life; however, their pathomechanisms are unknown. Prolactin-releasing peptide (PrRP) is a stress mediator; we therefore hypothesized that PrRP may be involved in the development of stress disorders. PrRP is produced by the medullary A1/A2 noradrenaline (NA) cells, which transmit stress signals to forebrain centers, and by non-NA cells in the hypothalamic dorsomedial nucleus. We found in male rats that both PrRP and PrRP-NA cells innervate melanin-concentrating hormone (MCH) producing neurons in the dorsolateral hypothalamus (DLH). These cells serve as a key hub for regulating sleep and affective states. Ex vivo, PrRP hyperpolarized MCH neurons and further increased the hyperpolarization caused by NA. Following sleep deprivation, intracerebroventricular PrRP injection reduced the number of REM sleep-active MCH cells. PrRP expression in the dorsomedial nucleus was upregulated by sleep deprivation, while downregulated by REM sleep rebound. Both in learned helplessness paradigm and after peripheral inflammation, impaired coping with sustained stress was associated with (1) overactivation of PrRP cells, (2) PrRP protein and receptor depletion in the DLH, and (3) dysregulation of MCH expression. Exposure to stress in the PrRP-insensitive period led to increased passive coping with stress. Normal PrRP signaling, therefore, seems to protect animals against stress-related disorders. PrRP signaling in the DLH is an important component of the PrRP's action, which may be mediated by MCH neurons. Moreover, PrRP receptors were downregulated in the DLH of human suicidal victims. As stress-related mental disorders are the leading cause of suicide, our findings may have particular translational relevance.

SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Treatment resistance to monoaminergic antidepressants is a major problem. Neuropeptides that modulate the central monoaminergic signaling are promising targets for developing alternative therapeutic strategies. We found that stress-responsive prolactin-releasing peptide (PrRP) cells innervated melanin-concentrating hormone (MCH) neurons that are crucial in the regulation of sleep and mood. PrRP inhibited MCH cell activity and enhanced the inhibitory effect evoked by noradrenaline, a classic monoamine, on MCH neurons. We observed that impaired PrRP signaling led to failure in coping with chronic/repeated stress and was associated with altered MCH expression. We found alterations of the PrRP system also in suicidal human subjects. PrRP dysfunction may underlie stress disorders, and fine-tuning MCH activity by PrRP may be an important part of the mechanism.

  • GABA(A) receptor
  • MCH
  • mental disorders
  • sleep
  • stress
  • suicide

Introduction

Chronic or repeated exposure to stress may cause serious mental health conditions, such as anxiety, post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), and major depressive disorder (Smoller, 2016). Dysregulation of the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, the stress axis, as well as mood and sleep disturbances are typical symptoms in patients, which develop by unknown mechanisms (Goldstein and Walker, 2014; Bao and Swaab, 2019).

Serotonin and noradrenaline (NA) reuptake inhibitor (SNRI) antidepressants are commonly used to treat major depressive disorder, anxiety, and PTSD, although treatment resistance to conventional SNRIs occurs frequently (Voineskos et al., 2020). Neuropeptides, which can modulate neurotransmitter levels in the brain (neuromodulators), may offer alternative antidepressant targets (Mantas et al., 2022). Here, we designate prolactin-releasing peptide (PrRP)/prolactin-releasing hormone (PRLH) as a novel candidate known to be involved in the regulation of stress, sleep, and mood (Dodd and Luckman, 2013). PrRP may act in part by affecting melanin-concentrating hormone (MCH) producing cells in the dorsolateral hypothalamus (DLH). Our hypothesis is based on the following.

PrRP neurons are in a decisive position for transmitting stress-related signals toward higher brain centers and influence the efficacy of noradrenergic transmission. The adaptive response of the HPA axis to homeostatic stressors and peripheral inflammation depends on the integrity of the ascending pathway from the medullary A1/A2 noradrenergic cell groups (Pacak et al., 1995). PrRP is expressed exclusively in the A1/A2 cell groups (Chen et al., 1999) and the hypothalamic dorsomedial nucleus (DM) (Dodd and Luckman, 2013). PrRP cells respond to various psychogenic stressors, such as conditioned-fear stimuli, foot shock, restraint, inflammatory stress, etc. (Ohiwa et al., 2007; Dodd and Luckman, 2013). PrRP-deficient mice fail to increase plasma ACTH following reexposure to a conditioned fear stimulus (Yoshida et al., 2014). Serving key role in stress, PrRP stimulates the release of ACTH (Maruyama et al., 2001) and corticosterone (Mera et al., 2006) in vivo, and substantially potentiates the ACTH-stimulatory effect of NA (Maruyama et al., 2001).

PrRP also affects the sleep–wake cycle, although the available literature is contradictory (Zhang et al., 2001; Lin et al., 2002). Ex vivo studies suggest that the reticular nucleus of the thalamus may mediate some effects of PrRP on the sleep–wake cycle (Lin et al., 2002), where PrRP alias PRLH receptor (PRLHR) expression is high, but PrRP axons are absent (Dodd and Luckman, 2013). In contrast, PrRP fibers innervate the DLH (Dodd and Luckman, 2013). The DLH is populated by MCH cells (Verret et al., 2003), which cells are considered a key regulatory hub of sleep and mood, moreover, participate in the control of stress-response (Smith et al., 2006; Diniz and Bittencourt, 2017; Bandaru et al., 2020). A series of data highlights MCH dysfunction underpinning the pathophysiology of anxiety, depression, and PTSD (Chaki et al., 2005; Chung et al., 2011; Torterolo et al., 2015; Concetti et al., 2020). Although antagonists of MCH1 receptor (MCH1R) have shown promise as antidepressant drugs (Chung et al., 2011; Johansson and Löfberg, 2015), the exact mechanism of how MCH contributes to stress-related mental disorders remains to be elucidated. MCH neuronal activity during acute stress (foot shock) was necessary for subsequent coping with contextual fear (Izawa et al., 2019) and for cued fear extinction (Concetti et al., 2020). Controversially, the administration of MCH in a repeated stress paradigm (learned helplessness [LHe]) impaired the animals' ability to cope adequately with a second foot shock, resulting in helpless behavior (Urbanavicius et al., 2019). Thus, it appears that proper control of MCH signaling is critical in preventing the adverse effects of chronic/repeated stress.

We applied an array of histologic and electrophysiological methods, as well as depression models, and analyzed human PrRP receptor expression in the DLH of suicidal subjects. Our data reveal that the PrRP system may contribute to the development of stress-related mental disorders because of defects in PrRP signaling caused by chronic/repeated stress. We propose that PrRP regulates the function of MCH neurons; furthermore, PrRP signaling in the DLH is impaired in animal models of depression and in suicidal humans.

Materials and Methods

Animals

The experimental animals were male Wistar rats (Toxi-Coop), housed and kept under standard laboratory conditions with 12/12 h light/dark cycle (lights on at 6:00 A.M., unless otherwise stated). We used males to exclude sex effects on the data, as PrRP expression varies depending on female gonadal hormone levels (Tóth et al., 2008). Rats were 10-12 weeks old, unless otherwise indicated. All experiments were performed in accordance with the National Institutes of Health's “Principles of Laboratory Animal Care” (NIH Publications No. 85-23, revised 1985), international standards (European Community Council Directive, 86/609/EEC, 1986, 2010), and specific national laws (the Hungarian Governmental Regulations on animal studies 40/2013). The experiments were approved by the National Scientific Ethical Committee on Animal Experimentation, the Ethical Review Board of the Semmelweis University (PE/EA/1563-7/2017, PE/EA/850-2/2016, PTE/84 033-6/2020), and the Animal Welfare Committee of the Institute of Experimental Medicine (PE/EA/43-4/2019) and met the guidelines of the Animal Hygiene and Food Control Department, Ministry of Agriculture, Hungary and the European Communities Council Directive recommendations for the care and use of laboratory animals (2010/63/EU).

For brain surgeries and transcardial perfusions, anesthesia was given to rats by intramuscular injection of a ketamine (50 mg/kg)/xylazine (4 mg/kg) (Richter Gedeon) cocktail. Animals were decapitated or were perfusion-fixed with 4% paraformaldehyde in 0.1 m PBS, pH 7.4. Brains were removed quickly, frozen and stored at −80°C. Perfusion-fixed brains were cryoprotected in 20% sucrose solution before freezing.

Immunohistochemistry (IHC)

For IHC, standard chromogenic or fluorescent immunohistochemical methods were applied (Table 1) using serial coronal 50-µm-thick free-floating sections. When ISH and IHC were combined, 20-µm-thick slide-mounted sections were used. Primary antibodies were applied after serum (1% BSA or 3% NDS, supplemented with 0.1% Triton X-100) and endogenous peroxidase (3% H2O2, all from Merck Life Science) activity blocking steps. Secondary antibody-conjugated peroxidase activity was eliminated by using 3% H2O2 solution supplemented with 0.01% Na-azide. Microwave treatment (5 min) was performed in 0.1 m citric acid (pH 6.0, Merck) for antigen retrieval, prevention of nonspecific cross reactions of antibodies, and/or for blocking HRP enzyme activity (Tóth and Mezey, 2007). Multiple detections were performed sequentially. The following reagents were used:

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Summary of the immunohistochemical reactionsa

Primary antibodies

Primary antibodies are as follows: rabbit anti-cFos (catalog #ABE 457, Merck); anti-pro-MCH (catalog #sc-28931, raised in rabbit or sc-14509, raised in goat, Santa Cruz Biotechnology); rabbit anti-Orexin A (aa:14-33, catalog #R-104-100, Novus Biologicals); rabbit anti-PrRP (catalog #H-008-52, Phoenix Pharmaceuticals); and mouse anti-TH (catalog #MAB318, Millipore).

Detection reagents

Detection reagents include the following: AlexaFluor-488-conjugated Donkey Anti-Rabbit IgG (catalog #A21206), AlexaFluor-594-conjugated Donkey Anti-Mouse IgG (catalog #A21203), AlexaFluor-647-conjugated Donkey Anti-Goat IgG (catalog #A32849), AlexaFluor-568-conjugated streptavidin (catalog #S11226), and Tyramide Signal Amplification (TSA) Kits (catalog #T20912, T20920, T20925) (Fisher Scientific; Invitrogen); biotinylated donkey anti-rabbit IgG (catalog #711-065-152) and peroxidase-conjugated Donkey Anti-Goat IgG (catalog #705-035-003) (Jackson ImmunoResearch Laboratories, Izinta Trading); biotinylated goat anti-rabbit IgG (catalog #BA-1000, Vector Laboratories); DAB (catalog #D12384), extravidin-horseradish peroxidase (catalog #E2886) (Merck); and Fluorescein Tyramide Reagent Pack (catalog #SAT701B, PerkinElmer Life and Analytical Sciences).

Analysis of PrRP and TH coexpression in the varicosities in the rat hypothalamus

Serial coronal sections from perfusion-fixed naive rats (n = 2) were triple-immunolabeled for MCH, PrRP, and TH (see above). Confocal z-stack images were acquired bilaterally through a 63× lens objective (see Imaging) from three sections per animal containing the highest density of MCH cells. The z separation was adjusted to 3 µm to avoid capturing the same varicosity twice. The number of PrRP+ varicosities were determined in the perifornical area (PFA) (20 z stacks) and the lateral hypothalamus (LH) (17 z stacks) in the single optical sections (1 AU, ROI size: 135 × 135 µm2). TH positivity of the same profiles was analyzed in a separate channel, using the multipoint tool in the ImageJ 1.32j software (Wayne Rasband; National Institutes of Health). The percentage of PrRP-TH coexpression was calculated for each optical section and used for the statistical analysis.

ISH

Radioactive ISH was used for localization of the PRLHR and Neuropeptide FF receptor 2 (NPFF2R) mRNAs (n = 2 rats), and for measuring PrRP mRNA levels in sleep-deprived animals. Hybridizations were performed using serial coronal 12-µm-thick fresh frozen and 20-µm-thick perfusion fixed sections, as described previously (Tóth et al., 2008) using [35S]UTP-labeled (Per-Form Hungaria) antisense riboprobes. The probes were generated by in vitro transcription (MAXIscript Kit, Fisher Scientific) according to the manufacturer's instructions using the following rat cDNA sequences: PrRP (10-240 bs, GenBank AB015418) and PRLHR (189-636 bs, GenBank NM_139193) cloned into pBC KS+ vector (Addgene), as well as NPFF2R (112-796 bs, GenBank AF268900.1) cloned into pGemT Easy vector (Promega). The specificity of the cDNA fragments was verified by sequencing and assessed by BLAST screening of the rat genome (https://blast.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/Blast.cgi). For signal detection, the sections were apposed to a BAS-MS imaging plate (Fuji PhotoFilm) for 5 d. The signals were read out by a Fujifilm FLA-8000 Image Analyzer. The sections were dipped into Kodak NTB autoradiographic emulsion (Agar Scientific), exposed for 5 d (PrRP ISH) or 6 weeks (PRLHR and NPFF2R ISHs), developed using Kodak Dektol developer and Fixer (Sigma-Aldrich), and were counterstained with Giemsa (Sigma), except when processed for IHC before ISH signal detection.

Brain slice electrophysiology

Whole-cell patch-clamp experiments were conducted using acute rat brain slices from 30-d-old rats (n = 20) to evaluate the effect of PrRP 12-31 (3.5 µm, #P7357, Merck) on MCH neurons and to test the effect of PrRP on the NA (10 µm, #A9512, Merck) evoked responses. Rats were anesthetized by 1% Isoflurane (Baxter) 1-2 h after lights on. After decapitation, the brain was removed and 250 µm thick coronal slices containing the DLH (bregma −1.43 mm) were generated with a Leica VT-1000S vibratome (Leica Microsystems) in ice-cold, reduced sodium aCSF equilibrated with 95% O2 and 5% CO2, (composition in mm as follows: saccharose 205, KCl 2.5, NaHCO3 26, MgCl2 5, NaH2PO4 1.25, CaCl2 1, glucose 10). The upper part of the slice was dissected just above the mammillothalamic tract to avoid influence of thalamic neurons expressing PRLHR (Ibata et al., 2000). Then the slices were incubated in aCSF (composition in mm as follows: NaCl 130, KCl 3.5, NaHCO3 26, MgSO4 1.2, NaH2PO4 1.25, CaCl2 2.5, glucose 10) for 1 h at room temperature. Recordings were conducted in aCSF at 33°C using an Axopatch-200B patch-clamp amplifier, Digidata-1322A data acquisition system, and pCLAMP 10.4 software (also used for analyses of data, Molecular Devices) under a BX51WI IR-DIC Olympus microscope. The patch electrodes (OD = 1.5 mm, thin wall, Hilgenberg) were pulled with a Flaming-Brown P-97 puller (Sutter Instruments), the resistance was 2-3 mΩ. The intracellular pipette solution contained the following (in mm): K-gluconate 130, KCl 10, NaCl 10, HEPES 10, MgCl2 0.1, EGTA 1, Mg-ATP 4, Na-GTP 0.3 and biocytin (1.5 mg/ml, all from Merck).

Cells in the PFA were injected with hyperpolarizing/depolarizing square-step-pulses (duration: 900 ms, amplitudes: −30 and 30 pA, respectively) in current-clamp mode. MCH neurons were identified by their characteristic ion current responses (van den Pol et al., 2004) and by post hoc IHC for MCH and detection of biocytin. Ion currents were recorded in voltage-clamp mode at −70 mV holding potential. Resting potentials (Vrest) were recorded in current-clamp mode at 0 pA holding current in the absence/presence of TTX (660 nm, Bio-Techne R&D Systems). TTX was added 10 min before recordings started and then it was continuously present in the aCSF bath solution. Measurements started with initial control recordings. Either PrRP 12-31 or NA (3.5 µm, #P7357 and 10 µm, #A9512, respectively, Merck) was added in single boluses into the recording chamber. When added together (PrRP + NA), NA was applied immediately after PrRP. Consecutive applications of NA and PrRP + NA were performed, when Vrest returned to the baseline. When the α2-adrenergic receptor antagonist yohimbine (2 µm, #1127, Bio-Techne) or the GABA(A) receptor antagonist picrotoxin (100 µm, #1128, Tocris) was applied, they were continuously present in the bath solution from the 10th min before NA administration. Slices were fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde overnight and processed for fluorescence detection of MCH and biocytin.

Brain surgeries

Implantation of EEG electrodes

Stainless-steel screw electrodes were implanted over the left frontal cortex (stereotaxic coordinates: 2.0 mm lateral to the midline and 2.0 mm anterior to bregma) and over the left parietal cortex (2.0 mm lateral to midline and 2.0 mm anterior to λ) for frontoparietal EEG recordings. An occipital electrode as a ground was also implanted. A pair of EMG electrodes (stainless-steel spring electrodes covered by silicon rubber) was placed into the neck muscles.

Intracerebroventricular (ICV) cannulation

A polyethylene guide cannula (0.58 mm inner diameter, Intramedic PE-50, Clay Adams, Thomas Scientific) was inserted into the right lateral ventricle under stereotaxic control (−0.8 mm caudal to bregma, 2.0 mm lateral to midline, 4.0 mm ventral to the surface of the skull). EEG electrodes were implanted at the same time, when needed. Rats were allowed to recover and were handled for at least 7 d.

EEG recording and analysis

The effects of ICV-injected saline and different doses of PrRP12-31 (1.6, 4.0, and 10 nmol/5 µl, Merck) on the sleep–wake stages were investigated. Injections were given to awake rats at the beginning of the light phase (lights on at 10:00 A.M.) in a crossover design with 3 d washouts between the treatments (n = 8). EEG recordings were compared among the different doses of PrRP and saline.

Rats were placed and kept individually during the study in glass recording chambers (35 × 35 × 40 cm), while attached to a flexible recording cable to allow free movement. The recorded EEG/EMG signals (Vital Recorder software, Kissei Comtec) were amplified (5000 and 10,000 times for EEG and EMG, respectively), and were subjected to analog filtering (<0.5 Hz and >60 Hz). Analog/digital conversion was performed at a sampling rate of 128 Hz. During the whole period of the recordings (24 h), rats were undisturbed, and had access to food and water ad libitum. Sleep-EEG analysis was performed in 10 s periods (epochs) using the automatic detection function of Sleep Sign for Animal software (Kissei) based on conventional criteria (Kantor et al., 2004) followed by visual assessment by experienced EEG scorers.

The following sleep–wake stages were differentiated; wakefulness: EEG signal is characterized by low-amplitude activity at β (14-29 Hz) and α (10-13 Hz) frequencies, with high EMG and intense locomotor activity; non-REM sleep (NREMS): EEG is characterized by high-amplitude slow cortical waves (0.5-4 Hz) interrupted by spindles (10-12 Hz), with reduced EMG activity and minimal motor activity; REM sleep (REMS): EEG is characterized by low-amplitude and high-frequency activity with regular theta waves (5-9 Hz), accompanied by silent EMG and motor activity with occasional twitching (Kantor et al., 2004). Power spectral analysis was computed for consecutive 10 s epochs at the frequency range of 0.5-60 Hz (fast Fourier transformation routine, Hanning window, frequency resolution: 0.25 Hz). Adjacent 0.25 Hz bins were summed into 1 Hz bins. Bins are marked by their upper limits (2 Hz refers to 1.25-2.00 Hz). The values of consecutive epochs, separately in wakefulness, NREMS, and REMS were averaged for the first 60 min after treatment. Epochs that contained artifacts or transition between sleep–wake stages were excluded from the power spectral analysis.

Sleep deprivation

REMS deprivation (SD) was performed using the flower pot method, as described previously (Vas et al., 2013). Rats (250-350 g) were sleep-deprived for 72 h starting at lights on (at 10:00 A.M.) on small (SP, diameter: 6.5 cm) or large platforms (LP, diameter: 13 cm) surrounded by water or kept in their home cages (HC) as controls. REMS is eliminated on SP because rats fell into the water because of muscle atony during REMS, whereas LP enables REMS. Thus, LP-kept rats serve as stress controls. Platform sizes and rat weights were chosen based on our previous work (Kitka et al., 2009) and other studies (Maloney et al., 1999; Verret et al., 2003; Machado et al., 2004) validating the method. After 72 h, subgroups of SD rats were allowed to have recovery sleep (2 h) (SPR, LPR) in their HCs.

Rats were killed and the brains were used for quantitative ISH (5 groups, n = 5/group) or IHC (5 groups, n = 3/group). Additional animals were used for EEG analysis of the recovery sleep (3 h, SPR and LPR groups, n = 6/group). Bodyweight change and food intake during the 72 h were measured for the cohort used for PrRP mRNA and protein analysis (n = 8/group). EEG recordings during the sleep recovery period were compared with the undisturbed baseline recordings of each rat (1-3 h after lights on, under the same conditions).

Measurement of PrRP mRNA expression levels in SD animals

Fresh frozen brains were cut into 12-µm-thick serial coronal sections in a cryostat, mounted onto Superfrost UltraPlus slides (Fisher Scientific), and stored at −80°C. PrRP mRNA was localized in the sections by radioactive ISH providing a linear relationship between the signal intensity and the mRNA expression level (Chen et al., 2012). Optical densities (mean gray values) of the individual nuclei were measured from the storage phosphor recordings in the A1 and A2 cell groups and between 14.04 and 14.70 mm, as well as in the DM between 3.36 and 4.08 mm caudal to bregma (Paxinos and Watson, 2007) with Image J 1.32j software. Data were determined in three and two consecutive sections in the brainstem and the hypothalamus, respectively, selected from the centers of the investigated areas. The same nuclei were evaluated by applying the same settings both across the sections and across the animals. Background values were measured in parallel and subtracted. In the A1 and DM, where optical densities showed significant differences among the groups, we determined mRNA expression levels also within the individual cells from darkfield microscopic images of autoradiographic emulsion-coated sections as described previously (Tóth et al., 2008). Microphotographs were captured through a 10× objective lens on an Olympus BX60 microscope (Olympus Hungary) from one representative section per animal (see Imaging). The silver grains were selected using the threshold tool in the ImageJ software. The amount of the silver grains over the individual cells was evaluated within identical ROIs and expressed as the area (pixels) covered by grains (Wittmann et al., 2015). In all cases, the signal threshold was set to be >3 times the mean value of two or three background samples measured in the surrounding region. All measurements were performed bilaterally.

Counting of PrRP-immunopositive cells in SD animals

PrRP-immunolabeled cells were visualized on serial coronal sections using DAB as chromogen. Microphotographs were captured through a 10× objective lens on an Olympus microscope (see Imaging). Cells were counted bilaterally in three sections showing the strongest signals in A1 and A2 cell groups, as well as in one or two sections in the DM using the touch count tool in the ImageJ 1.32j software.

Counting of MCH and cFos-immunopositive cells

Cells were visualized on serial coronal sections using Ni-DAB for cFos and DAB for MCH as chromogens. cFos, MCH, and double-labeled cells were counted bilaterally in four sections within 100 × 300 µm2 ROIs, placed over the PFA or LH using the touch count tool in the ImageJ 1.32j software.

LHe paradigm

The LHe was performed as described previously (Otrokocsi et al., 2017) with slight modifications. Automated soundproof shuttle-boxes interfaced with a computer running Med-PC IV software (Med Associates) were used for the experiment. The shuttle-boxes consisted of two identical compartments and were equipped with photobeam sensors, stimulus light, a tone generator, stainless-steel grid floor, and a guillotine door between the compartments. The paradigm consisted of a training and a test session. On the morning of the experimental day, rats were placed individually in the left or right compartment of the shuttle-box with the guillotine door closed and left to explore for 5 min. The training session lasted for 40 min. Rats in the shocked group (n = 10) received 120 trials of 5 s cue (light and tone stimuli) followed by an inescapable (closed door), uncontrollable electric foot shock at 0.4 mA with random duration ranging from 5 to 15 s and unpredictable intershock intervals. Control animals (n = 5) did not receive the cues and foot shocks but were otherwise treated similarly. The test session started 7 d later by returning the animals to their previously assigned compartment. All rats received 30 trials of 5 s cues during which the guillotine door was open, followed by an unexpected and escapable foot shock (0.4 mA intensity, 10 s duration with random intershock intervals). Escape during stimulus or escape during shock was scored if the animal crossed the door during cue presentation or the 10 s shock, respectively. Escape latency was measured. Escape failure was scored on the animal not crossing the door during the trial. Animals were classified as being LHe-resilient or LHe-susceptible (“helpless”) by applying the k-means clustering (k = 2) method (Marques et al., 2022). The analysis was applied on the escape failure and escape latency data pairs (n = 15). The results were validated by applying the mean + 2 SDs criterion for the escape failure data. Bodyweight and food intake were measured immediately before the training and the test sessions, so the bodyweight change, and food intake were calculated over the 7 d (between the two sessions). Fecal discharge during sessions was counted. Rats were decapitated 24 h after testing between 11:00 A.M. and 12:00 A.M. Trunk blood was collected, the plasma was separated by centrifugation and stored at −80°C for corticosterone RIA (Zelena et al., 2003). The brains were frozen and processed for microdissection and subsequent RT-PCR.

Microdissection of rat brain tissue

Fresh frozen brains were cut into 300-µm-thick slices in a cryostat and mounted on untreated glass slides (Fisher Scientific). The regions containing the DLH, DM, A1, and the A2 cell groups were identified using a head magnifier, with reference to the Rat Brain Atlas (Paxinos and Watson, 2007). The areas were removed from the sections bilaterally using sterile punch needles with an inside diameter of 0.7 mm. The sections were kept frozen in the cryostat throughout the whole procedure. The samples were collected in RNase/DNase-free tubes (Eppendorf) containing NucleoZOL (Macherey-Nagel), homogenized and stored at −80°C.

RIA

Corticosterone was measured in 10 µl unextracted plasma, as described previously (Zelena et al., 2003). Assay sensitivity was 1 pmol. The intra-assay coefficient of variation was 7.5%. All the samples were measured in one assay.

Lipopolysaccharide (LPS) treatments

Rats were treated with intraperitoneal injections of LPS (1 mg/BW kg, O55:B5, Merck) or pyrogen-free saline. Animals in Cohort 1 were killed for IHC (Table 1) 3 or 24 h (n = 3/group) and for RT-PCR 3 h (n = 5/group) after treatments. Cohort 2 was assessed in the forced swim test (FST) 24 h after LPS (LPS: n = 6, control: n = 5).

Quantitative evaluation of the immunofluorescence PrRP and MCH signals

Sections immunostained for PrRP and MCH from control and 24 h LPS-treated animals were selected and matched according to their rostro-caudal levels. Confocal images from the PFA and LH were acquired through a 10× lens objective (see Imaging). Measurements were performed using the ImageJ 1.32j software. The PrRP fibers were separated from the background using the threshold tool. Density of the fibers was determined as the area (pixels) covered by fibers within a 100 × 300 µm2 ROI, placed over the PFA or LH. The total corrected cellular fluorescence in the MCH-immunostained neurons was calculated by measuring the integrated density of the cell and subtracting the background (area of measured cell × mean fluorescence of background readings) (McCloy et al., 2014). The cell bodies were outlined, and the relevant parameters were measured along with three adjacent background readings using the same outline. Only in focus-plane cells (20 cells/image) were evaluated. Data obtained from bilateral measurements of two PrRP and one MCH-immunostained sections from each animal were used for the statistical analysis.

FST

Rats were placed individually in a cylinder (diameter: 18 cm, height: 61 cm) filled (up to 30 cm) with water at 27°C-28°C, during the light phase between 10:00 AM and 2:00 PM (Porsolt et al., 1978; Slattery and Cryan, 2012). Tests (6 min) were performed 24 h following a training session (15 min). PrRP (4 nmol) was given ICV to rats 30 min before the test session. LPS was given to rats in a separate experiment right after the training session. Behavior of the animals (n = 6 and n = 5 for treatment and control [saline] groups, respectively) was recorded by a camcorder and analyzed later by an observer blinded to the treatment groups using Solomon Coder software (https://solomoncoder.com). During the test, the time spent floating (immobility), struggling, and swimming was determined. Floating was scored when the general activity of the animal was minimized to occasional and small movements of legs or tail necessary to keep the nose above the water. Struggling was considered when the animal was permanently breaking the water surface by intense movements of forepaws usually directed against the walls. Swimming was defined as making active swimming motions, more than necessary to merely keep the head above water (Mlynarik et al., 2007). Immobility (floating) time (%) during the test was rated as passive coping behavior. Each animal was dried and returned to its cage after sessions. Bodyweight and food intake between the sessions were measured.

Human subjects and microdissection of human brain tissue

DLH tissue samples from postmortem control (n = 7) and suicidal (n = 8) male subjects were provided by the Human Brain Tissue Bank (Semmelweis University, Budapest, Hungary). We included only males in the study to avoid possible sex bias in the data (Tóth et al., 2008) and to be consistent with our animal studies. Clinical information and permission from the patient or the next of kin for the use of the brain tissue for research purposes had been previously received. The medical history of the subjects was obtained from medical or clinical records, and from pathologic and neuropathological reports. Demographic characteristics and available relevant clinical and neuropathological information of the cohort are provided in Table 2. The tissues were stored at −80°C until dissection and later kept frozen throughout the whole procedure. Tissue samples were removed from the brains at the rostral mammillary level from 1.0- to 1.5-mm-thick coronal sections cut by an electric slicer. The regions were identified using the human brain atlas (Mai et al., 2015) and a stereomicroscope with prechilled stage. The samples were removed by using sterile punch needles with inside diameter of 1.0 mm (Palkovits, 1973), collected in RNase/DNase-free tubes (Eppendorf) and stored immediately at −80°C and used for RT-PCR analyses.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2.

Demographic, clinical, and postmortem characteristics of the control and suicide subjectsa

Samples were collected in accordance with the Ethical Rules for Using Human Tissues for Medical Research in Hungary (HM 34/1999) and the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki) and approved by institutional ethics committees of the Semmelweis University. Human brain microdissection procedures were approved by the Regional and Institutional Committee of Science and Research Ethics of Scientific Council of Health (34/2002/TUKEB-13 716/2013/EHR, Budapest, Hungary) and the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki). The study was performed in the course of an approved study by the Ethical Committee of the Semmelweis University.

RT-PCR

Total RNA was isolated using a GeneJET RNA Purification Kit. RNA quality and quantity were determined with a NanoDrop 2000 Microvolume Spectrophotometer and in the human samples also by examining the ribosomal RNA bands on agarose gels. cDNAs were prepared using a High Capacity cDNA Reverse Transcription Kit. Amplifications were performed on an ABI StepOnePlus Real-Time PCR system using TaqMan Universal Master Mix II and commercially available TaqMan assays (Table 3). (All above were purchased from Fisher Scientific.) Expression levels were normalized to GAPDH in the rat samples and to acidic ribosomal protein P0 in the human samples. Samples were measured in technical parallels. Relative fold changes were calculated by the comparative CT method (2−ΔΔCt). Amplifications were not detected in the no-template and a reverse transcriptase minus reactions.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 3.

Catalog numbers of the Fisher Scientific TaqMan assays used for RT-PCRsa

Imaging

Light microscopic examinations were performed using an Olympus BX60 microscope equipped with a SPOTXplorer digital CCD camera (Diagnostic Instruments). Pictures were taken using UPlanFL 4×/0.13, 10×/0.30, or 20×/0.50 objectives. Confocal images were acquired on an LSM 780 confocal laser-scanning microscope (Zeiss). For PrRP and MCH quantitative measurements, pictures were captured using a 10×/0.45 M27 objective with pinhole size 6.5 and 1 AU, respectively. MCH coexpression with biocytin was investigated using 63×/1.4 Oil DIC M27 objective with pinhole size 1.47 AU. For the quantitative analysis of PrRP and TH coexpression and for the illustrative pictures of the MCH neurons, single optical sections were captured using a Plan-Apochromat 63×/1.4 Oil DIC M27 objective with z separation of 3 and 1 µm, respectively. In case of the MCH staining, the stacks were z-projected in the ImageJ 1.32j software. Contrast and sharpness of the illustrative images were adjusted in Adobe Photoshop CS 8.0. Multipanel figures were assembled in Adobe Illustrator CS3.

Experimental design and statistical analysis

The statistical evaluations were performed using Sigmastat 3.5 application (Systat Software). The size of the experimental groups was determined based on the literature of the field and our previous experience. Each experiment presented in the paper was repeated in multiple animals (n = 3-8/groups, see relevant sections). In all experiments, relevant animals were allocated to experimental groups randomly. Data analysis was performed blinded to experimental groups in all cases where the results would have been biased otherwise (in case of more than one treatment groups and manual data analysis). Statistical significance was accepted when p < 0.05. Data are expressed as mean ± SEM, except ΔCt data that are shown using box-and-whiskers plots. Paired or unpaired Student's t tests (two-sided) were used for the comparison of two groups with normal distribution of data; otherwise, we applied Mann–Whitney U statistics. When more than two groups were compared, one- or two-way ANOVA or repeated-measures ANOVA were conducted, depending on the experimental setup. Pearson's correlation was used to examine correlations with normal distribution of the data.

Results

Both medullary and hypothalamic PrRP neurons project to the DLH

The interaction between PrRP and MCH neurons requires an appropriate anatomic basis; therefore, we first performed morphologic studies. Our IHC findings revealed that PrRP+ axonal terminals distribute within the DLH. Numerous PrRP+ axonal terminals established close appositions with MCH+ neurons. This was seen mostly within two areas of the DLH, namely, the PFA and the LH (Fig. 1A). Confocal microscopy analysis confirmed the catecholamine content (TH positivity) of the medullary PrRP cells, which were located in the A1 and A2 cell groups. In A2, they were distributed in the nucleus of the solitary tract (NTS) (Chen et al., 1999; Tóth et al., 2008). PrRP neurons in the DM were, however, negative for TH (Fig. 1B). MCH-, PrRP-, and TH-triple-labeled sections showed the existence of intimate contacts between the A1/A2- and DM-originated PrRP+ varicosities and MCH+ neurons (Fig. 1C, left). When counting 711 and 579 PrRP+ varicosities in the PFA and LH, respectively, ∼60% of the varicosities contained TH in both investigated areas (Fig. 1C,D).

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Anatomical relationship between PrRP and MCH neurons in the DLH. A, Double IHC. Top left, Dense PrRP fiber (blue) network surrounds the MCH cells (brown) mainly in the PFA and LH. Right, The PFA and the LH (framed on A) at higher magnification. Bottom left, Multiple PrRP+ axonal varicosities contact the perikarya and dendrites of MCH neurons. B, Noradrenergic and non-noradrenergic PrRP cell populations. Top, Location of the PrRP cells (red) in the hypothalamic DM, and in the A1 and A2 (within the NTS) NA cell groups. Double immunolabeling of TH (red) and PrRP (green) producing cells confirms the catecholamine content of medullary PrRP cells (yellow) (Chen et al., 1999; Tóth et al., 2008). PrRP cells in the DM lack TH. C, Confocal images of triple-labeled sections. Left, Both TH– (top) and TH+ (green, bottom) PrRP+ terminals (magenta) form close contacts with MCH (blue) neurons. Right, TH– (circles) and TH+ (arrowheads) axonal varicosities of PrRP+ fibers were analyzed in separate channels. D, Quantitative analysis of the different types of PrRP+ profiles in the DLH. Error bars indicate mean ± SEM. Two-way ANOVA, TH: F(1,70) = 22.46, p < 0.001. Student-Newman-Keuls multiple comparison test, TH+ versus TH–: *p = 0.001, n = 20 (PFA) and *p = 0.002, n = 17 (LH). E, Top left, Location of cells expressing PrRP receptors (PrRPRs): NPFF2R (closed circles) and PRLHR (open circles). Bottom, NPFF2R-expressing cells in the LH and DM as well as PRLHR-expressing cells in the PFA detected by radioactive ISH. The autoradiography shows black silver grain conglomerations over the positive cells. The sections were counterstained by Giemsa (blue). Top right, MCH- and orexin-immunopositive neurons (red arrows) do not express PRLHR and NPFF2R (black arrows). f, Fornix; ot, optic tract; 3v, third ventricle. The schematic drawings of the coronal sections are from the Paxinos atlas (Paxinos and Watson, 2007).

To see whether MCH neurons express PrRP receptors, we performed ISH combined with IHC. PRLHR mRNA-expressing cells were restricted to the PFA. NPFF2R, which mediates some effects of PrRP (Ma et al., 2009), was detected in the dorsal periventricular region in the DM and in a narrow field of the rostral LH adjacent to the optic tract (Fig. 1E, left and bottom). Importantly, the receptor-expressing cells were negative for MCH, which excludes the possibility that the intimate contacts between PrRP+ and MCH+ neuronal elements are direct synaptic contacts. Orexin cells, which are also a major cell population of the DLH (Kitka et al., 2011; Vas et al., 2013), did not express PrRP receptors either (Fig. 1E, top, right).

ICV PrRP injection promotes sleep

Since there is a strong relationship between stress, sleep, and mood (van Dalfsen and Markus, 2018; Steiger and Pawlowski, 2019), and the DLH is a critical site of sleep–wakefulness regulation (Bonnavion et al., 2016), next we examined the effect of ICV PrRP administration on the sleep–wake cycle. PrRP (0, 1.6, 4, and 10 nmol) injection at the beginning of the passive phase (Fig. 2A) elicited an acute sleep-promoting effect by increasing the amount of NREMS and decreasing NREMS latency. Both effects ceased within 30 min and were significant with the higher doses of PrRP only (Fig. 2B,C). Lower doses of PrRP (1.6 and 4.0 nmol) also affected the quantitative EEG spectra during NREMS by reducing the power of the δ (1-4 Hz) frequency range (Fig. 2D). The amount of wakefulness diminished gradually along with the increasing doses of PrRP (Fig. 2E). The amount of REMS was nearly zero in the short period of PrRP's effects; therefore, the effects of PrRP on REMS and REMS latency could not be investigated using this experimental setup (Fig. 2F,G).

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Effects of PrRP on EEG parameters. A, Schematic and timeline of the experiment. Frontoparietal EEG was recorded on the left (red). Different doses of PrRP, or vehicle (saline), were injected intracerebroventricularly (ICV) through a chronically implanted cannula (blue) into the right ventricle, at the beginning of passive (light) phase using a crossover design. Gray squares represent the days when treatments and recordings were performed. B, The amount of NREMS. Zero is the time point of the drug administration. One-way repeated-measures ANOVA 0-30 min, F(3,21) = 5.14, p = 0.008. C, NREMS latency. One-way repeated-measures ANOVA, F(3,21) = 6.56, p = 0.003. D, EEG δ power (1-4 Hz) during the first hour after injections. Two-way repeated-measures ANOVA, treatment × frequency interaction: F(9,63) = 2.86, p = 0.007. E, Time spent awake. One-way repeated-measures ANOVA, F(3,21) = 5.09, p = 0.008. F, The amount and (G) the latency of REMS. For post hoc analysis, Bonferroni multiple comparison tests were performed. *p < 0.05 versus vehicle; n = 8/group. Error bars indicate mean ± SEM.

PrRP expression is associated with sleep–wake stages

To challenge MCH neurons that are known to be involved in promoting REMS (Bandaru et al., 2020), we used the flower pot SD paradigm, which eliminates REMS completely, and enhances the REMS pressure (i.e., the need for REMS, Fig. 3A) (Maloney et al., 1999; Verret et al., 2003; Machado et al., 2004). During the recovery sleep following 72 h SD, REMS was recovered in the SPR rats, by producing twice as much REMS compared with the baseline, whereas the amount of NREMS decreased. The LPR group revealed no change in the sleep parameters versus the baseline (Fig. 3B).

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

PrRP expression varies with the amount of sleep. A, Schematic of SD method. Rats were placed individually on round platforms surrounded by water. REMS is eliminated on SP because rats fell into the water because of muscle atony during REMS, whereas LP enables REMS. Thus, LP-kept rats serve as stress controls (Maloney et al., 1999; Machado et al., 2004; Kitka et al., 2009). B, Sleep parameters during the recovery sleep. The amount of NREMS decreases, while the amount of REMS increases significantly in the SP rebound (SPR), but not in the LP rebound (LPR) group, compared with baseline recorded for each rat. Two-way repeated-measures ANOVA, vigilance state × pot interaction: F(2,15) = 31.6, p < 0.0001, NREMS *p < 0.0001 and REMS *p = 0.0002; n = 6/group. C, Food intake (top) and bodyweight change (bottom) during SD. Kruskal–Wallis test, H2 = 17.19, p < 0.001, *p < 0.05 versus HC; n = 8/group. D, PrRP mRNA expression levels in the DM, the A1 and A2 (NTS) cell groups. Graphs represent the quantitative results measured as optical densities of the ISH signals. DM: one-way ANOVA, F(4,20) = 4.96, p = 0.006, SP versus HC *p < 0.01, SP versus all the other groups #p ≤ 0.01. A1 cell group: one-way ANOVA, F(4,20) = 4.16, p = 0.013, all groups versus HC *p < 0.05. NTS: no alterations were detected, n = 5/group. E, Number of PrRP-immunopositive cells. DM: one-way ANOVA, F(4,10) = 3.54, p = 0.048, SP versus SPR and LPR #p < 0.05, SP versus HC and LP p = 0.053 and p = 0.061, respectively. A1 cell group: one-way ANOVA, F(4,10) = 28.11, p < 0.001, all groups versus HC *p < 0.001. There was no change in the NTS. n = 3/group. Error bars indicate mean ± SEM. For multiple comparisons, the Holm–Sidak method (B) and the Student-Newman-Keuls (C–E) test were used.

Despite consuming a similar amount of food to HC rats (Fig. 3C, top), both SP and LP rats lost weight compared with HC animals (Fig. 3C, bottom). This effect was independent of the size of the platforms.

The SP- but not the LP-induced SD increased the amount of PrRP mRNA in the DM, and this was normalized during recovery (Fig. 3D). Additionally, both SP and LP conditions elevated PrRP mRNA levels remarkably in the A1 cell group, which remained high during the recovery sleep. Neither the SP- nor the LP-induced SD altered the expression of PrRP mRNA in the NTS (Fig. 3D). PrRP protein levels showed parallel alterations in the relevant experimental groups to the PrRP mRNA levels (Figs. 3E, 4A). Measurements at the cellular level showed that changes in the expression of PrRP mRNA in both the DM (Fig. 4A) and the A1 cell group (Fig. 4B) were because of the elevated expression of the PrRP mRNA within the individual cells.

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Cellular changes after SD. A, B, Detection of PrRP mRNA and protein in the cells in the DM and the A1 cell group, respectively. Top, left, Darkfield pictures: autoradiography of PrRP mRNA-labeled cells. The density of the silver grains (white spots) over the positive cells changes in proportion to the amount of mRNA. Bottom, left, Immunohistochemical detection of PrRP+ neurons. Scale bars, 100 µm. Right, Graphs represent the results of the mRNA level measurements at cellular level. Data are expressed as the area covered by silver grains over the individual cells. One-way ANOVA, DM: F(4,24) = 10.53, p < 0.001, A1: F(4,24) = 40.15, p < 0.001, Student-Newman-Keuls multiple comparison test, *p < 0.001 versus HC, #p < 0.001 versus all other groups; n = 5/group. C, PrRP (4 nmol/5 µl) or saline (5 µl) was administered ICV into SP-deprived rats prior of recovery sleep. D, MCH (blue), cFos (red), and PrRP (green) triple fluorescence IHC showed close contacts between PrRP+ fibers and MCH+ neurons, which were activated by REMS rebound. E, Percentages of MCH+ and non-MCH+ neurons exhibiting cFos signal in the PFA and LH in animals treated with PrRP, or saline. Two-way ANOVA, effect of treatment: F(1,16) = 28.73, p < 0.001; n = 5/group. Student-Newman-Keuls multiple comparison test, *p < 0.01 versus saline. Error bars indicate mean ± SEM.

PrRP inhibits REMS-active MCH neurons in vivo

Our flower pot experimental data provided evidence for the active participation of the endogenous PrRP in the sleep–wake regulation. Additionally, it provided a useful experimental setup for the functional investigation of the morphologically established circuits between PrRP and MCH neurons. Evoking rebound sleep after SD by the flower pot method is a powerful way to activate the majority of MCH neurons in the DLH, which can be detected by cFos IHC (Verret et al., 2003; Kitka et al., 2011). To see whether PrRP affects the function of REMS-active MCH neurons, SP rats were injected with PrRP (4 nmol, ICV), or vehicle just before recovery sleep (Fig. 4C). We found that PrRP fibers frequently formed close contacts with cFos+/MCH+ neurons (Fig. 4D). In the PrRP-treated rats, the ratio of cFos+/MCH+ neurons was significantly lower compared with controls in both the PFA and LH (Fig. 4E, left). The percentage of other types of cFos+ cells was unaffected by the PrRP treatment (Fig. 4E, right).

PrRP hyperpolarizes MCH neurons and greatly increases the effect of NA ex vivo

To characterize the observed effect of PrRP on MCH neurons, we performed whole-cell patch-clamp experiments using brain slices from 30-d-old rats. At this age, the MCH neuronal system is fully developed (Risold et al., 2009), and the MCH cell population appears similar to those in adults (Fig. 5A, left). MCH neurons in the PFA (Fig. 5A, right) were identified by their characteristic electrophysiological responses to current steps (Fig. 5B) as follows: (1) adaptation of firing during the depolarizing step; (2) absence of spontaneous spikes at resting state; (3) absence of rebound depolarization at the end of the hyperpolarizing step; and (4) absence of sag at the beginning of the hyperpolarizing step (van den Pol et al., 2004). Identity of the measured MCH neurons was also confirmed by post hoc IHC (Fig. 5C).

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

PrRP inhibits the MCH neurons. A, Left, Immunohistochemical detection of MCH neurons in 30-d-old rats. Right, Schematics (Paxinos and Watson, 2007) of the brain slice preparation for patch-clamp recording. Acute coronal slices containing the hypothalamus were used with the thalamus, and the cortex removed. Neurons in the PFA were patched using a biocytin (bi) filled electrode. B, Electrophysiological identification of the MCH neurons was performed by their response to current steps (van den Pol et al., 2004). C, Identity of the measured cells was confirmed by post hoc IHC. D, PrRP (3.5 µm) elicits a transient outward current in MCH neurons in the absence of TTX. Student's t test (paired), t = 14.745, df = 5, *p = 0.000. E, Membrane potential changes (ΔVrest) of MCH neurons in response to PrRP in the absence of TTX. Student's t test (paired), t = 9.785, df = 5, *p = 0.000. F, There is no response to PrRP in the presence of TTX. G, ΔVrest in response to repeated administration of NA (10 µm, dashed arrows), and after pretreatment with PrRP (solid arrow) in the absence (left) and presence of TTX (right). H, Graphs summarize the effects of PrRP on the NA-induced responses. One-way repeated-measures ANOVA. ΔVrest no TTX: F(2,10) = 32.6, p < 0.001, Holm–Sidak multiple comparison tests, PrRP+NA versus first and second NA, *p < 0.0001. I, ΔVrest in response to NA in the absence (–) and presence (+) of the α2-adrenergic receptor antagonist yohimbine (2 µm, horizontal line above the recording). Student's t test, t = 8.900, df = 5, *p = 0.000. J, ΔVrest in response to repeated administration of NA (dashed arrows), and after pretreatment with PrRP (solid arrow) in the presence of the GABA(A) receptor antagonist picrotoxin (100 µm) and absence of TTX. K, Graphs summarize the effects shown in J. Error bars indicate mean ± SEM. ncells = 6 for all except ncells = 4 in F. One or two cells/animal were measured in independent experiments.

PrRP (3.5 µm) evoked an outward current in the MCH cells, starting within 15 s after its application (Imax: 36.06 ± 2.45 pA, duration: 28.00 ± 4.67 s) (Fig. 5D). PrRP hyperpolarized MCH neurons in the absence of the voltage-gated Na-channel blocker TTX with a 4.66 ± 0.48 mV change in the resting potential (ΔVrest) and with a duration of 27.42 ± 3.71 s (Fig. 5E). This effect was not observed in the presence of TTX (Fig. 5F).

We also investigated the interaction between PrRP and NA. As expected (van den Pol et al., 2004), NA (10 µm) hyperpolarized the patched MCH neuron repeatedly (Fig. 5G, left, Fig. 5H). When PrRP (3.5 µm) was added just before the third application of NA, the response was almost doubled. Meanwhile, the mean durations of the triggered Vrest changes were unaltered (Fig. 5G, left, Fig. 5H). In the presence of TTX (Fig. 5G, right, Fig. 5H), the response to NA was similar to that observed in the absence of TTX. PrRP did not modify the NA-triggered response in the presence of TTX (Fig. 5G, right, Fig. 5H). The response to NA was completely blocked in the presence of the α2-adrenergic receptor antagonist yohimbine (2 µm, Fig. 5I) (van den Pol et al., 2004).

Next, we used the same experimental setup to identify the cell type mediating the inhibitory effect of PrRP on MCH neurons. This time, we used bath-applied picrotoxin (100 µm), a GABA(A) receptor antagonist. We assumed that PrRP acts via inhibitory interneurons in the PFA, because PRLHR is a Gq-coupled receptor (Dodd and Luckman, 2013) and is highly expressed by GABAergic cells in the reticular nucleus of the thalamus (Lin et al., 2002). Indeed, whereas NA elicited similar responses in MCH neurons than previously, PrRP failed to modify the effect of NA in the presence of picrotoxin and absence of TTX (Fig. 5J,K). Together, our findings revealed a presynaptic, GABA(A) receptor mediated, inhibitory effect of PrRP on MCH neurons, which substantially adds to the postsynaptic action of NA. The presence of close contacts between MCH neurons and PrRP varicosities may therefore suggest that PrRP receptors are located at the GABAergic terminals innervating MCH cells.

Overload of PrRP neurons underlines increased passive coping in FST

To determine whether previous exposure to PrRP (“artificial stress”) affects subsequent coping with stress, we administered ICV PrRP to rats 30 min before subjecting them to FST (Fig. 6A, Experiment 1). The effect of ICV PrRP on ACTH release ceases after 10 min (Seal et al., 2002), in line with the effects of PrRP on the EEG, which worn off within 30 min. Post-PrRP animals showed increased immobility (“floating”) and decreased active coping (“struggling”) behavior compared with the control group (Fig. 6B). The percentage of time spent swimming was unaltered (saline: 20.2 ± 4.5%; PrRP: 18.0 ± 4.5%).

Figure 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 6.

Evaluation of the inflammation-induced depression model. A, Timelines of the experiments. LPS was added intraperitoneally to induce inflammation. PrRP was injected intracerebroventricularly. Control rats received saline. B, C, Exp. 1. and 2, respectively: Student's t-tests, df = 9, saline: n = 5, treated: n = 6. Percentage of the time spent passively (“floating”) and actively (“struggling”) in the FST 30 min after (B) PrRP (floating: *p = 0.036, struggling: *p = 0.035, t = −2.459 and t = 2.484, respectively) or 24 h after (C) LPS treatments (floating: *p = 0.034, struggling: *p = 0.030, t = −2.499 and t = 2.576, respectively). D, Bodyweight change (t = 5.213, *p = 0.001) and (E) food intake (p = 0.056) during the 24 h after the LPS injections. F, PrRP cell (brown) activation 3 h after LPS treatment. The cFos nuclear signal (black) appears in the activated neurons. The inserts show PrRP neurons at higher magnification. DMX: dorsal motor nucleus of the vagus nerve. G, Exp. 3: Student's t-tests, df = 8, n = 5/group. Relative mRNA levels of TNFα (*p = 0.001, t = 37.153, measured in the A2 cell group), PrRP (DM: *p = 0.001, t = 4.759, A1: *p = 0.008, t = 3.501, NTS: *p = 0.005, t = 3.814), PrRP receptors (PRLHR and NPFF2R: *p = 0.02, t = −2.898 and t = −2.890, respectively) and pro-MCH (*p = 0.001, t = −6.854) 3 h post-LPS treatment. H, Exp. 3: Student's t-tests, df = 4, n = 3/group. PrRP immunopositivity in DLH axons 24 h after treatments. The image illustrates the PFA. The bar graph shows the density of PrRP+ fibers in the area (PFA: *p = 0.021, LH: *p = 0.034, t = 3.675 and t = 3.168). I, MCH immunopositive cells in the PFA 24 h after treatments. The arrowheads point to axonal processes. The bar graph shows the total cellular fluorescence measured in the MCH neurons, Student's t-test, t = –3.989 df = 4, *p = 0.016, n = 3/group, error bars indicate mean ± SEM.

Next, we used LPS treatment to induce inflammatory stress in rats (Fig. 6A, Experiment 2). LPS has long-term effects, first triggering a surge of pro-inflammatory cytokines and sickness behavior, leading to the development of “depression-like” behavior in the late phase (24 h) (Frenois et al., 2007; Dantzer, 2009). Compared with controls, LPS-treated rats spent more time “floating” and less time “struggling” in the FST 24 h after the treatment (Fig. 6C), with no alteration in the percentage of time spent swimming (saline: 30.4 ± 4.6%, LPS: 22.9 ± 7.0%). The LPS-treated rats lost weight (Fig. 6D), although their 24 h food intake (Fig. 6E) was not significantly different from controls.

We then investigated the LPS-induced effects on the brain (Fig. 6A, Experiment 3). A high number of PrRP cells in the A1 and A2 cell groups were cFos+ in the LPS-treated animals 3 h after injection (Fig. 6F). At the same time, the mRNA expression of the pro-inflammatory cytokine, TNFα, was strongly upregulated by LPS in the A2 cell group (Fig. 6G). The expression of PrRP mRNA was also increased in all PrRP-producing cell groups, while the expression of PrRP receptors and pro-MCH was decreased in DLH (Fig. 6G). One day following LPS treatment, PrRP immunopositivity was severely depleted in axons in the PFA and LH (Fig. 6H). Meanwhile, MCH protein levels were elevated in the neuronal perikarya (Fig. 6I).

Normal PrRP signaling in the DLH may be protective against the development of LHe

To further examine the role of PrRP in regulation of mood, we applied the LHe paradigm (Fig. 7A). Helpless behavior developed in 50% of the stressed rats (LHe-susceptible rats). This was clearly indicated by the number of missed escapes (Fig. 7B) and the escape latency (Fig. 7C). The escape behavior of LHe-resilient animals was normal.

Figure 7.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 7.

Rats with helpless behavior show altered PrRP and MCH signaling. A, Schematic of the LHe paradigm. During the training, one group of animals is exposed to a series of uncontrollable and inescapable foot shock (two-chamber cage, door closed). During the test, all animals receive a series of foot shock, with prior light and sound cues provided while the door opens. B, Number of missed escapes, F(2,12) = 27.02, p < 0.001. *p < 0.001 versus control. #p < 0.001 versus LHe-resilient. C, Delay to escape, F(2,12) = 16.06, p < 0.001. *p < 0.001 versus control. #p = 0.002 versus LHe-resilient. D, Number of fecal pellets, F(2,12) = 137.2, p < 0.001. *p < 0.001 versus control. E, Corticosterone levels 24 h after the test session, F(2,12) = 6.82, p = 0.011. *p = 0.016, *p = 0.01 versus control, for LHe-resilient and LHe-susceptible, respectively. F, Bodyweight change and (G) food intake between the training and the test. H–J, Relative mRNA levels 1 d after the test. H, PrRP: DM: F(2,12) = 6.04, p = 0.015. *p = 0.025 versus control. #p = 0.012 versus LHe-resilient. A1 cell group: F(2,12) = 16.82, p < 0.001. *p < 0.004, *p = 0.001 versus control for LHe-resilient and LHe-susceptible rats, respectively. p = 0.053 LHe-resilient versus LHe-susceptible. NTS: F(2,12) = 3.29, p = 0.073. I, PrRP receptors in the DLH: PRLHR: F(2,12) = 5.14, p = 0.024. *p = 0.028 versus control. #p = 0.029 versus LHe-resilient. NPFF2R: F(2,12) = 7.24, p = 0.009. *p = 0.015 versus control. #p = 0.009 versus LHe-resilient. J, pro-MCH: F(2,12) = 8.63, p = 0.005. *p = 0.006 versus control. &p = 0.006 versus LHe-susceptible. Statistics for RT-PCR were calculated from the ΔCt values. Error bars indicate mean ± SEM; n = 5/group. One-way ANOVA and Student-Newman-Keuls multiple comparison test were used for all statistics.

Fecal discharge is a reliable marker of acute stress in rats (Bindra and Thompson, 1953). LHe-susceptible and LHe-resilient animals produced an equally high number of fecal pellets because of the inescapable shock received during the “training.” Nonshocked controls showed no signs of acute stress (Fig. 7D). The escapable shock received 1 week later (“test”) was a moderate level of acute stress for all animals equally (Fig. 7D).

Blood corticosterone concentrations were elevated 24 h after the test session in both the LHe-susceptible and LHe-resilient rats, indicating that the training induced chronic activation of the HPA axis (Fig. 7E). The level of chronic stress was mild, reflected by the comparable bodyweight gain (Fig. 7F) and food intake (Fig. 7G) among the groups. Importantly, although HPA activity between the LHe-susceptible and LHe-resilient rats was similar, helpless behavior developed only in the LHe-susceptible rats.

The relative expression of PrRP mRNA in the DM doubled specifically in LHe-susceptible rats (Fig. 7H). In the A1 cell group, PrRP mRNA level was remarkably elevated in both LHe-susceptible and LHe-resilient rats, with a near-significant difference between the two groups. The relative expression of PrRP mRNA was statistically unchanged in the NTS (Fig. 7H). Meanwhile, PRLHR and NPFF2R expressions in the LH and PFA were considerably downregulated exclusively in the LHe-susceptible group (Fig. 7I), suggesting that the development of helpless behavior could definitely be linked to the malfunction of the PrRP system in the DLH. Along with this, the expression level of pro-MCH mRNA was markedly downregulated in the LHe-resilient group, but not in the LHe-susceptible group compared with nonshocked controls (Fig. 7J), pointing to a possible link between PrRP signaling and MCH function.

PrRP receptor expression is downregulated in suicidal individuals

To support the potential translational relevance of our data obtained in animal models, we measured the relative mRNA expressions of PRLHR and NPFF2R in postmortem tissue samples from suicidal and control males. Samples were obtained from the lateral hypothalamic area (LHA) and dorsal hypothalamic area (DHA) (Fig. 8A), which correspond to the LH and PFA in rats, respectively (Krolewski et al., 2010). Both PrRP receptors were expressed in the investigated areas with a predominance of NPFF2R mRNA. We detected a remarkable downregulation of both receptors in the suicidal group (Fig. 8B). We note that suicidal individuals were younger than controls in case of LHA samples (Table 2); however, there was no correlation between the ages and ΔCt values of the subjects (Table 4). We also measured the expression of another housekeeping gene, GAPDH, in the samples to verify the selectivity of our results. The expression levels of GAPDH did not differ between the control and suicidal groups in any of the investigated areas (Fig. 8C).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 4.

Correlation analysis data between mRNA levels of the PrRP receptors (ΔCt values) and the age of human subjectsa

Figure 8.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 8.

Relative mRNA expression of PrRP receptors in the LHA and DHA in control and suicidal human subjects. A, Schematic of the right human hypothalamus (Mai et al., 2015). White circles represent the size and location of the areas within the LHA (blue) and DHA (beige), from which tissue samples were taken for RT-PCR. f, Fornix; ot, optic tract; 3v, third ventricle. B, RT-PCR data (ΔCt and fold change) of PrRP receptors. LHAreceptors: F(1,10) = 27.71, p < 0.001. #p < 0.001 versus PRLHR within both groups and LHAgroups: F(1,10) = 14.70, p = 0.003. *p = 0.048 versus controls within both receptors. DHAreceptors: F(1,10) = 23.05, p < 0.001. #p < 0.001 versus PRLHR within both groups and DHAgroups: F(1,10) = 12.51, p = 0.05. *p = 0.023 and *p = 0.005 versus controls within PRLHR and NPFF2R, respectively. C, RT-PCR data of GAPDH. There is no significant difference between the control and suicidal samples. Two-way repeated-measures ANOVA: B, n = 6/group; C, n = 5 or 6/group. One control DHA sample was excluded for technical reasons. Statistics for all RT-PCR data were calculated using the ΔCt values. Symbols label the results of the Student-Newman-Keuls multiple comparison tests. Bar graphs represent mean ± SEM. Box (indicating the first and third quartiles) and whisker (extending to the minimum and maximum data points) plots are used for showing ΔCt data.

Discussion

Our study points to the potential involvement of the PrRP system in the pathophysiology of stress-related mental disorders. We also show a link between failures of the PrRP signaling and dysregulation of the MCH system, known to be involved in the pathomechanism of depression, anxiety, and PTSD (Chaki et al., 2005; Lagos et al., 2011; Torterolo et al., 2015; Concetti et al., 2020). We find downregulation of PrRP receptors in DLH of suicidal individuals, highlighting the possible clinical relevance of our findings.

Emotional brain function is causally and bidirectionally related to sleep (Goldstein and Walker, 2014). Most antidepressants impact sleep, and SD may elicit antidepressant effects (Steiger and Pawlowski, 2019). Thus, getting a better understanding of common mechanisms that regulate both sleep and mood has a fundamental importance. PrRP applied at the beginning of the passive phase produced a brief NREMS-promoting effect against the high sleep pressure in awake rats, in concordance with Zhang et al. (2001). One limitation of our study is that we might have detected a longer-lasting effect of PrRP if it had been administered during the active phase, when sleep propensity is low. Interestingly, the increase in NREMS was accompanied by a decrease in δ power. Similar effects were observed after treatment with diazepam, a classic benzodiazepine drug that acts as a nonselective agonist on different GABA(A) receptor subtypes and has a subtype-specific spatial distribution (Rudolph et al., 2001; Kopp et al., 2004). Our data thus suggest that the effects of PrRP are mediated, at least in part, by GABA. Consistent with this, PrRP inhibited MCH neurons through a GABA(A) receptor-mediated mechanism in our experiments. In addition, PrRP also acts on GABAergic cells in the reticular nucleus of the thalamus (Lin et al., 2002).

Our ex and in vivo results suggest that PrRP inhibits MCH neurons and substantially adds to the inhibitory effect of NA on MCH neurons. This is in line with earlier data reporting the cooperation of PrRP and NA to elicit ACTH release (Maruyama et al., 2001). The release of neuropeptides during neuronal firing varies with time and depends also on the firing pattern of the neuron (Cropper et al., 2018). Therefore, it is possible that the PrRP to NA ratio changes in the proximity of the MCH neurons depending on the actual activity of PrRP and PrRP-NA neurons. We hypothesize that PrRP may be an ideal molecule to fine-tune the activity of MCH neurons, by enabling and maintaining it within appropriate limits.

The expression of neuropeptides is dynamic, and their release is followed by de novo synthesis in the cell body (Hokfelt et al., 2000). We identified a PrRP cell population in the DM, in which the expression of PrRP was upregulated (suggesting release) by SD when most of the MCH neurons are usually inhibited (van den Pol et al., 2004; Blanco-Centurion et al., 2016). However, MCH neurons are critical to promote REMS, and the number of actively firing MCH cells increases over time during NREMS and peaks during REMS (Blanco-Centurion et al., 2019). The active state of MCH neurons during REMS recovery (Verret et al., 2003; Hassani et al., 2009) was associated with the downregulation of PrRP in the DM in our study. Together with the fact that ICV PrRP injection reduced the number of cFos+ MCH neurons during REMS recovery, and the number of cFos+ MCH neurons correlates with the number of REMS episodes (Kitka et al., 2011), it is likely that PrRP is involved in the inhibition of REMS under certain challenged conditions. Since REMS-active MCH neurons regulate hippocampus-dependent forgetting of contextual fear-related memories (Izawa et al., 2019), PrRP may influence the development of stress-related mental disorders associated with hippocampal dysfunction by modulating MCH activity (Cominski et al., 2014; Wingenfeld and Wolf, 2014). In addition, the majority of REMS-active MCH neurons are also active during exploratory behavior (Blanco-Centurion et al., 2019), which changes with anxiety (Heinz et al., 2021). Further, PrRP expression was elevated in the A1 cell group in all treatment groups in the SD experiment, suggesting a stress-related reaction on the platforms. This is in harmony with the key role of the A1 cell group in the activation of the HPA axis (Buller et al., 2001; Tóth et al., 2008) that essentially modulates sleep as well (Buckley and Schatzberg, 2005).

We postulated the involvement of PrRP in the development of repeated/chronic stress-evoked mental disorders based on different types of animal models. In the FST experiment, we activated the PrRP neurons (Dodd and Luckman, 2013) 30 min after the ICV administration of PrRP. The PrRP receptors were presumably inactivated by the time of the test, since GPCRs, like receptors of PrRP, undergo a ligand-stimulated desensitization/internalization (Luttrell and Lefkowitz, 2002; Madsen et al., 2012). This happens within several minutes in vivo, rendering the cell to be insensitive for hours (Scherrer et al., 2006). Indeed, the acute effects of ICV PrRP were no longer detectable than 30 min (Seal et al., 2002), and previous exposure to PrRP led to impaired running stress-stimulated ACTH release (Ohiwa et al., 2007). This experiment, therefore, directly linked previous exposure to PrRP to increased passive coping, suggesting that PrRP dysfunction may underlie the development of stress disorders.

We observed a depletion of PrRP immunoreactivity in axons innervating the DLH 24 h after LPS injection in the LPS-induced depression model (Frenois et al., 2007; Dantzer, 2009). There had been a prolonged PrRP release in the area: we detected downregulation of PrRP receptors in the DLH, and upregulation of PrRP expression (Hokfelt et al., 2000; Luttrell and Lefkowitz, 2002), while most of the medullary PrRP cells were still cFos-positive 3 h after LPS injection, indicating active firing of neurons (Barth et al., 2004). Others reported that cFos positivity of neurons in the medulla was detected up to 6 h following LPS treatment (Frenois et al., 2007). After an initial decrease, MCH immunoreactivity increased 24 h after treatment. This experiment also confirmed that impaired PrRP signaling may be associated with behavioral despair.

The role of neuroinflammation in the pathophysiology stress disorders has been highlighted recently (Echeverria et al., 2016). PrRP-NA neurons in the NTS represent the first interface between the vagus-mediated peripheral signals in the brain, as vagal afferents innervate most noradrenergic cells directly (Appleyard et al., 2007). The importance of the vagus-mediated peripheral signals is further supported by the finding that stimulation of the vagus nerve has anti-inflammatory and anti-depressant effects (Liu et al., 2020).

Using the LHe model, we designated the stress-induced PrRP overload in the DLH as one of the mechanisms potentially responsible for the susceptibility to LHe. PrRP overload could also affect the function of MCH cells. Foot shock activates the medullary PrRP neurons (Morales and Sawchenko, 2003). MCH neurons are also activated by foot shock, and inhibition of this activity causes a relapse into excessive cued fear response (freezing) for weeks (Concetti et al., 2020). Therefore, if the training-induced PrRP overactivation inhibited the optimal function of MCH cells in LHe-sensitive animals, they may have exhibited an enhanced fear response during the test.

On the other hand, acute, pretest MCH treatment increased the helpless behavior of previously shocked rats compared with nonshocked controls in the LHe paradigm (Urbanavicius et al., 2019). Chronic downregulation of MCH expression in LHe-resilient animals, therefore, may have been protective against the enhanced sensitivity to MCH in the test eliciting MCH cell activation. Importantly, PrRP receptors were not downregulated in the DLH in the LHe-resilient animals, in contrast to LHe-susceptible rats. Given the role of PrRP as a potential negative regulator of MCH cell activity, it is possible that the chronic/repeated stress-induced impairment of PrRP signaling contributes to failure of downregulation of MCH in LHe-sensitive rats. Together with MCH hypersensitivity, this may lead to a reduced ability to cope with stressful situations. Since the development of helpless behavior and symptoms in the LHe model are reminiscent of those of human depression (Wang et al., 2017), PrRP receptor downregulation found in the corresponding hypothalamic areas in suicidal individuals may have a fundamental significance. Although the etiology of suicide is complex, stress-related mental disorders underlie almost all cases, with depression being at the forefront (van Heeringen and Mann, 2014; Bryan, 2016; Bachmann, 2018).

Based on our multiple correlative data, we suggest that repeated/chronic stress leads to PrRP overload, and dysfunction of the PrRP system, consequently increasing the risk of developing stress-induced mental disorders. Dysregulation of MCH activity may also contribute to the development of these mental disorders and is likely to be associated with PrRP dysfunction in the DLH. Further work is needed to verify this hypothesis.

SNRI antidepressants are widely used to treat stress-related mental disorders, although the side effects and frequent inefficacy limit their benefits (Voineskos et al., 2020). Development of antidepressant drugs with a combined effect on monoaminergic and peptidergic systems may open new directions. Compounds targeting the GPCRs, such as PrRP receptors, are optimal for this purpose and may have a great therapeutic potential in the future (Mantas et al., 2022).

Footnotes

  • This work was supported by Hungarian Brain Research Program Grants 2017-1.2.1-NKP-2017-00002 and KTIA_13_NAPA-II/14; and National Development Agency Grant KTIA_NAP_13-1-2013-0001 and NKFI K 115422. Project TKP2021-EGA-25 has been implemented with the support provided by the Ministry of Innovation and Technology of Hungary from the National Research, Development and Innovation Fund, financed under the TKP2021-EGA funding scheme. We thank Judit Kerti, Szilvia Deák, and Ágnes Ruzsits for the indispensable technical assistance; and Jacqueline Casey for excellent contribution in proofreading the final draft.

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Zsuzsanna E. Tóth at toth.zsuzsanna.emese{at}med.semmelweis-univ.hu

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.

References

  1. ↵
    1. Appleyard SM,
    2. Marks D,
    3. Kobayashi K,
    4. Okano H,
    5. Low MJ,
    6. Andresen MC
    (2007) Visceral afferents directly activate catecholamine neurons in the solitary tract nucleus. J Neurosci 27:13292–13302. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3502-07.2007 pmid:18045923
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  2. ↵
    1. Bachmann S
    (2018) Epidemiology of suicide and the psychiatric perspective. Int J Environ Res Public Health 15:1425.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Bandaru SS,
    2. Khanday MA,
    3. Ibrahim N,
    4. Naganuma F,
    5. Vetrivelan R
    (2020) Sleep–wake control by melanin-concentrating hormone (MCH) neurons: a review of recent findings. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep 20:55. doi:10.1007/s11910-020-01075-x pmid:33006677
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Bao AM,
    2. Swaab DF
    (2019) The human hypothalamus in mood disorders: the HPA axis in the center. IBRO Rep 6:45–53. doi:10.1016/j.ibror.2018.11.008 pmid:31211281
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Barth AL,
    2. Gerkin RC,
    3. Dean KL
    (2004) Alteration of neuronal firing properties after in vivo experience in a FosGFP transgenic mouse. J Neurosci 24:6466–6475. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4737-03.2004 pmid:15269256
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  6. ↵
    1. Bindra D,
    2. Thompson WR
    (1953) An evaluation of defecation and urination as measures of fearfulness. J Comp Physiol Psychol 46:43–45. doi:10.1037/h0057952 pmid:13034953
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Blanco-Centurion C,
    2. Liu M,
    3. Konadhode RP,
    4. Zhang X,
    5. Pelluru D,
    6. van den Pol AN,
    7. Shiromani PJ
    (2016) Optogenetic activation of melanin-concentrating hormone neurons increases non-rapid eye movement and rapid eye movement sleep during the night in rats. Eur J Neurosci 44:2846–2857. doi:10.1111/ejn.13410 pmid:27657541
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Blanco-Centurion C,
    2. Luo S,
    3. Spergel DJ,
    4. Vidal-Ortiz A,
    5. Oprisan SA,
    6. Van den Pol AN,
    7. Liu M,
    8. Shiromani PJ
    (2019) Dynamic network activation of hypothalamic MCH neurons in REM sleep and exploratory behavior. J Neurosci 39:4986–4998. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0305-19.2019
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  9. ↵
    1. Bonnavion P,
    2. Mickelsen LE,
    3. Fujita A,
    4. de Lecea L,
    5. Jackson AC
    (2016) Hubs and spokes of the lateral hypothalamus: cell types, circuits and behaviour. J Physiol 594:6443–6462. doi:10.1113/JP271946 pmid:27302606
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Bryan CJ
    (2016) Treating PTSD within the context of heightened suicide risk. Curr Psychiatry Rep 18:73. doi:10.1007/s11920-016-0708-z pmid:27314245
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Buckley TM,
    2. Schatzberg AF
    (2005) On the interactions of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis and sleep: normal HPA axis activity and circadian rhythm, exemplary sleep disorders. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 90:3106–3114. doi:10.1210/jc.2004-1056 pmid:15728214
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Buller K,
    2. Xu Y,
    3. Dayas C,
    4. Day T
    (2001) Dorsal and ventral medullary catecholamine cell groups contribute differentially to systemic interleukin-1beta-induced hypothalamic pituitary adrenal axis responses. Neuroendocrinology 73:129–138. doi:10.1159/000054629 pmid:11244300
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Chaki S,
    2. Funakoshi T,
    3. Hirota-Okuno S,
    4. Nishiguchi M,
    5. Shimazaki T,
    6. Iijima M,
    7. Grottick AJ,
    8. Kanuma K,
    9. Omodera K,
    10. Sekiguchi Y,
    11. Okuyama S,
    12. Tran TA,
    13. Semple G,
    14. Thomsen W
    (2005) Anxiolytic- and antidepressant-like profile of ATC0065 and ATC0175: nonpeptidic and orally active melanin-concentrating hormone receptor 1 antagonists. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 313:831–839. doi:10.1124/jpet.104.081711 pmid:15677346
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  14. ↵
    1. Chen C,
    2. Dun SL,
    3. Dun NJ,
    4. Chang JK
    (1999) Prolactin-releasing peptide-immunoreactivity in A1 and A2 noradrenergic neurons of the rat medulla. Brain Res 822:276–279. doi:10.1016/s0006-8993(99)01153-1 pmid:10082910
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Chen CC,
    2. Wada K,
    3. Jarvis ED
    (2012) Radioactive in situ hybridization for detecting diverse gene expression patterns in tissue. J Vis Exp 62:3764. doi:10.3791/3764
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  16. ↵
    1. Chung S,
    2. Parks GS,
    3. Lee C,
    4. Civelli O
    (2011) Recent updates on the melanin-concentrating hormone (MCH) and its receptor system: lessons from MCH1R antagonists. J Mol Neurosci 43:115–121. doi:10.1007/s12031-010-9411-4 pmid:20582487
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Cominski TP,
    2. Jiao X,
    3. Catuzzi JE,
    4. Stewart AL,
    5. Pang KC
    (2014) The role of the hippocampus in avoidance learning and anxiety vulnerability. Front Behav Neurosci 8:273. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00273 pmid:25152721
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Concetti C,
    2. Bracey EF,
    3. Peleg-Raibstein D,
    4. Burdakov D
    (2020) Control of fear extinction by hypothalamic melanin-concentrating hormone-expressing neurons. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 117:22514–22521. doi:10.1073/pnas.2007993117 pmid:32848057
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  19. ↵
    1. Cropper EC,
    2. Jing J,
    3. Vilim FS,
    4. Weiss KR
    (2018) Peptide cotransmitters as dynamic, intrinsic modulators of network activity. Front Neural Circuits 12:78. doi:10.3389/fncir.2018.00078
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Dantzer R
    (2009) Cytokine, sickness behavior, and depression. Immunol Allergy Clin North Am 29:247–264. doi:10.1016/j.iac.2009.02.002 pmid:19389580
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Diniz GB,
    2. Bittencourt JC
    (2017) The melanin-concentrating hormone as an integrative peptide driving motivated behaviors. Front Syst Neurosci 11:32. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2017.00032
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Dodd GT,
    2. Luckman SM
    (2013) Physiological roles of GPR10 and PrRP signaling. Front Endocrinol (Lausanne) 4:20. doi:10.3389/fendo.2013.00020 pmid:23467899
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Echeverria V,
    2. Grizzell JA,
    3. Barreto GE
    (2016) Neuroinflammation: a therapeutic target of cotinine for the treatment of psychiatric disorders? Curr Pharm Des 22:1324–1333. doi:10.2174/138161282210160304112511 pmid:26972290
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Frenois F,
    2. Moreau M,
    3. O'Connor J,
    4. Lawson M,
    5. Micon C,
    6. Lestage J,
    7. Kelley KW,
    8. Dantzer R,
    9. Castanon N
    (2007) Lipopolysaccharide induces delayed FosB/DeltaFosB immunostaining within the mouse extended amygdala, hippocampus and hypothalamus, that parallel the expression of depressive-like behavior. Psychoneuroendocrinology 32:516–531. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007.03.005 pmid:17482371
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Goldstein AN,
    2. Walker MP
    (2014) The role of sleep in emotional brain function. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 10:679–708. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032813-153716 pmid:24499013
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Hassani OK,
    2. Lee MG,
    3. Jones BE
    (2009) Melanin-concentrating hormone neurons discharge in a reciprocal manner to orexin neurons across the sleep–wake cycle. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106:2418–2422. doi:10.1073/pnas.0811400106 pmid:19188611
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  27. ↵
    1. Heinz DE,
    2. Schottle VA,
    3. Nemcova P,
    4. Binder FP,
    5. Ebert T,
    6. Domschke K,
    7. Wotjak CT
    (2021) Exploratory drive, fear, and anxiety are dissociable and independent components in foraging mice. Transl Psychiatry 11:318. doi:10.1038/s41398-021-01458-9 pmid:34039953
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    1. Hokfelt T,
    2. Broberger C,
    3. Xu ZQ,
    4. Sergeyev V,
    5. Ubink R,
    6. Diez M
    (2000) Neuropeptides: an overview. Neuropharmacology 39:1337–1356. doi:10.1016/s0028-3908(00)00010-1 pmid:10818251
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    1. Ibata Y,
    2. Iijima N,
    3. Kataoka Y,
    4. Kakihara K,
    5. Tanaka M,
    6. Hosoya M,
    7. Hinuma S
    (2000) Morphological survey of prolactin-releasing peptide and its receptor with special reference to their functional roles in the brain. Neurosci Res 38:223–230. doi:10.1016/s0168-0102(00)00182-6 pmid:11070188
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Izawa S,
    2. Chowdhury S,
    3. Miyazaki T,
    4. Mukai Y,
    5. Ono D,
    6. Inoue R,
    7. Ohmura Y,
    8. Mizoguchi H,
    9. Kimura K,
    10. Yoshioka M,
    11. Terao A,
    12. Kilduff TS,
    13. Yamanaka A
    (2019) REM sleep-active MCH neurons are involved in forgetting hippocampus-dependent memories. Science 365:1308–1313. doi:10.1126/science.aax9238 pmid:31604241
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  31. ↵
    1. Johansson A,
    2. Löfberg C
    (2015) Novel MCH1 receptor antagonists: a patent review. Expert Opin Ther Pat 25:193–207. doi:10.1517/13543776.2014.993382 pmid:25522065
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. Kantor S,
    2. Jakus R,
    3. Balogh B,
    4. Benko A,
    5. Bagdy G
    (2004) Increased wakefulness, motor activity and decreased theta activity after blockade of the 5-HT2B receptor by the subtype-selective antagonist SB-215505. Br J Pharmacol 142:1332–1342. doi:10.1038/sj.bjp.0705887 pmid:15265808
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Kitka T,
    2. Katai Z,
    3. Pap D,
    4. Molnar E,
    5. Adori C,
    6. Bagdy G
    (2009) Small platform sleep deprivation selectively increases the average duration of rapid eye movement sleep episodes during sleep rebound. Behav Brain Res 205:482–487. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2009.08.004 pmid:19665493
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Kitka T,
    2. Adori C,
    3. Katai Z,
    4. Vas S,
    5. Molnar E,
    6. Papp RS,
    7. Toth ZE,
    8. Bagdy G
    (2011) Association between the activation of MCH and orexin immunorective neurons and REM sleep architecture during REM rebound after a three day long REM deprivation. Neurochem Int 59:686–694. doi:10.1016/j.neuint.2011.06.015 pmid:21740944
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Kopp C,
    2. Rudolph U,
    3. Tobler I
    (2004) Sleep EEG changes after zolpidem in mice. Neuroreport 15:2299–2302. doi:10.1097/00001756-200410050-00031 pmid:15371753
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Krolewski DM,
    2. Medina A,
    3. Kerman IA,
    4. Bernard R,
    5. Burke S,
    6. Thompson RC,
    7. Bunney WE Jr.,
    8. Schatzberg AF,
    9. Myers RM,
    10. Akil H,
    11. Jones EG,
    12. Watson SJ
    (2010) Expression patterns of corticotropin-releasing factor, arginine vasopressin, histidine decarboxylase, melanin-concentrating hormone, and orexin genes in the human hypothalamus. J Comp Neurol 518:4591–4611. doi:10.1002/cne.22480 pmid:20886624
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Lagos P,
    2. Urbanavicius J,
    3. Scorza MC,
    4. Miraballes R,
    5. Torterolo P
    (2011) Depressive-like profile induced by MCH microinjections into the dorsal raphe nucleus evaluated in the forced swim test. Behav Brain Res 218:259–266. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2010.10.035 pmid:21056060
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    1. Lin SH,
    2. Arai AC,
    3. España RA,
    4. Berridge CW,
    5. Leslie FM,
    6. Huguenard JR,
    7. Vergnes M,
    8. Civelli O
    (2002) Prolactin-releasing peptide (PrRP) promotes awakening and suppresses absence seizures. Neuroscience 114:229–238. doi:10.1016/s0306-4522(02)00248-8 pmid:12207968
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Liu CH,
    2. Yang MH,
    3. Zhang GZ,
    4. Wang XX,
    5. Li B,
    6. Li M,
    7. Woelfer M,
    8. Walter M,
    9. Wang L
    (2020) Neural networks and the anti-inflammatory effect of transcutaneous auricular vagus nerve stimulation in depression. J Neuroinflammation 17:54. doi:10.1186/s12974-020-01732-5 pmid:32050990
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Luttrell LM,
    2. Lefkowitz RJ
    (2002) The role of beta-arrestins in the termination and transduction of G-protein-coupled receptor signals. J Cell Sci 115:455–465. doi:10.1242/jcs.115.3.455 pmid:11861753
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  41. ↵
    1. Ma L,
    2. MacTavish D,
    3. Simonin F,
    4. Bourguignon JJ,
    5. Watanabe T,
    6. Jhamandas JH
    (2009) Prolactin-releasing peptide effects in the rat brain are mediated through the Neuropeptide FF receptor. Eur J Neurosci 30:1585–1593. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06956.x pmid:19821834
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. Machado RB,
    2. Hipólide DC,
    3. Benedito-Silva AA,
    4. Tufik S
    (2004) Sleep deprivation induced by the modified multiple platform technique: quantification of sleep loss and recovery. Brain Res 1004:45–51. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2004.01.019 pmid:15033418
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Madsen KL,
    2. Thorsen TS,
    3. Rahbek-Clemmensen T,
    4. Eriksen J,
    5. Gether U
    (2012) Protein interacting with C kinase 1 (PICK1) reduces reinsertion rates of interaction partners sorted to Rab11-dependent slow recycling pathway. J Biol Chem 287:12293–12308. doi:10.1074/jbc.M111.294702 pmid:22303009
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  44. ↵
    1. Mai JK,
    2. Majtanik M,
    3. Paxinos G
    (2015) Atlas of the human brain, Ed 4. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.
  45. ↵
    1. Maloney KJ,
    2. Mainville L,
    3. Jones BE
    (1999) Differential c-Fos expression in cholinergic, monoaminergic, and GABAergic cell groups of the pontomesencephalic tegmentum after paradoxical sleep deprivation and recovery. J Neurosci 19:3057–3072. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-08-03057.1999 pmid:10191323
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  46. ↵
    1. Mantas I,
    2. Saarinen M,
    3. Xu ZD,
    4. Svenningsson P
    (2022) Update on GPCR-based targets for the development of novel antidepressants. Mol Psychiatry 27:534–558. doi:10.1038/s41380-021-01040-1
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  47. ↵
    1. Marques DB,
    2. Ruggiero RN,
    3. Bueno-Junior LS,
    4. Rossignoli MT,
    5. Leite JP
    (2022) Prediction of learned resistance or helplessness by hippocampal-prefrontal cortical network activity during stress. J Neurosci 42:81–96. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0128-21.2021
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  48. ↵
    1. Maruyama M,
    2. Matsumoto H,
    3. Fujiwara K,
    4. Noguchi J,
    5. Kitada C,
    6. Fujino M,
    7. Inoue K
    (2001) Prolactin-releasing peptide as a novel stress mediator in the central nervous system. Endocrinology 142:2032–2038. doi:10.1210/endo.142.5.8118 pmid:11316770
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    1. McCloy RA,
    2. Rogers S,
    3. Caldon CE,
    4. Lorca T,
    5. Castro A,
    6. Burgess A
    (2014) Partial inhibition of Cdk1 in G2 phase overrides the SAC and decouples mitotic events. Cell Cycle 13:1400–1412. doi:10.4161/cc.28401 pmid:24626186
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    1. Mera T,
    2. Fujihara H,
    3. Kawasaki M,
    4. Hashimoto H,
    5. Saito T,
    6. Shibata M,
    7. Saito J,
    8. Oka T,
    9. Tsuji S,
    10. Onaka T,
    11. Ueta Y
    (2006) Prolactin-releasing peptide is a potent mediator of stress responses in the brain through the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus. Neuroscience 141:1069–1086. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.04.023 pmid:16730416
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    1. Mlynarik M,
    2. Zelena D,
    3. Bagdy G,
    4. Makara GB,
    5. Jezova D
    (2007) Signs of attenuated depression-like behavior in vasopressin deficient Brattleboro rats. Horm Behav 51:395–405. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.12.007 pmid:17258216
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    1. Morales T,
    2. Sawchenko PE
    (2003) Brainstem prolactin-releasing peptide neurons are sensitive to stress and lactation. Neuroscience 121:771–778. doi:10.1016/s0306-4522(03)00522-0 pmid:14568035
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    1. Ohiwa N,
    2. Chang H,
    3. Saito T,
    4. Onaka T,
    5. Fujikawa T,
    6. Soya H
    (2007) Possible inhibitory role of prolactin-releasing peptide for ACTH release associated with running stress. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 292:R497–R504. doi:10.1152/ajpregu.00345.2006 pmid:16917019
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    1. Otrokocsi L,
    2. Kittel Á,
    3. Sperlágh B
    (2017) P2X7 receptors drive spine synapse plasticity in the learned helplessness model of depression. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 20:813–822. doi:10.1093/ijnp/pyx046 pmid:28633291
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    1. Pacak K,
    2. Palkovits M,
    3. Kopin IJ,
    4. Goldstein DS
    (1995) Stress-induced norepinephrine release in the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus and pituitary-adrenocortical and sympathoadrenal activity: in vivo microdialysis studies. Front Neuroendocrinol 16:89–150. doi:10.1006/frne.1995.1004 pmid:7621982
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  56. ↵
    1. Palkovits M
    (1973) Isolated removal of hypothalamic or other brain nuclei of the rat. Brain Res 59:449–450. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(73)90290-4
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  57. ↵
    1. Paxinos G,
    2. Watson C
    (2007) The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates, Ed 6. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
  58. ↵
    1. Porsolt RD,
    2. Anton G,
    3. Blavet N,
    4. Jalfre M
    (1978) Behavioural despair in rats: a new model sensitive to antidepressant treatments. Eur J Pharmacol 47:379–391. doi:10.1016/0014-2999(78)90118-8 pmid:204499
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    1. Risold PY,
    2. Croizier S,
    3. Legagneux K,
    4. Brischoux F,
    5. Fellmann D,
    6. Griffond B
    (2009) The development of the MCH system. Peptides 30:1969–1972. doi:10.1016/j.peptides.2009.07.016 pmid:19647770
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  60. ↵
    1. Rudolph U,
    2. Crestani F,
    3. Möhler H
    (2001) GABA(A) receptor subtypes: dissecting their pharmacological functions. Trends Pharmacol Sci 22:188–194. doi:10.1016/s0165-6147(00)01646-1 pmid:11282419
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  61. ↵
    1. Scherrer G,
    2. Tryoen-Tóth P,
    3. Filliol D,
    4. Matifas A,
    5. Laustriat D,
    6. Cao YQ,
    7. Basbaum AI,
    8. Dierich A,
    9. Vonesh JL,
    10. Gavériaux-Ruff C,
    11. Kieffer BL
    (2006) Knockin mice expressing fluorescent delta-opioid receptors uncover G protein-coupled receptor dynamics in vivo. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103:9691–9696. doi:10.1073/pnas.0603359103 pmid:16766653
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  62. ↵
    1. Seal LJ,
    2. Small CJ,
    3. Dhillo WS,
    4. Kennedy AR,
    5. Ghatei MA,
    6. Bloom SR
    (2002) Prolactin-releasing peptide releases corticotropin-releasing hormone and increases plasma adrenocorticotropin via the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus. Neuroendocrinology 76:70–78. doi:10.1159/000064427 pmid:12169768
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  63. ↵
    1. Slattery DA,
    2. Cryan JF
    (2012) Using the rat forced swim test to assess antidepressant-like activity in rodents. Nat Protoc 7:1009–1014. doi:10.1038/nprot.2012.044 pmid:22555240
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  64. ↵
    1. Smith DG,
    2. Davis RJ,
    3. Rorick-Kehn L,
    4. Morin M,
    5. Witkin JM,
    6. McKinzie DL,
    7. Nomikos GG,
    8. Gehlert DR
    (2006) Melanin-concentrating hormone-1 receptor modulates neuroendocrine, behavioral, and corticolimbic neurochemical stress responses in mice. Neuropsychopharmacology 31:1135–1145. doi:10.1038/sj.npp.1300913 pmid:16205780
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  65. ↵
    1. Smoller JW
    (2016) The genetics of stress-related disorders: PTSD, depression, and anxiety disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology 41:297–319. doi:10.1038/npp.2015.266 pmid:26321314
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  66. ↵
    1. Steiger A,
    2. Pawlowski M
    (2019) Depression and sleep. Int J Mol Sci 20:607. doi:10.3390/ijms20030607
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  67. ↵
    1. Torterolo P,
    2. Scorza C,
    3. Lagos P,
    4. Urbanavicius J,
    5. Benedetto L,
    6. Pascovich C,
    7. López-Hill X,
    8. Chase MH,
    9. Monti JM
    (2015) Melanin-concentrating hormone (MCH): role in REM sleep and depression. Front Neurosci 9:475. doi:10.3389/fnins.2015.00475 pmid:26733789
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  68. ↵
    1. Tóth ZE,
    2. Mezey E
    (2007) Simultaneous visualization of multiple antigens with tyramide signal amplification using antibodies from the same species. J Histochem Cytochem 55:545–554. doi:10.1369/jhc.6A7134.2007 pmid:17242468
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  69. ↵
    1. Tóth ZE,
    2. Zelena D,
    3. Mergl Z,
    4. Kirilly E,
    5. Várnai P,
    6. Mezey E,
    7. Makara GB,
    8. Palkovits M
    (2008) Chronic repeated restraint stress increases prolactin-releasing peptide/tyrosine-hydroxylase ratio with gender-related differences in the rat brain. J Neurochem 104:653–666. doi:10.1111/j.1471-4159.2007.05069.x pmid:18199117
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  70. ↵
    1. Urbanavicius J,
    2. Fabius S,
    3. Roncalho A,
    4. Joca S,
    5. Torterolo P,
    6. Scorza C
    (2019) Melanin-concentrating hormone in the locus coeruleus aggravates helpless behavior in stressed rats. Behav Brain Res 374:112120. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2019.112120 pmid:31376444
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  71. ↵
    1. van Dalfsen JH,
    2. Markus CR
    (2018) The influence of sleep on human hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis reactivity: a systematic review. Sleep Med Rev 39:187–194. doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2017.10.002 pmid:29126903
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  72. ↵
    1. van den Pol AN,
    2. Acuna-Goycolea C,
    3. Clark KR,
    4. Ghosh PK
    (2004) Physiological properties of hypothalamic MCH neurons identified with selective expression of reporter gene after recombinant virus infection. Neuron 42:635–652. doi:10.1016/s0896-6273(04)00251-x pmid:15157424
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  73. ↵
    1. van Heeringen K,
    2. Mann JJ
    (2014) The neurobiology of suicide. Lancet Psychiatry 1:63–72. doi:10.1016/S2215-0366(14)70220-2 pmid:26360403
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  74. ↵
    1. Vas S,
    2. Ádori C,
    3. Könczöl K,
    4. Kátai Z,
    5. Pap D,
    6. Papp RS,
    7. Bagdy G,
    8. Palkovits M,
    9. Tóth ZE
    (2013) Nesfatin-1/NUCB2 as a potential new element of sleep regulation in rats. PLoS One 8:e59809. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0059809 pmid:23560056
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  75. ↵
    1. Verret L,
    2. Goutagny R,
    3. Fort P,
    4. Cagnon L,
    5. Salvert D,
    6. Léger L,
    7. Boissard R,
    8. Salin P,
    9. Peyron C,
    10. Luppi PH
    (2003) A role of melanin-concentrating hormone producing neurons in the central regulation of paradoxical sleep. BMC Neurosci 4:19. doi:10.1186/1471-2202-4-19 pmid:12964948
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  76. ↵
    1. Voineskos D,
    2. Daskalakis ZJ,
    3. Blumberger DM
    (2020) Management of treatment-resistant depression: challenges and strategies. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat 16:221–234. doi:10.2147/NDT.S198774 pmid:32021216
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  77. ↵
    1. Wang Q,
    2. Timberlake MA 2nd.,
    3. Prall K,
    4. Dwivedi Y
    (2017) The recent progress in animal models of depression. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 77:99–109. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2017.04.008 pmid:28396255
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  78. ↵
    1. Wingenfeld K,
    2. Wolf OT
    (2014) Stress, memory, and the hippocampus. Front Neurol Neurosci 34:109–120. doi:10.1159/000356423 pmid:24777135
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  79. ↵
    1. Wittmann G,
    2. Mohácsik P,
    3. Balkhi MY,
    4. Gereben B,
    5. Lechan RM
    (2015) Endotoxin-induced inflammation down-regulates L-type amino acid transporter 1 (LAT1) expression at the blood-brain barrier of male rats and mice. Fluids Barriers CNS 12:21. doi:10.1186/s12987-015-0016-8
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  80. ↵
    1. Yoshida M,
    2. Takayanagi Y,
    3. Onaka T
    (2014) The medial amygdala-medullary PrRP-synthesizing neuron pathway mediates neuroendocrine responses to contextual conditioned fear in male rodents. Endocrinology 155:2996–3004. doi:10.1210/en.2013-1411 pmid:24877622
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  81. ↵
    1. Zelena D,
    2. Mergl Z,
    3. Foldes A,
    4. Kovács KJ,
    5. Tóth Z,
    6. Makara GB
    (2003) Role of hypothalamic inputs in maintaining pituitary-adrenal responsiveness in repeated restraint. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab 285:E1110–E1117. doi:10.1152/ajpendo.00219.2003 pmid:14534078
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  82. ↵
    1. Zhang SQ,
    2. Inoué S,
    3. Kimura M
    (2001) Sleep-promoting activity of prolactin-releasing peptide (PrRP) in the rat. Neuroreport 12:3173–3176. doi:10.1097/00001756-200110290-00006 pmid:11711850
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 43 (5)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 43, Issue 5
1 Feb 2023
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Masthead (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Prolactin-Releasing Peptide Contributes to Stress-Related Mood Disorders and Inhibits Sleep/Mood Regulatory Melanin-Concentrating Hormone Neurons in Rats
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Prolactin-Releasing Peptide Contributes to Stress-Related Mood Disorders and Inhibits Sleep/Mood Regulatory Melanin-Concentrating Hormone Neurons in Rats
Szilvia Vas, Rege S. Papp, Katalin Könczöl, Emese Bogáthy, Noémi Papp, Csaba Ádori, Máté Durst, Klaudia Sípos, Klementina Ocskay, Imre Farkas, Flóra Bálint, Szilamér Ferenci, Bibiána Török, Anita Kovács, Evelin Szabó, Dóra Zelena, Krisztina J. Kovács, Anna Földes, Erzsébet Kató, László Köles, György Bagdy, Miklós Palkovits, Zsuzsanna E. Tóth
Journal of Neuroscience 1 February 2023, 43 (5) 846-862; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2139-21.2022

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Prolactin-Releasing Peptide Contributes to Stress-Related Mood Disorders and Inhibits Sleep/Mood Regulatory Melanin-Concentrating Hormone Neurons in Rats
Szilvia Vas, Rege S. Papp, Katalin Könczöl, Emese Bogáthy, Noémi Papp, Csaba Ádori, Máté Durst, Klaudia Sípos, Klementina Ocskay, Imre Farkas, Flóra Bálint, Szilamér Ferenci, Bibiána Török, Anita Kovács, Evelin Szabó, Dóra Zelena, Krisztina J. Kovács, Anna Földes, Erzsébet Kató, László Köles, György Bagdy, Miklós Palkovits, Zsuzsanna E. Tóth
Journal of Neuroscience 1 February 2023, 43 (5) 846-862; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2139-21.2022
Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • GABA(A) receptor
  • MCH
  • mental disorders
  • sleep
  • stress
  • suicide

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Articles

  • Mechanisms of Dominant Electrophysiological Features of Four Subtypes of Layer 1 Interneurons
  • Neural index of reinforcement learning predicts improved stimulus-response retention under high working memory load
  • Activity-dependent Nr4a2 induction modulates synaptic expression of AMPA receptors and plasticity via a Ca2+/CRTC1/CREB pathway
Show more Research Articles

Neurobiology of Disease

  • Involvement of GABAergic Interneuron Subtypes in 4-Aminopyridine-Induced Seizure-Like Events in Mouse Entorhinal Cortex in Vitro
  • Spared Premotor Areas Undergo Rapid Nonlinear Changes in Functional Organization Following a Focal Ischemic Infarct in Primary Motor Cortex of Squirrel Monkeys
  • Epilepsy-Related CDKL5 Deficiency Slows Synaptic Vesicle Endocytosis in Central Nerve Terminals
Show more Neurobiology of Disease
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Policy
  • Contact
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2023 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.