Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Featured ArticleResearch Articles, Development/Plasticity/Repair

Changes in Cortical Microstructure of the Human Brain Resulting from Long-Term Motor Learning

Nico Lehmann, Norman Aye, Jörn Kaufmann, Hans-Jochen Heinze, Emrah Düzel, Gabriel Ziegler and Marco Taubert
Journal of Neuroscience 13 December 2023, 43 (50) 8637-8648; https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0537-23.2023
Nico Lehmann
1Faculty of Human Sciences, Institute III, Department of Sport Science, Otto von Guericke University, Magdeburg 39104, Germany
2Department of Neurology, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig 04103, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Nico Lehmann
Norman Aye
1Faculty of Human Sciences, Institute III, Department of Sport Science, Otto von Guericke University, Magdeburg 39104, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Jörn Kaufmann
3Department of Neurology, Otto von Guericke University, Magdeburg 39120, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Jörn Kaufmann
Hans-Jochen Heinze
3Department of Neurology, Otto von Guericke University, Magdeburg 39120, Germany
4German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Magdeburg 39120, Germany
5Center for Behavioral and Brain Science (CBBS), Otto von Guericke University, Magdeburg 39106, Germany
6Leibniz-Institute for Neurobiology (LIN), Magdeburg 39118, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Emrah Düzel
4German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Magdeburg 39120, Germany
5Center for Behavioral and Brain Science (CBBS), Otto von Guericke University, Magdeburg 39106, Germany
7Institute of Cognitive Neurology and Dementia Research, Otto von Guericke University, Magdeburg 39120, Germany
8Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London WC1N 3AZ, United Kingdom
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Gabriel Ziegler
4German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Magdeburg 39120, Germany
7Institute of Cognitive Neurology and Dementia Research, Otto von Guericke University, Magdeburg 39120, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Marco Taubert
1Faculty of Human Sciences, Institute III, Department of Sport Science, Otto von Guericke University, Magdeburg 39104, Germany
5Center for Behavioral and Brain Science (CBBS), Otto von Guericke University, Magdeburg 39106, Germany
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Marco Taubert
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

The mechanisms subserving motor skill acquisition and learning in the intact human brain are not fully understood. Previous studies in animals have demonstrated a causal relationship between motor learning and structural rearrangements of synaptic connections, raising the question of whether neurite-specific changes are also observable in humans. Here, we use advanced diffusion magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), sensitive to dendritic and axonal processes, to investigate neuroplasticity in response to long-term motor learning. We recruited healthy male and female human participants (age range 19–29) who learned a challenging dynamic balancing task (DBT) over four consecutive weeks. Diffusion MRI signals were fitted using Neurite Orientation Dispersion and Density Imaging (NODDI), a theory-driven biophysical model of diffusion, yielding measures of tissue volume, neurite density and the organizational complexity of neurites. While NODDI indices were unchanged and reliable during the control period, neurite orientation dispersion increased significantly during the learning period mainly in primary sensorimotor, prefrontal, premotor, supplementary, and cingulate motor areas. Importantly, reorganization of cortical microstructure during the learning phase predicted concurrent behavioral changes, whereas there was no relationship between microstructural changes during the control phase and learning. Changes in neurite complexity were independent of alterations in tissue density, cortical thickness, and intracortical myelin. Our results are in line with the notion that structural modulation of neurites is a key mechanism supporting complex motor learning in humans.

SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT The structural correlates of motor learning in the human brain are not fully understood. Results from animal studies suggest that synaptic remodeling (e.g., reorganization of dendritic spines) in sensorimotor-related brain areas is a crucial mechanism for the formation of motor memory. Using state-of-the-art diffusion magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), we found a behaviorally relevant increase in the organizational complexity of neocortical microstructure, mainly in primary sensorimotor, prefrontal, premotor, supplementary, and cingulate motor regions, following training of a challenging dynamic balancing task (DBT). Follow-up analyses suggested structural modulation of synapses as a plausible mechanism driving this increase, while colocalized changes in cortical thickness, tissue density, and intracortical myelin could not be detected. These results advance our knowledge about the neurobiological basis of motor learning in humans.

  • brain microstructure
  • diffusion
  • MRI
  • motor learning
  • neuroplasticity
  • NODDI
  • skill acquisition

Introduction

The production of well coordinated movements is among the most remarkable abilities of humans, and understanding the neural basis underlying motor learning is a core topic of the neurosciences (Dayan and Cohen, 2011). Research in animals has shown that motor learning is accompanied by changes in the size, shape and volume of the brain (Rosenzweig and Bennett, 1996; Markham and Greenough, 2004; Zatorre et al., 2012). At a finer scale, it is assumed that several neuronal and non-neuronal processes contribute to these macrostructural alterations (Zatorre et al., 2012). In the gray matter (GM), synaptic remodeling is a well established mechanism underlying motor learning (Xu et al., 2009; Hayashi-Takagi et al., 2015), whereas myelin-related changes have been conclusively shown to occur in white matter (WM) tissue (Sampaio-Baptista et al., 2013; McKenzie et al., 2014; Bacmeister et al., 2020). Of note, neuroplastic processes in GM and WM were demonstrated to be highly dynamic with transient retraction and expansion tendencies (Xu et al., 2009; Wenger et al., 2017; Bacmeister et al., 2020), requiring highly specific methods to detect them.

Because of methodological limitations in in vivo sampling of brain tissue, it has long been impossible to investigate whether the learning-related plasticity processes demonstrated in animals, such as changes in neurite structures (i.e., dendrites and axons), also apply in humans. Over the past two decades, longitudinal studies using magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) with sensitized contrast to the mobility of water yielded promising achievements in this respect (for review, see Zatorre et al., 2012; Tardif et al., 2016; Lerch et al., 2017). Usually, these studies employed diffusion sensitization at low b-values (≤1000 s/mm−2) and fitted the measured signal to a diffusion tensor (DTI; Basser and Pierpaoli, 1996). Previous studies of motor learning based on this approach have shown changes in diffusivity and anisotropy in both GM (Sagi et al., 2012) and WM (Scholz et al., 2009).

Albeit sensitive to change, tensor-derived metrics are biologically ambiguous because they reflect a voxel average of the degree of hindrance or restriction experienced by the diffusing water (Jones et al., 2013; Lerch et al., 2017). Key neuroplastic processes related to the brain's neurite architecture or myelin remodeling can therefore not be directly inferred from DTI. Moreover, DTI is not well suited to deal with highly dispersed and complex tissue microstructure present in the vast majority of WM, but especially in GM voxels (Jones et al., 2013; Tardif et al., 2016; Lerch et al., 2017; Assaf, 2019).

Recent advances in diffusion MRI do now offer the possibility to characterize key neuroplastic processes and their dynamics in unprecedented specificity, while possibly also offering higher sensitivity to detect neuroplastic changes and brain-behavior-correlations (Kamiya et al., 2020). For instance, multicompartment models like Neurite Orientation Dispersion and Density Imaging (NODDI) can cope with complex fiber arrangements and can differentiate between intraneurite restricted (e.g., axons and dendrites) and extraneurite hindered (e.g., cell bodies, glial cells) diffusion. Extensive validation studies have shown a high correspondence between NODDI-based estimates of neurite density (Grussu et al., 2017; Wang et al., 2019; Gong et al., 2020) and orientation dispersion (Grussu et al., 2017; Mollink et al., 2017; Sato et al., 2017; Schilling et al., 2018) with their histologic counterparts.

Here, we investigate whether key neuroplastic mechanisms present in animals can also be observed in humans. Specifically, we aim to identify brain areas showing correlated changes (C. Thomas and Baker, 2013) between NODDI-derived microstructural indices and concurrent motor learning. To achieve this, we use an established dynamic balancing task (DBT; Wulf et al., 2001) as a learning paradigm, whose acquisition process is expected to result in structural reorganization, particularly in sensorimotor, supplementary motor and prefrontal areas and associated nerve fiber tracts, according to previous study results (Taubert et al., 2010, 2016; Lehmann et al., 2020, 2022). To narrow down possible neurobiological correlates underlying plasticity (Tardif et al., 2016), we use information from multiple state-of-the-art MRI modalities.

Materials and Methods

Participants and experimental design

We used a controlled within-subject design (see below) to investigate whether learning the DBT would change the microstructure of the brain's GM and WM. Twenty-four cognitively healthy adults [21 ♂, 3 ♀; age: (arithmetic) mean (M) = 22.21, SD = 3.05, range 19–29; body mass index (BMI): M = 23.55, SD = 2.52, range 18.99–29.40] with no history of neurologic, psychiatric or systemic diseases were included in this trial. Further exclusion criteria were contraindications to MRI, body mass index (BMI) > 30 kg/cm2, a history of neuropsychiatric diseases, left-handedness, self-reported physical activity of >2 h/week, prior experience with the DBT, and past or present performance-oriented participation in endurance and/or coordinative-demanding sports. The sample was a subset of the subjects that participated in our previous reliability studies (Lehmann et al., 2021; Aye et al., 2022). The study was performed in accordance with the ethical standards as laid down in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments. Approval was granted by the Ethics Committee of Otto von Guericke University Magdeburg (approval number 106/98). Written informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

The experimental design consisted of a control phase and a learning phase of equal duration. MRI measurements were conducted three times at four-week intervals. In the first four-week period, we assessed the reliability of our MR imaging protocol. After this control period subjects engaged in four consecutive weeks of learning the DBT. At the end of the learning period subjects received MRI scans identical to those in MRI1 and MRI2. To avoid acute influences of motor training on MRI (e.g., increased functional activity or blood flow), training sessions (TSs) and MRI measurements were performed at least 24 h apart. Note that all subjects participated in both phases of the experiment, such that each subject serves as its own control (cf. A.G. Thomas et al., 2009), reducing the influence of potential confounding variables. In other words, the MRI signal changes during the learning phase (MRI2–MRI3) can be related to the changes during the control phase (MRI1–MRI2), which functions as the baseline. The within-subjects design also ensures optimal statistical power because of the removal of variance associated with between-subject differences (Poldrack, 2000; C. Thomas and Baker, 2013; Szucs and Ioannidis, 2020).

Whole-body dynamic balancing task (DBT) and quantification of motor learning

After the four-week scan-rescan control interval was completed, subjects commenced with learning the DBT over four weeks, with two training sessions (TSs) per week at least 24 h apart. DBT training was performed on an unstable seesaw-like platform (stability platform, model 16030, Lafayette Instrument) moveable in a medio-lateral direction with a maximum deviation of 26° on either side of the pivot. Standing with both feet on the platform, subjects were instructed to keep the board in a horizontal position for as long as possible during each trial. Every TS consisted of 15 trials with a duration of 30 s each and an intertrial break of 90 s to avoid fatigue. The behavioral outcome measure was the time (millisecond timer) in which subjects kept the platform within a deviation range of ±3° from the horizontal (time balancing, BAL). After each trial, subjects received verbal feedback on how long the board was in the target zone (knowledge of results), while no feedback was provided on strategy or other aspects of the task (discovery learning approach; Orrell et al., 2006). During task execution, participants' attention was directed to a fixation cross affixed to the wall in front of them (external focus of attention).

To get a summary measure of learning performance over eight training sessions, we first averaged the 15 BAL values belonging to each training session. Second, we fitted a general power function y(x)=a×xn(1) to the session-averaged DBT data of each individual (Ivry, 1996). For whole-brain nonparametric combination (NPC) analyses, we used the slope value (exponent n) of the power function, adjusted for the base a (Manning and Dubois, 1962; Adams, 1987) by residualization, as a dependent variable.

MR image acquisition

MRI data were acquired on a 3T MAGNETOM Prisma system (Siemens Healthcare) using a 64-channel head coil. The same measurement protocol was used for each volunteer and scanning session. Subjects were asked to relax, keep their mind free of any thoughts, and to move as little as possible. A pillow was placed surrounding the sides and the back of the head to minimize head motion and within-subject as well as between-subject differences in positioning.

Whole-brain diffusion MR images were obtained with a monopolar single-shot spin echo echo planar image (EPI) sequence: echo time (TE) = 74 ms; repetition time (TR) = 4970 ms; flip angle α = 90°; parallel GRAPPA acceleration factor = 2, matrix: 130 × 130; field-of-view (FOV) = 208 × 208 mm; nominal spatial resolution = 1.6 × 1.6 × 1.6 mm; multiband acceleration factor = 2; phase-encoding direction: anterior ≫ posterior). The sampling scheme was designed according to Caruyer et al. (2013; http://www.emmanuelcaruyer.com/q-space-sampling.php) and consisted of 228 isotropically distributed diffusion sensitization directions including three diffusion shells with b-values of 1000 s/mm2 (38 directions), 2000 s/mm2 (76 directions), and 3000 s/mm2 (114 directions). Note that diffusion sensitization in this protocol is considered strong enough to sensitize to pools of water with restricted diffusion (especially neuritic elements) and to reduce signal contributions of the faster moving extraneurite water (Jones et al., 2013; Weiskopf et al., 2021). Fourteen images without diffusion weighting (b = 0 s/mm2) were collected interleaved throughout the acquisition. To generate appropriate fieldmaps to correct for susceptibility-induced distortions, nine b = 0 s/mm2 images with reversed phase encoding (posterior ≫ anterior) were also acquired. The total scan duration was 22 min 31 s.

To quantify cortical macrostructure, we used a standard T1w three-dimensional magnetization-prepared rapid gradient echo sequence (Mugler and Brookeman, 1990) based on which cortical thickness was estimated. The imaging parameters used were as follows: 240 sagittal slices, inversion time, TI = 1100 ms; TR = 2600 ms; TE = 5.18 ms; readout pulse flip angle, α = 7°; parallel GRAPPA acceleration factor = 2; acquisition matrix = 320 × 320; FOV = 256 × 256 mm; nominal spatial resolution = 0.8 × 0.8 × 0.8 mm; scan duration = 7 min 25 s.

Not least, to test myelin-related hypotheses (Tardif et al., 2016; Lerch et al., 2017; Natu et al., 2019; Lazari and Lipp, 2021), we used magnetization transfer saturation (MTsat) maps (Helms et al., 2008) based on MT-weighted images. MT-weighted images were acquired (as part of a multiparameter mapping protocol) with a multiecho FLASH scan with TR = 37 ms/α = 7°. Multiple gradient echoes were acquired with alternating readout polarity at six equidistant TEs between 2.46 and 14.76 ms. Other acquisition parameters were: 224 sagittal slices, nominal spatial resolution = 0.8 × 0.8 × 0.8 mm, FOV = 230 × 230 mm. Transmit and receive field correction acquisition was done before with the following imaging parameters: 56 sagittal slices, FOV = 230 × 230 mm, TR = 4.1 ms, TE = 1.98 ms for B1− and TR = 2000 ms, TE1 = TE2 = 14 ms, 24 sagittal slices, slice thickness = 5 mm, α = 90°, 120°, 60°, 135°, 45° for the RF map which was used for the B1+ correction. MT-weighted images were further preprocessed as outlined in Aye et al. (2022).

Preprocessing of diffusion-weighted images

Preprocessing of diffusion-weighted images followed the standard FSL diffusion processing pipeline (Smith et al., 2004). This pipeline starts with identifying susceptibility-induced distortions based on reversed phase-encoding b = 0 s/mm2 volumes using the topup tool (Andersson et al., 2003). Afterwards, corrections for eddy currents and subject movement were conducted using eddy (Andersson and Sotiropoulos, 2016), and the fieldmap that resulted from topup was applied. Realignment of images in the course of motion correction was accompanied by appropriate correction of gradient directions (Leemans and Jones, 2009).

Fitting of microstructural maps

In the next step, diffusion imaging data were fitted with NODDI (Zhang et al., 2012) and with the conventional diffusion tensor (Basser and Pierpaoli, 1996). Note that both modeling approaches were used in WM, because it has been suggested that the tensor-model might be particularly sensitive to change in tissue environments with approximately parallel fiber bundles (Tardif et al., 2016; Alexander et al., 2019; Kamiya et al., 2020). Unlike WM, GM exhibits an even more heterogeneous neural environment and shows above that a problematic contamination with CSF (Tardif et al., 2016; Assaf, 2019). From this it follows that several assumptions of the tensor model, such as the presence of one principal diffusion direction per voxel or single-exponential signal decay, are unlikely to be fulfilled in GM (Jones et al., 2013; Lerch et al., 2017; Fukutomi et al., 2019). On the contrary, NODDI is particularly well suited to assess neuroplastic change in tissue environments with highly dispersed and complex microstructure (e.g., crossing, fanning, or kissing fibers) because it allows for distributed fiber orientations within voxels (Tardif et al., 2016; Lerch et al., 2017; Assaf, 2019; Wang et al., 2019). Furthermore, NODDI-derived tissue indices are free from CSF contamination, making it particularly well suited for the study of plasticity in GM (Assaf, 2019; Fukutomi et al., 2019). Since NODDI is also considered to be more sensitive to microstructural change in GM than the tensor (Tavor et al., 2013; Vukovic et al., 2021), only NODDI maps were used to assess plasticity in this particular tissue class.

NODDI is a multicompartment model of diffusion aiming at the estimation of biologically interpretable microstructural tissue properties in each image voxel (Zhang et al., 2012). It assumes that the diffusion signal arises from the sum of three distinguishable tissue environments, namely intraneurite space, extraneurite space, and free water (for details of the model, see Zhang et al., 2012; Kraguljac et al., 2023). There are three microstructural maps emerging from this model that are used for statistical analyses: the isotropic volume fraction (ISO), the neurite density index (NDI), and the orientation dispersion index (ODI). ISO represents the signal from freely diffusing water or CSF (as opposed to tissue) in a voxel. Note that ISO and the tissue volume fraction, which comprises the intraneurite and extraneurite compartments, sum up to 1. This means that a higher (lower) value of ISO must be accompanied by a lower (higher) value of the tissue volume fraction. NDI is the proportion of tissue that is intended to represent contributions from axons, dendrites or neural processes (Zhang et al., 2012; Assaf, 2019). The relative volume fraction of the extraneurite compartment (1-NDI) is assumed to mainly comprise contributions from cellular membranes of somas and glial cells (Zhang et al., 2012). Not least, ODI is a metric based on the Watson distribution concentration parameter κ (Zhang et al., 2012). It describes the spatial configuration of the intraneurite space and represents how widely the orientations of the neurites spread out in space (Kraguljac et al., 2023).

In the present study, NODDI parameter maps were estimated from corrected multishell diffusion images (b = 0 s/mm2, b = 1000 s/mm2, b = 2000 s/mm2, and b = 3000 s/mm2). To fit the model in WM voxels, we used the NODDI MATLAB Toolbox v1.0.1 (http://nitrc.org/projects/noddi_toolbox) with default settings, implementing the model formulation of Zhang et al. (2012). Fukutomi et al. (2018) and Guerrero et al. (2019) have recently suggested that the fixed NODDI model parameter intrinsic free diffusivity (din), which reflects the rate of diffusion along individual neurites, needs to be adjusted in GM voxels. To this end, GM-optimized NODDI maps with din set to 1.1 × 10−3 mm2/s (instead of the WM optimum of 1.7 × 10−3 mm2/s) were calculated using the Accelerated Microstructure Imaging via Convex Optimization (AMICO) v1.5.2 toolbox running in python (Daducci et al., 2015).

To assess microstructural plasticity in WM, we also fitted a diffusion tensor (Basser and Pierpaoli, 1996) at each voxel of the preprocessed images (b = 0 s/mm2 and b = 1000 s/mm2 shells only) using FSL's dtifit. The diffusion indices fractional anisotropy (FA), mean diffusivity (MD), and radial diffusivity (λ⊥) were computed from the eigenvalues of the diffusion tensor with the standard formulas as described by Pierpaoli and Basser (1996).

Image normalization using tract-based and gray matter-based spatial statistics

To ensure optimal sensitivity of longitudinal exploratory whole-brain analyses, it is paramount to have accurate alignment of the microstructural maps within each subject as well as cross-subject voxel-based correspondence in standard (MNI152) space. To this end, we used skeleton-based registration approaches optimized for the conditions of WM and GM, respectively. The underlying idea of these approaches is to project diffusion imaging-derived microstructural maps to an alignment-invariant sample-specific skeleton which either represents the center of the main WM tracts (Smith et al., 2006) or the maximally probable GM voxels (Nazeri et al., 2015), as outlined below. Besides improving registration, skeletonization is also hoped to minimize the influence of partial-volume contamination of microstructural maps (Smith et al., 2006; Nazeri et al., 2015; Lerch et al., 2017).

For WM microstructural maps, we used a reliable (Madhyastha et al., 2014; Lehmann et al., 2021) and sensitive (Lehmann et al., 2020) TBSS-based (Smith et al., 2006) processing routine, starting with determining an unbiased midspace between the three FA images of each participant (Smith et al., 2002). In the next step, the native space FA images of each participant were linearly registered to the individual midspace and subsequently averaged to generate a within-subject FA template (Madhyastha et al., 2014; Lehmann et al., 2020, 2021). Afterwards, each subject's FA template was nonlinearly aligned to every other one to identify the most representative template of the sample (target). After warping each subject's template to the target, images were linearly registered to MNI152 space (FMRIB58 1-mm template) using affine transformation, and a group-average FA image was thinned and binarized with an FA-value of >0.25 (skeletonization). Finally, the previously created warp fields were applied to all midpoint-space registered NODDI/DTI maps of the three measurement points, and the aligned NODDI/DTI maps in MNI space were projected onto the groupwise skeleton.

For GM microstructural maps, we used a modified gray matter-based spatial statistics (GBSS) pipeline inspired by TBSS (Nazeri et al., 2015; bash scripts available at https://github.com/arash-n/GBSS). GBSS starts with a segmentation of brain tissues using the NODDI and DTI data. Specifically, the CSF volume fraction (fCSF) corresponds NODDI's ISO map, while the WM volume fraction (fWM) was derived from a two-tissue classification (WM/non-WM) of FA images using the Atropos segmentation tool implemented in ANTs (Avants et al., 2011). GM fraction maps were then calculated according to the formula: fGM=1−(fCSF+fWM).(2)

Note that we used midspace-registered average images of each subject for the processing steps mentioned above. Next, fGM and diffusion microstructural maps were warped to MNI152 space using the registrations estimated in the course of TBSS. Afterwards, fGM maps in MNI space were averaged resulting in a mean GM image which was subsequently thinned and binarized (skeletonization). Only voxels with fGM > 0.5 in >50% of the subjects were retained. Note that this threshold was set to a slightly more liberal level than the one originally proposed by the authors (cf. Nazeri et al., 2015) to ensure an adequate representation especially of the motor regions on the skeleton, which are known to have a relatively thin cortex. Next, each subject's maximally probable local GM voxel perpendicular to the skeleton was identified. Finally, applying the estimated projections from the previous step, GM-optimized NODDI and MTsat maps (after boundary-based intermodal registration to native diffusion space, Greve and Fischl, 2009) of each subject and measurement point were registered onto the skeleton.

Quality assurance and reliability assessment

We calculated a recently proposed index of diffusion MR image quality, the temporal signal-to-noise ratio (tSNR), from the preprocessed and brain-extracted DWI data. TSNR is a metric that captures both scanner-induced and subject-induced imaging artifacts and allows to reliably differentiate between poor and good image quality (Roalf et al., 2016). For each subject and imaging session, tSNR was estimated by first calculating the mean and standard deviation of each voxel's intensity over time, and then averaging the resulting values across all brain voxels to yield a single metric of image quality (Farrell et al., 2007; Roalf et al., 2016). Note that this calculation was performed separately for the b = 0 s/mm2 and b = 1000 s/mm2 shells, respectively (Farrell et al., 2007; Roalf et al., 2016). tSNR was not calculated for the shells with higher diffusion weightings because their signal intensity and noise profiles differ considerably from volumes without diffusion weighting (Farrell et al., 2007).

TSNR data were subsequently compared between measurement sessions (MRI1, MRI2, MRI3) by means of a one-way repeated-measurements ANOVAs (RM-ANOVAs). Furthermore, we checked for the presence of tSNR outliers using the absolute deviation around the median (MAD) framework (Leys et al., 2013). Specifically, an individual's tSNR value was categorized as an outlier if it was lower than 2.5 times the MAD below the group median (Leys et al., 2013).

Furthermore, we calculated the reliability metrics within-subject coefficient of variation (CVWS, Hopkins, 2000) and intraclass coefficient (two-way mixed model, single measure, absolute agreement; cf. McGraw and Wong, 1996) based on each individual's median voxel on the GM skeleton at time points MRI1 and MRI2 (i.e., in the absence of training). To do so, we employed the procedures and formulas that we have previously used to assess NODDI reliability in WM (Lehmann et al., 2021).

Not least, we tested for the presence of systematic error or drift in the global signal by comparing the median voxel of NODDI maps on the GM skeleton between measurement sessions (MRI1, MRI2, MRI3) with a one-way RM-ANOVA. Note in this respect that a global change in microstructural maps after training is not expected. Rather, we assume that plasticity effects occur in a regional fashion in networks that were previously identified to change with stabilometer training (Taubert et al., 2010, 2016; Lehmann et al., 2020, 2022).

Assessment of behaviorally relevant plasticity

Whole-brain voxel-wise statistical analyses on skeletonized microstructural maps were conducted using the Permutation Analysis of Linear Models (PALM) v. alpha115 toolbox (Winkler et al., 2014, 2016) running in a MATLAB R2017B environment. Specifically, we aimed to identify clusters of voxels simultaneously showing both (1) changes in microstructural indices over time and (2) a correlation between neuroplasticity and motor learning (C. Thomas and Baker, 2013). To this end, the global hypothesis of behaviorally relevant neuroplasticity was broken down into a set of four subhypotheses (general linear models), each sensitive to the empirical predictions of the theory (for a graphical overview, see Fig. 1). PALM's modified nonparametric combination (NPC) framework was then used to collate the information from these partial tests to produce a single measurement summarizing the statistical evidence for the underlying complex theory (Winkler et al., 2016).

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Overview of the statistical analyses using the NPC framework. For each imaging modality, change images between the MRI measurements before (MRI1, MRI2) and after the learning phase (MRI3) were calculated. Afterwards, both resulting change images (ΔMRI1–MRI3 and ΔMRI2–MRI3) were subjected to a one sample t test against zero to address learning-related structural plasticity (top row). Likewise, two submodels addressing the correlation between brain changes and DBT learning rate were set up (bottom row). Union-intersection tests (UIT) were then conducted to identify clusters of voxels in which there was, on aggregate, evidence for learning-related plasticity in conjunction with behavioral relevant plasticity. To this end, the UIT outputs a single measurement that summarizes evidence across the four submodels in every voxel.

To practically implement the abovementioned complex hypothesis test, we first calculated percentage change images between MRI1 and MRI3 as well as between MRI2 and MRI3 and created design matrices as outlined in the following. The first set of designs addressed whether motor learning did induce changes in WM or GM tissue microstructure. The corresponding hypothesis was formalized by means of one-sample t tests comparing voxelwise percentage change maps against zero. To emphasize the aspect of reproducibility, the t test was not only conducted on percentage change images between MRI2–MRI3, but also for the time interval MRI1–MRI3 (Fig. 1). The idea behind this is that the neuroplastic effect should be present regardless of whether MRI1 or MRI2 is defined as the baseline, provided that the maps were unchanged and reliable between MRI1 and MRI2 (Lehmann et al., 2021). The other design matrices postulate that learning-related neuroplasticity correlates with DBT learning. To this end, we used regression-type models to test for a linear relationship between (percentage) changes in the microstructural maps and (residualized) DBT learning rate. Again, these tests were conducted for the time intervals MRI1–MRI3 and MRI2–MRI3. We used directional (one-sided) contrasts based on the anticipated pattern of results (for example, increase in microstructure and positive correlation with behavior, or vice versa) in all submodels.

We then used the modified NPC framework as implemented in PALM (Winkler et al., 2014, 2016) to jointly test and aggregate significance across the statistical submodels introduced above. In brief, NPC tests the null hypothesis that the null hypotheses of all partial tests are true (alternative hypothesis: any is false). NPC works by first testing each component hypothesis separately using synchronized permutations. Because of this random resampling, NPC relies on minimal statistical assumptions (e.g., with respect to the distribution of the data). Second, the resulting statistics for each permutation are recorded and based on them, the empirical null distribution for each of the partial tests is constructed. Finally, at each voxel, the test statistics resulting from the previous step are combined across subtests, in our case by using Fisher's (1932) combining function. The joint statistic is considered to be significant if an aggregate of the partial tests is significant, therefore forming a union-intersection test. Note also that the NPC procedure implicitly deals with nonindependence between partial tests under the null hypothesis owing to the use of synchronized permutations.

We ran the modified NPC procedure with 5000 permutations of the data to build up the empirical null distribution from which statistical inference was performed. Cluster-like structures in the images were formed using the threshold-free cluster enhancement (TFCE) approach (Smith and Nichols, 2009). The resulting statistical maps were then subjected to cluster-based family-wise error (FWE) correction by using the distribution of the maximum statistic (Smith and Nichols, 2009; Winkler et al., 2014). Results were considered to be significant at p < 0.05 (FWE-corrected), while potentially meaningful statistical trends were assessed with a more liberal FWE-threshold of p < 0.1.

Intermodal relationships of microstructural changes

For significant results emerging from whole-brain analyses, we assessed intermodal relationships between microstructural change maps to gain a more granular insight into the plasticity process. Specifically, we were interested in processes that might drive the plasticity results. In addition to looking at the correlations between learning-related changes in the NODDI maps (ODI, NDI, ISO), here we also consider myelin-sensitive MTsat images (Lazari and Lipp, 2021) to examine in detail whether changes in the NODDI maps covary with myelin-related processes or are relatively independent of them. To this end, within significant clusters, we extracted the median voxel intensities of the microstructural change maps (ΔMRI2–MRI3) from the skeleton. Afterwards, we fitted multiple linear regression models with the modality of the significant cluster as dependent variable and the remaining modalities as independent variables.

It has been suggested that MRI-derived microstructural maps sample different, yet correlated, biophysical properties of the local tissue environment, depending on the MR sequence parameters (Weiskopf et al., 2021). Therefore, it must be assumed that the amount of shared variance (collinearity) between the predictor variables is high, which complicates inferences about the relative importance of predictors. To circumvent this problem, we have calculated relative weights (RWs) instead of “standard” regression coefficients. Briefly, from the original regressors, the RW analysis approach creates a new set of regressors that is as highly correlated as possible with the original set of variables but orthogonal (uncorrelated) to each other (Johnson, 2000). Therefore, an RW reflects the contribution of a predictor to variance of the dependent variable, considering the predictor's unique contribution as well as its common contribution with the other predictors in the model. RW analyses decomposes the coefficient of determination (multiple R2) of the model into contributions from the individual regressors (Johnson, 2000). Regression models and RWs were calculated using R's standard library (v 4.1.1) and the tool RWA Web with bootstrap confidence interval (CI) estimation (Tonidandel and LeBreton, 2015). For statistical significance testing, 10,000 samples were drawn using the bias-corrected and accelerated (BCa) method of bootstrapping.

Presence of macrostructural and tissue volume changes

To be able to interpret microstructural changes especially in GM, it is paramount to also consider colocalized changes in cortical macrostructure and tissue volume. For example, a loss of neurite structures in combination with cortical thinning might manifest itself in an increase of the NODDI parameter neurite density (Colgan et al., 2016). Furthermore, as the NODDI metrics ODI and NDI are corrected for the contribution of free water (ISO), interpreting their changes during learning should consider potential changes in tissue volume (1-ISO).

With respect to cortical macrostructure, we estimated cortical thickness based on T1-weighted images using the default settings of the CAT12 toolbox v12.7 r1738 (Christian Gaser, Structural Brain Mapping Group, Jena University Hospital; http://www.neuro.uni-jena.de/cat12/) within SPM12 v7771 (Statistical Parametric Mapping, Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging; http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm12/) for MATLAB R2017b (MathWorks). After preprocessing, the Human Connectome Project Multi-Modal Parcellation (HCP-MMP; Glasser et al., 2016) atlas was mapped to the estimated surfaces of each individual and measurement point by using the spherical registration parameters obtained from the surface-based processing (Gaser et al., 2022). Afterwards, cortical thickness was calculated for selected task-relevant regions of interest (ROIs) in native space. A particular brain region qualified as an ROI if both of the following criteria were given: (1) presence of a significant neuroplastic effect showing up in both hemispheres based on the exploratory whole-brain (NPC) analyses in the present study, (2) evidence of neuroplastic change after DBT training found in previous studies (Taubert et al., 2010, 2016; Lehmann et al., 2020). For each ROI, we calculated percentage change in cortical thickness between measurement points MRI2 and MRI3 and compared the obtained values against zero by means of one-sample t tests. Finally, the resulting p-values from the previous step were combined using Brown's (1975) method, which takes into consideration the internal correlation (dependency) of the data (Cinar and Viechtbauer, 2022). If the aggregated p-value is significant, this would lend support to the notion of regional specificity of the neuroplastic effect. Similarly, we calculated percentage change scores between MRI2 and MRI3 in NODDI-derived free water maps (ISO) based on the median voxel within (volumetric) ROIs, conducted one-sample t tests against zero and pooled the results with Brown's (1975) method; t tests and p-value combinations were done using R v4.1.1 and the R package poolr (Cinar and Viechtbauer, 2022).

Results

Motor skill practice improved performance in the DBT

We started our analyses by analyzing motor performance of the DBT. We found that the general power function yielded an adequate fit to individual's learning data (Fig. 2) with a median coefficient of determination of R2 = 0.93 (25th percentile: 0.89, 75th percentile: 0.95). Participants' learning rates (exponent of the power function) were significantly different from zero, t(23) = 13.84, p < 0.001, Hedges' g = 2.73, 95% CI [1.59,3.86], suggesting that the DBT performance of the sample improved during training (large effect).

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

DBT performance significantly improved over eight sessions of learning. Colored dots represent the averaged within-session performance of the n = 24 participants. To assess learning, each individual's data were fitted with a general power function (colored lines).

Quality and reliability of MR metrics

Before commencing with the analysis of training-induced brain plasticity, we compared the MRI measurement sessions for differences in tSNR (Farrell et al., 2007; Roalf et al., 2016) and checked the data for the presence of outliers. By means of One-Way RM-ANOVAs (including MRI1, MRI2, MRI3), we found that tSNR was stable over time (b = 0 s/mm2 data: F(1.43,32.78) = 0.62, p = 0.49, Greenhouse-Geisser corrected; b = 1000 s/mm2 data: F(2,46) = 0.25, p = 0.78). Likewise, individual tSNR values did not fall outside the predefined rejection criterion of 2.5*MAD below the median (Leys et al., 2013), thus indicating the absence of issues with respect to MR image quality.

Scan-rescan reliability was assessed between measurements MRI1 and MRI2, a time interval in which no change was expected because of the absence of training. With respect to microstructural maps in WM, our analyses revealed high measurement precision and remarkable properties to discriminate between individuals. Because the reliability of NODDI metrics was restricted to WM in our previous paper (cf. Lehmann et al., 2021), we calculated absolute (CVWS) and relative (ICC) reliabilities only for NODDI maps in the GM. To this end, we extracted the median voxel of GM-optimized NODDI maps (Fukutomi et al., 2018; Guerrero et al., 2019) from the GBSS-skeleton of the measurements MRI1 and MRI2. Two-way mixed model ICC (McGraw and Wong, 1996) and CVWS (Hopkins, 2000) revealed good-to-excellent reliability of the NODDI metrics in GM tissue (Fig. 3). We also subjected the NODDI maps to a one-way RM-ANOVA to test whether median voxels extracted from the GBSS-derived skeleton means would differ depending on measurement point (MRI1, MRI2, MRI3). Again, no differences in NDI, F(2,46) = 0.24, p = 0.79, ODI, F(2,46) = 1.38, p = 0.26 and FISO, F(2,46) = 0.02, p = 0.98 were found, therefore indicating the absence of systematic error (Fig. 3).

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Assessment of reliability and systematic error of NODDI metrics in the gray matter. All statistics and graphs presented are based on the median of voxels on the GBSS whole-brain skeleton (50th percentile) extracted separately for each of the n = 24 participants at the different time points of the experiment (MRI1, MRI2, MRI3). A, Examplary scatter plot of the test-retest correlation of GM-ODI during the control phase of the experiment (MRI1–MRI2). B, Relative (ICC) and absolute (CVWS) test-retest-reliability during the control phase of the experiment (MRI1–MRI2). Error bars represent 95% asymmetric CIs/confidence limits around the point estimates. All ICCs were significant at p < 0.001. C, Global values of NODDI metrics over the time course of the experiment. No significant differences between measurement points were found. ns = not significant.

Behaviorally relevant changes in cortical microstructure induced by motor learning

Using the modified NPC framework (Winkler et al., 2014, 2016), we continued by performing whole-brain analyses aimed at identifying clusters of voxels whose changes were both significantly different from zero (plasticity) and correlated with the DBT learning rate (behavioral relevance).

With respect to GM microstructure, we found a pattern of results consistent with the assumption that behaviorally relevant structural changes occurred in the brain during DBT learning. Specifically, a learning-associated increase in the NODDI metric ODI and a colocalized positive correlation between changes in ODI and DBT learning rate were found (Fig. 4; Table 1). The cluster extended mainly over the primary motor cortex, the anterior cingulate cortex, and the medial prefrontal cortex, as well as several subdivisions of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Because GM-ODI within this cluster was unchanged and reliable in the time interval without training (Fig. 4), it is unlikely that the neuroplastic effect was because of random measurement noise. Likewise, quality of diffusion MR images (averaged tSNR across MRI2 and MRI3 of the b = 0 s/mm2 data) was unrelated to percentage change in ODI during the learning period (MRI2–MRI3), r = −0.11, p = 0.61. Also note that within-cluster ODI changes between MRI2 and MRI3 (as well as MRI1 and MRI3) were correlated with DBT learning, whereas changes between MRI1 and MRI2 did not predict DBT learning (Fig. 4).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Peak coordinates and localization of significant clusters emerging from the voxel-based NPC analysis

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Training-related, behaviorally relevant changes in the cortical orientation dispersion index (ODI) derived from the NODDI model. The figure demonstrates that cortical ODI changed noticeably as a result of DBT learning; furthermore, the degree of change in cortical ODI correlated with improved performance (i.e., faster learning). A, Results from the whole-brain union-intersection test (UIT) on Δ_ODI maps based on the NPC methodology. Significant clusters depict voxels, in which the UIT revealed evidence for a training-induced increase in ODI as well as a correlation between Δ_ODI and DBT learning rate. For visualization, the clusters derived from PALM were fattened with the “tbss_fill” script and subsequently projected from MNI152 space onto the surface of the FSaverage brain using the CAT12 toolbox (Gaser et al., 2022). Clusters are displayed at p < 0.05, FWE-corrected (TFCE). Panels B and C show descriptive data illustrating the underlying idea of the NPC analysis. B, The data underlying the significant NPC cluster are presented as connected dots (within-cluster median of ODI of the n = 24 subjects at each measurement point, extracted after back-projection to native space). Boxplots are overlaid for each time point. Note that ODI in the time interval without learning (MRI1–MRI2) was highly reliable, both in terms of within-subject variation, CVWS = 1.83%, 95% CI [1.48,2.43] and intraclass correlation, ICC(C,1) = 0.86, 95% CI [0.71,0.94], p < 0.001. Also note the increase in ODI visible at MRI3 (postlearning). C, Scatterplots with lines of best fit show the relation between within-cluster Δ_ODI (calculated based on the data shown in panel B in different time intervals) and DBT learning rate. Positive correlations are apparent for the time intervals involving the learning period (i.e., MRI1–MRI3 and MRI2–MRI3), while data were randomly scattered for the correlation in the time interval without training (MRI1–MRI2; r = −0.13, p = 0.55). The correlations between ODI changes and DBT learning rate were significantly stronger in the time intervals with training than in the time intervals without training (MRI1–MRI3 vs MRI1–MRI2: z = 4.60, p < 0.001; MRI2–MRI3 vs MRI1–MRI2: z = 3.29, p = 0.001), as determined by Steiger's (1980) z-test.

Cortical macrostructure and tissue volume fraction unaltered by motor learning

Next, we looked for the presence of changes in cortical thickness and free water (ISO), the latter as an indicator of tissue volume, during motor learning. Based on the results of the NPC whole-brain analysis in the present study and the evidence from previous studies (Taubert et al., 2010, 2016; Lehmann et al., 2020, 2022), we chose the bilateral primary motor cortex (area four from the HCP-MMP atlas; Glasser et al., 2016) and the bilateral supplementary motor areas (6mp, 6ma, and SCEF according to Glasser et al., 2016) as ROIs. Separate for each modality, we then compared percentage change during learning (MRI2–MRI3) against zero by means of one-sample t tests. The eight p-values retrieved were then pooled within-modality using Brown's (1975) method on the basis of the calculated correlation structure. No significant training-induced changes in cortical thickness, χ2(7.98) = 8.91, p = 0.35 and free water (ISO), χ2(9.23) = 8.12, p = 0.54 were found.

Changes in orientational coherence are correlated with changes in neurite density, but not with intracortical myelin

While ODI is a measure representing geometric complexity (angular variation) of neurite orientation, several other microstructural processes have been proposed to contribute to structural plasticity in GM (Blumenfeld-Katzir et al., 2011; Keifer et al., 2015; Asan et al., 2021; Schmidt et al., 2021; Matsuda et al., 2022; Mediavilla et al., 2022). To gain more granular insight into the neuroplastic process during motor learning, we sought to predict changes in ODI by a linear combination of NDI, ISO, and MTsat change (Fig. 5). The multiple linear regression model significantly explained variance in the criterion Δ_ODI, multiple R2 = 0.40, adjusted R2 = 0.30, F(3,20) = 4.36, p = 0.02. Further analyses revealed that Δ_NDI was positively correlated with Δ_ODI and accounted for 77.61% of explainable variance in the criterion, which is a statistically significant contribution, RW = 0.31, 95% BCa CI [0.03,0.52]. On the contrary, the RWs for Δ_ISO and Δ_MTsat were not significant. Therefore, Δ_NDI was the most important predictor, differing significantly from the RWs obtained for Δ_ISO, RW_diff = −0.22, 95% BCa CI [−0.50,−0.09] and for Δ_MTsat, RW_diff = −0.31, 95% BCa CI [−0.59,−0.06].

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

Microstructural correlates of Δ_ODI in the cluster from NPC whole-brain analysis. A, Results of a multiple regression model with changes in Δ_NDI, Δ_ISO, and Δ_MT as predictors of Δ_ODI. Relative weights analysis was conducted to partition multiple R2 among the predictor variables, i.e., RWs of all predictors (depicted as points) sum to the multiple R2 of the model. Error bars represent 95% BCa CIs used to test the statistical significance of the RWs based on 10,000 bootstrap samples. B, Scatterplot with line of best fit shows the positive correlation between Δ_NDI and Δ_ODI.

Results of NODDI and DTI metrics in white matter

Whole-brain NPC analyses did not show nominally significant results in the WM as assessed with TBSS. However, we observed statistical trends for radial (λ⊥; pFWE < 0.06) and mean diffusivities (pFWE < 0.09) in prefrontal fiber tracts. NODDI results showed no trend regarding possible behaviorally relevant neuroplasticity (all ps > 0.1).

Discussion

Establishing behaviorally relevant structural changes in the human brain is a challenging endeavor. In addition to calls for rigorous study designs and appropriate analytical approaches to address this topic (C. Thomas and Baker, 2013), most studies to date have used MRI pulse sequences that do not allow for a biologically specific interpretation of neuroplastic changes (Tardif et al., 2016; Lerch et al., 2017). Using a powerful within-subject design in combination with multishell diffusion MRI, we show that training a complex DBT over four weeks increases the organizational complexity of cortical microstructure. Importantly, the microstructural changes during training correlated with the change in DBT performance, and structural remodeling of the brain was specific, meaning that it selectively affected brain areas associated with motor processes, in the absence of systematic global changes of tissue microstructure over time.

To the best of our knowledge, only two previous studies have investigated neuroplasticity in GM using multishell diffusion MRI combined with advanced biophysical modeling accounting for non-Gaussian diffusion. In response to two short learning sessions of a car-racing game, Tavor et al. (2013) report an increase in restricted diffusion in prefrontal, temporal, and parietal areas based on the CHARMED model (Assaf and Basser, 2005). Furthermore, Vukovic et al. (2021) detected widespread diffusion kurtosis (Jensen et al., 2005) changes following a brief language learning session. However, to our knowledge, the present study is the first to find cortical microstructural plasticity after a lengthy period of motor learning, as well as the first to report a correlation between microstructural change in GM and concomitant behavioral change.

Learning-induced changes in GM microstructure were predominantly present in the primary sensorimotor cortex, the supplementary and cingulate motor areas, the premotor cortex, but also extended to prefrontal areas. These regions are generally known to be involved in movement planning, motor control, and learning (Strick et al., 2021), but they have also been identified in previous work as important nodes in the structural brain network underlying DBT learning (Taubert et al., 2010, 2016; Lehmann et al., 2020, 2022).

Based on findings in animal models, it has been hypothesized that structural changes in neurites might also underlie learning in humans (Zatorre et al., 2012). Previous studies have indeed shown that diffusion MRI in general and the orientation distribution function derived from it in particular are sensitive to neuritic elements and their organizational complexity (Colgan et al., 2016; Assaf, 2019; Wang et al., 2019; Mak et al., 2021; Reveley et al., 2022). In light of such findings, a previous study in humans interpreted a long-lasting learning-induced reduction in neocortical mean diffusivity as an indication that a relatively stable memory engram had formed (Brodt et al., 2018). Although interpretation of NODDI indices in terms of the underlying cellular correlates remains challenging (Kamiya et al., 2020), our finding of a behaviorally relevant increase in the variability of neurite orientations in task-related brain areas seems also consistent with the notion that a motor engram has been formed. In this sense, on the one hand, it has been shown in animal models that motor learning is closely linked to structural changes in dendrites (Xu et al., 2009; Hayashi-Takagi et al., 2015; Hwang et al., 2022), and on the other hand, NODDI's ODI has commonly been interpreted as an index of dendritic complexity (Colgan et al., 2016; Assaf, 2019; Wang et al., 2019; Nazeri et al., 2020; Mak et al., 2021). Further supporting this interpretation is the fact that several studies of neuroplasticity in animals combining histology and MRI found that dendritic remodeling was accompanied by MRI-detectable signal changes (Blumenfeld-Katzir et al., 2011; Keifer et al., 2015; Schmidt et al., 2021; Matsuda et al., 2022). Another candidate mechanism that could give rise to a reduced orientational coherence of water diffusion is axonal growth (Lerch et al., 2011), which, however, has been observed less frequently compared with dendritic changes.

The interpretation that neuritic processes were affected by training is additionally supported by the positive correlation between changes in ODI and colocalized changes in NDI, an index of the relative volume fraction of the intraneurite compartment in the tissue (Zhang et al., 2012). However, in contrast to previous findings in humans (Tavor et al., 2013; Vukovic et al., 2021), no evidence for a significant increase in restricted diffusion was detected. We speculate that the timescales at which neuroplastic processes operate might have played a role here. For example, mice learning a forelimb reaching task show an increase in dendritic spine density within an hour, which returns to pretraining baseline within two weeks (Xu et al., 2009). Similar rapid plasticity processes might have taken place in the studies by Tavor et al. (2013) and Vukovic et al. (2021), who found changes in GM diffusion within just a few hours or days of training. In contrast, in our study a possible increase in neurite volume early in the learning process could have already disappeared after four weeks of training (Wenger et al., 2017).

In humans, morphometric changes of the brain's GM because of training are thought to result from changes in cortical thickness, surface area, tissue density, cellular composition of tissue microstructure or some permutation of these factors (Wenger et al., 2017; Asan et al., 2021). In the present study, there was no evidence that cortical thickness was changed after training or that cortical tissue was more densely packed. This is consistent with the findings of several studies on aging and neurodegenerative diseases, which collectively suggest that there is limited covariation between tissue properties derived from T1w imaging (GM volume/cortical thickness) and those from NODDI (Nazeri et al., 2015; Mak et al., 2021; Bai et al., 2022), underlining the position that diffusion imaging-derived indices may be sensitive for detecting aspects of neuroplasticity in GM that go beyond “conventional” methods (Tavor et al., 2013; Assaf, 2019; Kamiya et al., 2020; Vukovic et al., 2021).

Besides learning-induced changes in neuritic elements and neuroglia, research conducted in animals suggests that increased myelin density may be another cellular mechanism that significantly contributes to plasticity in the neocortex (Mediavilla et al., 2022). Although it has been suggested that apical dendrites are the main driver of restricted diffusion in the neocortex (Reveley et al., 2022), NODDI metrics have shown to be associated with myelin staining in postmortem samples of rodent brains (Sepehrband et al., 2015; Wang et al., 2019) and human spinal cord (Grussu et al., 2017). Likewise, a colocalization of cortical neurite density as assessed with NODDI and myelin content as estimated based on the T1w/T2w ratio has been reported (Fukutomi et al., 2018). In the present study, however, there was no significant association between training-induced changes in myelin-sensitive magnetization transfer maps and concurrent changes in ODI, suggesting that changes in intracortical myelination did not contribute to alterations in neurite complexity.

It is plausible that some limitations might have influenced the results obtained. First, in the controlled within-subjects design we used, as opposed to a parallel randomized controlled trial, it cannot be ruled out that confounders influenced the control and learning phases of the experiment differently. However, explanations for our results other than training appear unlikely because (1) direct interaction with the balancing task, not just life-as-usual, was required to induce microstructural plasticity, (2) NODDI metrics were reliable with no signs of undue influence caused by systematic and unsystematic error, (3) NODDI changes during the training but not the control period correlated with DBT learning. Second, it has been argued that some of the modeling assumptions that NODDI makes may be oversimplified (Jones et al., 2013; Kamiya et al., 2020), especially in complex tissues such as GM. One point concerns the fact that NODDI does not explicitly model glial cells (Kamiya et al., 2020), although this would be desirable because of the fact that neuroglial changes are closely coupled to neuronal changes (Perez-Alvarez et al., 2014). Such justified criticisms must be weighed against the facts that NODDI is highly reproducible (Lehmann et al., 2021; Coelho et al., 2022) and extensively validated by histologic examinations (cf. Kraguljac et al., 2023). Third, we unexpectedly did not find statistically significant evidence for neuroplastic changes in WM. Instead, we observed a trend toward decreased diffusivity in prefrontal fiber tracts, consistent with previous findings (Taubert et al., 2010; Lehmann et al., 2020). This might suggest that our study has been underpowered to detect medium-size effects in exploratory whole-brain analysis of the WM.

In summary, this study shows that advanced diffusion MRI combined with biophysical modeling is a promising approach to gain highly specific insights into the underlying mechanisms of neuroplasticity in humans. From an applied point of view, it is interesting to note that neurite orientation dispersion, which we found to increase after motor training, is often lowered in aging and neurodegenerative conditions (Nazeri et al., 2015; Nazeri et al., 2020; Mak et al., 2021; Bai et al., 2022). In perspective, it is therefore hoped that NODDI will increasingly be used as an MRI index for evaluating treatments to maintain brain health or to ameliorate the effects of disease.

Footnotes

  • This work was supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) Grant CRC 1436 TPC01. N.A. was supported by the innovation fund of the Otto von Guericke University Magdeburg (2039236003).

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Nico Lehmann at nico1.lehmann{at}ovgu.de

SfN exclusive license.

References

  1. ↵
    1. Adams JA
    (1987) Historical review and appraisal of research on the learning, retention, and transfer of human motor skills. Psychol Bull 101:41–74. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.101.1.41
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  2. ↵
    1. Alexander DC,
    2. Dyrby TB,
    3. Nilsson M,
    4. Zhang H
    (2019) Imaging brain microstructure with diffusion MRI: practicality and applications. NMR Biomed 32:e3841.
    OpenUrl
  3. ↵
    1. Andersson JLR,
    2. Sotiropoulos SN
    (2016) An integrated approach to correction for off-resonance effects and subject movement in diffusion MR imaging. Neuroimage 125:1063–1078. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.019 pmid:26481672
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Andersson JLR,
    2. Skare S,
    3. Ashburner J
    (2003) How to correct susceptibility distortions in spin-echo echo-planar images: application to diffusion tensor imaging. Neuroimage 20:870–888. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00336-7 pmid:14568458
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Asan L,
    2. Falfán-Melgoza C,
    3. Beretta CA,
    4. Sack M,
    5. Zheng L,
    6. Weber-Fahr W,
    7. Kuner T,
    8. Knabbe J
    (2021) Cellular correlates of gray matter volume changes in magnetic resonance morphometry identified by two-photon microscopy. Sci Rep 11:4234. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-83491-8 pmid:33608622
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Assaf Y
    (2019) Imaging laminar structures in the gray matter with diffusion MRI. Neuroimage 197:677–688. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.12.096 pmid:29309898
    OpenUrlPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Assaf Y,
    2. Basser PJ
    (2005) Composite hindered and restricted model of diffusion (CHARMED) MR imaging of the human brain. Neuroimage 27:48–58. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.03.042 pmid:15979342
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Avants BB,
    2. Tustison NJ,
    3. Song G,
    4. Cook PA,
    5. Klein A,
    6. Gee JC
    (2011) A reproducible evaluation of ANTs similarity metric performance in brain image registration. Neuroimage 54:2033–2044. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.025 pmid:20851191
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Aye N,
    2. Lehmann N,
    3. Kaufmann J,
    4. Heinze H-J,
    5. Düzel E,
    6. Taubert M,
    7. Ziegler G
    (2022) Test-retest reliability of multi-parametric maps (MPM) of brain microstructure. Neuroimage 256:119249. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.119249 pmid:35487455
    OpenUrlPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Bacmeister CM,
    2. Barr HJ,
    3. McClain CR,
    4. Thornton MA,
    5. Nettles D,
    6. Welle CG,
    7. Hughes EG
    (2020) Motor learning promotes remyelination via new and surviving oligodendrocytes. Nat Neurosci 23:819–831. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-020-0637-3 pmid:32424285
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Bai X,
    2. Guo T,
    3. Chen J,
    4. Guan X,
    5. Zhou C,
    6. Wu J,
    7. Liu X,
    8. Wu H,
    9. Wen J,
    10. Gu L,
    11. Gao T,
    12. Xuan M,
    13. Huang P,
    14. Zhang B,
    15. Xu X,
    16. Zhang M
    (2022) Microstructural but not macrostructural cortical degeneration occurs in Parkinson's disease with mild cognitive impairment. NPJ Parkinsons Dis 8:151. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41531-022-00416-6 pmid:36351910
    OpenUrlPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Basser PJ,
    2. Pierpaoli C
    (1996) Microstructural and physiological features of tissues elucidated by quantitative-diffusion-tensor MRI. J Magn Reson B 111:209–219. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmrb.1996.0086 pmid:8661285
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Blumenfeld-Katzir T,
    2. Pasternak O,
    3. Dagan M,
    4. Assaf Y
    (2011) Diffusion MRI of structural brain plasticity induced by a learning and memory task. PLoS One 6:e20678. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0020678 pmid:21701690
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Brodt S,
    2. Gais S,
    3. Beck J,
    4. Erb M,
    5. Scheffler K,
    6. Schönauer M
    (2018) Fast track to the neocortex: a memory engram in the posterior parietal cortex. Science 362:1045–1048. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aau2528 pmid:30498125
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  15. ↵
    1. Brown MB
    (1975) A method for combining non-independent, one-sided tests of significance. Biometrics 31:987. https://doi.org/10.2307/2529826
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  16. ↵
    1. Caruyer E,
    2. Lenglet C,
    3. Sapiro G,
    4. Deriche R
    (2013) Design of multishell sampling schemes with uniform coverage in diffusion MRI. Magn Reson Med 69:1534–1540. https://doi.org/10.1002/mrm.24736 pmid:23625329
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Cinar O,
    2. Viechtbauer W
    (2022) The poolr package for combining independent and dependent p values. J Stat Soft 101:42. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v101.i01
    OpenUrl
  18. ↵
    1. Coelho S,
    2. Baete SH,
    3. Lemberskiy G,
    4. Ades-Aron B,
    5. Barrol G,
    6. Veraart J,
    7. Novikov DS,
    8. Fieremans E
    (2022) Reproducibility of the standard model of diffusion in white matter on clinical MRI systems. Neuroimage 257:119290. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.119290 pmid:35545197
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Colgan N,
    2. Siow B,
    3. O'Callaghan JM,
    4. Harrison IF,
    5. Wells JA,
    6. Holmes HE,
    7. Ismail O,
    8. Richardson S,
    9. Alexander DC,
    10. Collins EC,
    11. Fisher EM,
    12. Johnson R,
    13. Schwarz AJ,
    14. Ahmed Z,
    15. O'Neill MJ,
    16. Murray TK,
    17. Zhang H,
    18. Lythgoe MF
    (2016) Application of neurite orientation dispersion and density imaging (NODDI) to a tau pathology model of Alzheimer's disease. Neuroimage 125:739–744. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.043 pmid:26505297
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Daducci A,
    2. Canales-Rodríguez EJ,
    3. Zhang H,
    4. Dyrby TB,
    5. Alexander DC,
    6. Thiran J-P
    (2015) Accelerated microstructure imaging via convex optimization (AMICO) from diffusion MRI data. Neuroimage 105:32–44. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.10.026 pmid:25462697
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Dayan E,
    2. Cohen LG
    (2011) Neuroplasticity subserving motor skill learning. Neuron 72:443–454. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2011.10.008 pmid:22078504
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Farrell JAD,
    2. Landman BA,
    3. Jones CK,
    4. Smith SA,
    5. Prince JL,
    6. van Zijl PCM,
    7. Mori S
    (2007) Effects of signal-to-noise ratio on the accuracy and reproducibility of diffusion tensor imaging-derived fractional anisotropy, mean diffusivity, and principal eigenvector measurements at 1.5 T. J Magn Reson Imaging 26:756–767. https://doi.org/10.1002/jmri.21053 pmid:17729339
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Fisher RA
    (1932) Statistical methods for research workers. Edinburgh: Oliver and Boyd.
  24. ↵
    1. Fukutomi H,
    2. Glasser MF,
    3. Zhang H,
    4. Autio JA,
    5. Coalson TS,
    6. Okada T,
    7. Togashi K,
    8. van Essen DC,
    9. Hayashi T
    (2018) Neurite imaging reveals microstructural variations in human cerebral cortical gray matter. Neuroimage 182:488–499. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.02.017 pmid:29448073
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Fukutomi H,
    2. Glasser MF,
    3. Murata K,
    4. Akasaka T,
    5. Fujimoto K,
    6. Yamamoto T,
    7. Autio JA,
    8. Okada T,
    9. Togashi K,
    10. Zhang H,
    11. van Essen DC,
    12. Hayashi T
    (2019) Diffusion tensor model links to neurite orientation dispersion and density imaging at high b-value in cerebral cortical gray matter. Sci Rep 9:12246. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-48671-7 pmid:31439874
    OpenUrlPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Gaser C,
    2. Dahnke R,
    3. Thompson PM,
    4. Kurth F,
    5. Luders E
    (2022) CAT – a computational anatomy toolbox for the analysis of structural MRI data. bioRxiv 495736. https://doi.org/10.1101/2022.06.11.495736.
  27. ↵
    1. Glasser MF,
    2. Coalson TS,
    3. Robinson EC,
    4. Hacker CD,
    5. Harwell J,
    6. Yacoub E,
    7. Ugurbil K,
    8. Andersson J,
    9. Beckmann CF,
    10. Jenkinson M,
    11. Smith SM,
    12. van Essen DC
    (2016) A multi-modal parcellation of human cerebral cortex. Nature 536:171–178. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature18933 pmid:27437579
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    1. Gong NJ,
    2. Dibb R,
    3. Pletnikov M,
    4. Benner E,
    5. Liu C
    (2020) Imaging microstructure with diffusion and susceptibility MR: neuronal density correlation in disrupted-in-schizophrenia-1 mutant mice. NMR Biomed 33:e4365. https://doi.org/10.1002/nbm.4365 pmid:32627266
    OpenUrlPubMed
  29. ↵
    1. Greve DN,
    2. Fischl B
    (2009) Accurate and robust brain image alignment using boundary-based registration. Neuroimage 48:63–72. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.060 pmid:19573611
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Grussu F,
    2. Schneider T,
    3. Tur C,
    4. Yates RL,
    5. Tachrount M,
    6. Ianuş A,
    7. Yiannakas MC,
    8. Newcombe J,
    9. Zhang H,
    10. Alexander DC,
    11. DeLuca GC,
    12. Gandini Wheeler-Kingshott CAM
    (2017) Neurite dispersion: a new marker of multiple sclerosis spinal cord pathology? Ann Clin Transl Neurol 4:663–679. https://doi.org/10.1002/acn3.445 pmid:28904988
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Guerrero JM,
    2. Adluru N,
    3. Bendlin BB,
    4. Goldsmith HH,
    5. Schaefer SM,
    6. Davidson RJ,
    7. Kecskemeti SR,
    8. Zhang H,
    9. Alexander AL
    (2019) Optimizing the intrinsic parallel diffusivity in NODDI: an extensive empirical evaluation. PLoS One 14:e0217118. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0217118 pmid:31553719
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. Hayashi-Takagi A,
    2. Yagishita S,
    3. Nakamura M,
    4. Shirai F,
    5. Wu YI,
    6. Loshbaugh AL,
    7. Kuhlman B,
    8. Hahn KM,
    9. Kasai H
    (2015) Labelling and optical erasure of synaptic memory traces in the motor cortex. Nature 525:333–338. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature15257 pmid:26352471
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Helms G,
    2. Dathe H,
    3. Kallenberg K,
    4. Dechent P
    (2008) High-resolution maps of magnetization transfer with inherent correction for RF inhomogeneity and T1 relaxation obtained from 3D FLASH MRI. Magn Reson Med 60:1396–1407. https://doi.org/10.1002/mrm.21732 pmid:19025906
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Hopkins WG
    (2000) Measures of reliability in sports medicine and science. Sports Med 30:1–15. https://doi.org/10.2165/00007256-200030010-00001 pmid:10907753
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Hwang FJ,
    2. Roth RH,
    3. Wu YW,
    4. Sun Y,
    5. Kwon DK,
    6. Liu Y,
    7. Ding JB
    (2022) Motor learning selectively strengthens cortical and striatal synapses of motor engram neurons. Neuron 110:2790–2801.e5. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2022.06.006 pmid:35809573
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Ivry R
    (1996) Representational issues in motor learning: phenomena and theory. In: Handbook of perception and action (Keele SW, Heuer H, eds), pp 263–330. London: Academic Press.
  37. ↵
    1. Jensen JH,
    2. Helpern JA,
    3. Ramani A,
    4. Lu H,
    5. Kaczynski K
    (2005) Diffusional kurtosis imaging: the quantification of non-gaussian water diffusion by means of magnetic resonance imaging. Magn Reson Med 53:1432–1440. https://doi.org/10.1002/mrm.20508 pmid:15906300
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    1. Johnson JW
    (2000) A heuristic method for estimating the relative weight of predictor variables in multiple regression. Multivariate Behav Res 35:1–19. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327906MBR3501_1 pmid:26777229
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Jones DK,
    2. Knösche TR,
    3. Turner R
    (2013) White matter integrity, fiber count, and other fallacies: the do's and don'ts of diffusion MRI. Neuroimage 73:239–254. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.081 pmid:22846632
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Kamiya K,
    2. Hori M,
    3. Aoki S
    (2020) NODDI in clinical research. J Neurosci Methods 346:108908. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2020.108908 pmid:32814118
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. Keifer OP,
    2. Hurt RC,
    3. Gutman DA,
    4. Keilholz SD,
    5. Gourley SL,
    6. Ressler KJ
    (2015) Voxel-based morphometry predicts shifts in dendritic spine density and morphology with auditory fear conditioning. Nat Commun 6:7582. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms8582 pmid:26151911
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. Kraguljac NV,
    2. Guerreri M,
    3. Strickland MJ,
    4. Zhang H
    (2023) Neurite orientation dispersion and density imaging in psychiatric disorders: a systematic literature review and a technical note. Biol Psychiatry Glob Open Sci 3:10–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsgos.2021.12.012 pmid:36712566
    OpenUrlPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Lazari A,
    2. Lipp I
    (2021) Can MRI measure myelin? Systematic review, qualitative assessment, and meta-analysis of studies validating microstructural imaging with myelin histology. Neuroimage 230:117744. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.117744 pmid:33524576
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    1. Leemans A,
    2. Jones DK
    (2009) The B-matrix must be rotated when correcting for subject motion in DTI data. Magn Reson Med 61:1336–1349. https://doi.org/10.1002/mrm.21890 pmid:19319973
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    1. Lehmann N,
    2. Villringer A,
    3. Taubert M
    (2020) Colocalized white matter plasticity and increased cerebral blood flow mediate the beneficial effect of cardiovascular exercise on long-term motor learning. J Neurosci 40:2416–2429. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2310-19.2020 pmid:32041897
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  46. ↵
    1. Lehmann N,
    2. Aye N,
    3. Kaufmann J,
    4. Heinze H-J,
    5. Düzel E,
    6. Ziegler G,
    7. Taubert M
    (2021) Longitudinal reproducibility of neurite orientation dispersion and density imaging (NODDI) derived metrics in the white matter. Neuroscience 457:165–185. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2021.01.005 pmid:33465411
    OpenUrlPubMed
  47. ↵
    1. Lehmann N,
    2. Villringer A,
    3. Taubert M
    (2022) Priming cardiovascular exercise improves complex motor skill learning by affecting the trajectory of learning-related brain plasticity. Sci Rep 12:1107. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-05145-7 pmid:35064175
    OpenUrlPubMed
  48. ↵
    1. Lerch JP,
    2. Yiu AP,
    3. Martinez-Canabal A,
    4. Pekar T,
    5. Bohbot VD,
    6. Frankland PW,
    7. Henkelman RM,
    8. Josselyn SA,
    9. Sled JG
    (2011) Maze training in mice induces MRI-detectable brain shape changes specific to the type of learning. Neuroimage 54:2086–2095. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.086 pmid:20932918
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    1. Lerch JP,
    2. van der Kouwe AJW,
    3. Raznahan A,
    4. Paus T,
    5. Johansen-Berg H,
    6. Miller KL,
    7. Smith SM,
    8. Fischl B,
    9. Sotiropoulos SN
    (2017) Studying neuroanatomy using MRI. Nat Neurosci 20:314–326. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4501 pmid:28230838
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    1. Leys C,
    2. Ley C,
    3. Klein O,
    4. Bernard P,
    5. Licata L
    (2013) Detecting outliers: do not use standard deviation around the mean, use absolute deviation around the median. J Exp Soc Psychol 49:764–766. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2013.03.013
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  51. ↵
    1. Madhyastha T,
    2. Mérillat S,
    3. Hirsiger S,
    4. Bezzola L,
    5. Liem F,
    6. Grabowski T,
    7. Jäncke L
    (2014) Longitudinal reliability of tract-based spatial statistics in diffusion tensor imaging. Hum Brain Mapp 35:4544–4555. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22493 pmid:24700773
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    1. Mak E,
    2. Holland N,
    3. Jones PS,
    4. Savulich G,
    5. Low A,
    6. Malpetti M,
    7. Kaalund SS,
    8. Passamonti L,
    9. Rittman T,
    10. Romero-Garcia R,
    11. Manavaki R,
    12. Williams GB,
    13. Hong YT,
    14. Fryer TD,
    15. Aigbirhio FI,
    16. O'Brien JT,
    17. Rowe JB
    (2021) In vivo coupling of dendritic complexity with presynaptic density in primary tauopathies. Neurobiol Aging 101:187–198. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2021.01.016 pmid:33631470
    OpenUrlPubMed
  53. ↵
    1. Manning WH,
    2. Dubois PH
    (1962) Correlational methods in research on human learning. Percept Mot Skills 15:287–321. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1962.15.2.287
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  54. ↵
    1. Markham JA,
    2. Greenough WT
    (2004) Experience-driven brain plasticity: beyond the synapse. Neuron Glia Biol 1:351–363. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1740925x05000219 pmid:16921405
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    1. Matsuda K,
    2. Nagasaka K,
    3. Kato J,
    4. Takashima I,
    5. Higo N
    (2022) Structural plasticity of motor cortices assessed by voxel-based morphometry and immunohistochemical analysis following internal capsular infarcts in macaque monkeys. Cereb Cortex Commun 3:tgac046. https://doi.org/10.1093/texcom/tgac046 pmid:36457456
    OpenUrlPubMed
  56. ↵
    1. McGraw KO,
    2. Wong SP
    (1996) Forming inferences about some intraclass correlation coefficients. Psychol Methods 1:30–46. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.1.1.30
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  57. ↵
    1. McKenzie IA,
    2. Ohayon D,
    3. Li H,
    4. Faria JP,
    5. de Emery B,
    6. Tohyama K,
    7. Richardson WD
    (2014) Motor skill learning requires active central myelination. Science 346:318–322. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1254960 pmid:25324381
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  58. ↵
    1. Mediavilla T,
    2. Özalay Ö,
    3. Estévez-Silva HM,
    4. Frias B,
    5. Orädd G,
    6. Sultan FR,
    7. Brozzoli C,
    8. Garzón B,
    9. Lövdén M,
    10. Marcellino DJ
    (2022) Learning-related contraction of gray matter in rodent sensorimotor cortex is associated with adaptive myelination. Elife 11:e77432. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.77432
    OpenUrl
  59. ↵
    1. Mollink J,
    2. Kleinnijenhuis M,
    3. van Cappellen Walsum A-M,
    4. Sotiropoulos SN,
    5. Cottaar M,
    6. Mirfin C,
    7. Heinrich MP,
    8. Jenkinson M,
    9. Pallebage-Gamarallage M,
    10. Ansorge O,
    11. Jbabdi S,
    12. Miller KL
    (2017) Evaluating fibre orientation dispersion in white matter: comparison of diffusion MRI, histology and polarized light imaging. Neuroimage 157:561–574. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.06.001 pmid:28602815
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  60. ↵
    1. Mugler JP,
    2. Brookeman JR
    (1990) Three-dimensional magnetization-prepared rapid gradient-echo imaging (3D MP RAGE). Magn Reson Med 15:152–157. https://doi.org/10.1002/mrm.1910150117 pmid:2374495
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  61. ↵
    1. Natu VS,
    2. Gomez J,
    3. Barnett M,
    4. Jeska B,
    5. Kirilina E,
    6. Jaeger C,
    7. Zhen Z,
    8. Cox S,
    9. Weiner KS,
    10. Weiskopf N,
    11. Grill-Spector K
    (2019) Apparent thinning of human visual cortex during childhood is associated with myelination. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 116:20750–20759. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1904931116 pmid:31548375
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  62. ↵
    1. Nazeri A,
    2. Chakravarty MM,
    3. Rotenberg DJ,
    4. Rajji TK,
    5. Rathi Y,
    6. Michailovich OV,
    7. Voineskos AN
    (2015) Functional consequences of neurite orientation dispersion and density in humans across the adult lifespan. J Neurosci 35:1753–1762. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3979-14.2015 pmid:25632148
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  63. ↵
    1. Nazeri A,
    2. Schifani C,
    3. Anderson JAE,
    4. Ameis SH,
    5. Voineskos AN
    (2020) In vivo imaging of gray matter microstructure in major psychiatric disorders: opportunities for clinical translation. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging 5:855–864. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2020.03.003 pmid:32381477
    OpenUrlPubMed
  64. ↵
    1. Orrell AJ,
    2. Eves FF,
    3. Masters RSW
    (2006) Implicit motor learning of a balancing task. Gait Posture 23:9–16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2004.11.010 pmid:16311189
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  65. ↵
    1. Perez-Alvarez A,
    2. Navarrete M,
    3. Covelo A,
    4. Martin ED,
    5. Araque A
    (2014) Structural and functional plasticity of astrocyte processes and dendritic spine interactions. J Neurosci 34:12738–12744. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2401-14.2014 pmid:25232111
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  66. ↵
    1. Pierpaoli C,
    2. Basser PJ
    (1996) Toward a quantitative assessment of diffusion anisotropy. Magn Reson Med 36:893–906. https://doi.org/10.1002/mrm.1910360612 pmid:8946355
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  67. ↵
    1. Poldrack RA
    (2000) Imaging brain plasticity: conceptual and methodological issues—a theoretical review. Neuroimage 12:1–13. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2000.0596 pmid:10875897
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  68. ↵
    1. Reveley C,
    2. Ye FQ,
    3. Mars RB,
    4. Matrov D,
    5. Chudasama Y,
    6. Leopold DA
    (2022) Diffusion MRI anisotropy in the cerebral cortex is determined by unmyelinated tissue features. Nat Commun 13:6702. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-022-34328-z pmid:36335105
    OpenUrlPubMed
  69. ↵
    1. Roalf DR,
    2. Quarmley M,
    3. Elliott MA,
    4. Satterthwaite TD,
    5. Vandekar SN,
    6. Ruparel K,
    7. Gennatas ED,
    8. Calkins ME,
    9. Moore TM,
    10. Hopson R,
    11. Prabhakaran K,
    12. Jackson CT,
    13. Verma R,
    14. Hakonarson H,
    15. Gur RC,
    16. Gur RE
    (2016) The impact of quality assurance assessment on diffusion tensor imaging outcomes in a large-scale population-based cohort. Neuroimage 125:903–919. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.068 pmid:26520775
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  70. ↵
    1. Rosenzweig MR,
    2. Bennett EL
    (1996) Psychobiology of plasticity: effects of training and experience on brain and behavior. Behav Brain Res 78:57–65. https://doi.org/10.1016/0166-4328(95)00216-2 pmid:8793038
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  71. ↵
    1. Sagi Y,
    2. Tavor I,
    3. Hofstetter S,
    4. Tzur-Moryosef S,
    5. Blumenfeld-Katzir T,
    6. Assaf Y
    (2012) Learning in the fast lane: new insights into neuroplasticity. Neuron 73:1195–1203. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2012.01.025 pmid:22445346
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  72. ↵
    1. Sampaio-Baptista C,
    2. Khrapitchev AA,
    3. Foxley S,
    4. Schlagheck T,
    5. Scholz J,
    6. Jbabdi S,
    7. DeLuca GC,
    8. Miller KL,
    9. Taylor A,
    10. Thomas N,
    11. Kleim J,
    12. Sibson NR,
    13. Bannerman D,
    14. Johansen-Berg H
    (2013) Motor skill learning induces changes in white matter microstructure and myelination. J Neurosci 33:19499–19503. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3048-13.2013 pmid:24336716
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  73. ↵
    1. Sato K,
    2. Kerever A,
    3. Kamagata K,
    4. Tsuruta K,
    5. Irie R,
    6. Tagawa K,
    7. Okazawa H,
    8. Arikawa-Hirasawa E,
    9. Nitta N,
    10. Aoki I,
    11. Aoki S
    (2017) Understanding microstructure of the brain by comparison of neurite orientation dispersion and density imaging (NODDI) with transparent mouse brain. Acta Radiol Open 6:2058460117703816. https://doi.org/10.1177/2058460117703816 pmid:28491462
    OpenUrlPubMed
  74. ↵
    1. Schilling KG,
    2. Janve V,
    3. Gao Y,
    4. Stepniewska I,
    5. Landman BA,
    6. Anderson AW
    (2018) Histological validation of diffusion MRI fiber orientation distributions and dispersion. Neuroimage 165:200–221. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.10.046 pmid:29074279
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  75. ↵
    1. Schmidt S,
    2. Gull S,
    3. Herrmann K-H,
    4. Boehme M,
    5. Irintchev A,
    6. Urbach A,
    7. Reichenbach JR,
    8. Klingner CM,
    9. Gaser C,
    10. Witte OW
    (2021) Experience-dependent structural plasticity in the adult brain: how the learning brain grows. Neuroimage 225:117502. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117502 pmid:33164876
    OpenUrlPubMed
  76. ↵
    1. Scholz J,
    2. Klein MC,
    3. Behrens TEJ,
    4. Johansen-Berg H
    (2009) Training induces changes in white-matter architecture. Nat Neurosci 12:1370–1371. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2412 pmid:19820707
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  77. ↵
    1. Sepehrband F,
    2. Clark KA,
    3. Ullmann JFP,
    4. Kurniawan ND,
    5. Leanage G,
    6. Reutens DC,
    7. Yang Z
    (2015) Brain tissue compartment density estimated using diffusion-weighted MRI yields tissue parameters consistent with histology. Hum Brain Mapp 36:3687–3702. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22872 pmid:26096639
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  78. ↵
    1. Smith SM,
    2. Nichols TE
    (2009) Threshold-free cluster enhancement: addressing problems of smoothing, threshold dependence and localisation in cluster inference. Neuroimage 44:83–98. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.061 pmid:18501637
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  79. ↵
    1. Smith SM,
    2. Zhang Y,
    3. Jenkinson M,
    4. Chen J,
    5. Matthews PM,
    6. Federico A,
    7. Stefano N de
    (2002) Accurate, robust, and automated longitudinal and cross-sectional brain change analysis. Neuroimage 17:479–489. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1040 pmid:12482100
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  80. ↵
    1. Smith SM,
    2. Jenkinson M,
    3. Woolrich MW,
    4. Beckmann CF,
    5. Behrens TEJ,
    6. Johansen-Berg H,
    7. Bannister PR,
    8. Luca M,
    9. de Drobnjak I,
    10. Flitney DE,
    11. Niazy RK,
    12. Saunders J,
    13. Vickers J,
    14. Zhang Y,
    15. Stefano N,
    16. de Brady JM,
    17. Matthews PM
    (2004) Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage 23 Suppl 1]:S208–S219. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051 pmid:15501092
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  81. ↵
    1. Smith SM,
    2. Jenkinson M,
    3. Johansen-Berg H,
    4. Rueckert D,
    5. Nichols TE,
    6. Mackay CE,
    7. Watkins KE,
    8. Ciccarelli O,
    9. Cader MZ,
    10. Matthews PM,
    11. Behrens TEJ
    (2006) Tract-based spatial statistics: voxelwise analysis of multi-subject diffusion data. Neuroimage 31:1487–1505. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.024 pmid:16624579
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  82. ↵
    1. Steiger JH
    (1980) Tests for comparing elements of a correlation matrix. Psychol Bull 87:245–251. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.87.2.245
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  83. ↵
    1. Strick PL,
    2. Dum RP,
    3. Rathelot J-A
    (2021) The cortical motor areas and the emergence of motor skills: a neuroanatomical perspective. Annu Rev Neurosci 44:425–447. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-070918-050216 pmid:33863253
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  84. ↵
    1. Szucs D,
    2. Ioannidis JPA
    (2020) Sample size evolution in neuroimaging research: an evaluation of highly-cited studies (1990-2012) and of latest practices (2017-2018) in high-impact journals. Neuroimage 221:117164. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117164 pmid:32679253
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  85. ↵
    1. Tardif CL,
    2. Gauthier CJ,
    3. Steele CJ,
    4. Bazin P-L,
    5. Schäfer A,
    6. Schaefer A,
    7. Turner R,
    8. Villringer A
    (2016) Advanced MRI techniques to improve our understanding of experience-induced neuroplasticity. Neuroimage 131:55–72. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.08.047 pmid:26318050
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  86. ↵
    1. Taubert M,
    2. Draganski B,
    3. Anwander A,
    4. Müller K,
    5. Horstmann A,
    6. Villringer A,
    7. Ragert P
    (2010) Dynamic properties of human brain structure: learning-related changes in cortical areas and associated fiber connections. J Neurosci 30:11670–11677. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2567-10.2010 pmid:20810887
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  87. ↵
    1. Taubert M,
    2. Mehnert J,
    3. Pleger B,
    4. Villringer A
    (2016) Rapid and specific gray matter changes in M1 induced by balance training. Neuroimage 133:399–407. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.03.017 pmid:26994831
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  88. ↵
    1. Tavor I,
    2. Hofstetter S,
    3. Assaf Y
    (2013) Micro-structural assessment of short term plasticity dynamics. Neuroimage 81:1–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.050 pmid:23702416
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  89. ↵
    1. Thomas AG,
    2. Marrett S,
    3. Saad ZS,
    4. Ruff DA,
    5. Martin A,
    6. Bandettini PA
    (2009) Functional but not structural changes associated with learning: an exploration of longitudinal voxel-based morphometry (VBM). Neuroimage 48:117–125. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.097 pmid:19520171
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  90. ↵
    1. Thomas C,
    2. Baker CI
    (2013) Teaching an adult brain new tricks: a critical review of evidence for training-dependent structural plasticity in humans. Neuroimage 73:225–236. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.069 pmid:22484409
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  91. ↵
    1. Tonidandel S,
    2. LeBreton JM
    (2015) RWA web: a free, comprehensive, web-based, and user-friendly tool for relative weight analyses. J Bus Psychol 30:207–216. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10869-014-9351-z
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  92. ↵
    1. Vukovic N,
    2. Hansen B,
    3. Lund TE,
    4. Jespersen S,
    5. Shtyrov Y
    (2021) Rapid microstructural plasticity in the cortical semantic network following a short language learning session. PLoS Biol 19:e3001290. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3001290 pmid:34125828
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  93. ↵
    1. Wang N,
    2. Zhang J,
    3. Cofer G,
    4. Qi Y,
    5. Anderson RJ,
    6. White LE,
    7. Johnson GA
    (2019) Neurite orientation dispersion and density imaging of mouse brain microstructure. Brain Struct Funct 224:1797–1813. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-019-01877-x pmid:31006072
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  94. ↵
    1. Weiskopf N,
    2. Edwards LJ,
    3. Helms G,
    4. Mohammadi S,
    5. Kirilina E
    (2021) Quantitative magnetic resonance imaging of brain anatomy and in vivo histology. Nat Rev Phys 3:570–588. https://doi.org/10.1038/s42254-021-00326-1
    OpenUrl
  95. ↵
    1. Wenger E,
    2. Brozzoli C,
    3. Lindenberger U,
    4. Lövdén M
    (2017) Expansion and renormalization of human brain structure during skill acquisition. Trends Cogn Sci 21:930–939. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.09.008 pmid:29149999
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  96. ↵
    1. Winkler AM,
    2. Ridgway GR,
    3. Webster MA,
    4. Smith SM,
    5. Nichols TE
    (2014) Permutation inference for the general linear model. Neuroimage 92:381–397. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.060 pmid:24530839
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  97. ↵
    1. Winkler AM,
    2. Webster MA,
    3. Brooks JC,
    4. Tracey I,
    5. Smith SM,
    6. Nichols TE
    (2016) Non-parametric combination and related permutation tests for neuroimaging. Hum Brain Mapp 37:1486–1511. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23115 pmid:26848101
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  98. ↵
    1. Wulf G,
    2. McNevin N,
    3. Shea CH
    (2001) The automaticity of complex motor skill learning as a function of attentional focus. Q J Exp Psychol A 54:1143–1154. https://doi.org/10.1080/713756012 pmid:11765737
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  99. ↵
    1. Xu T,
    2. Yu X,
    3. Perlik AJ,
    4. Tobin WF,
    5. Zweig JA,
    6. Tennant K,
    7. Jones T,
    8. Zuo Y
    (2009) Rapid formation and selective stabilization of synapses for enduring motor memories. Nature 462:915–919. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature08389 pmid:19946267
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  100. ↵
    1. Zatorre RJ,
    2. Fields RD,
    3. Johansen-Berg H
    (2012) Plasticity in gray and white: neuroimaging changes in brain structure during learning. Nat Neurosci 15:528–536. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3045 pmid:22426254
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  101. ↵
    1. Zhang H,
    2. Schneider T,
    3. Wheeler-Kingshott CAM,
    4. Alexander DC
    (2012) NODDI: practical in vivo neurite orientation dispersion and density imaging of the human brain. Neuroimage 61:1000–1016. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.072 pmid:22484410
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 43 (50)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 43, Issue 50
13 Dec 2023
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Masthead (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Changes in Cortical Microstructure of the Human Brain Resulting from Long-Term Motor Learning
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Changes in Cortical Microstructure of the Human Brain Resulting from Long-Term Motor Learning
Nico Lehmann, Norman Aye, Jörn Kaufmann, Hans-Jochen Heinze, Emrah Düzel, Gabriel Ziegler, Marco Taubert
Journal of Neuroscience 13 December 2023, 43 (50) 8637-8648; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0537-23.2023

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Changes in Cortical Microstructure of the Human Brain Resulting from Long-Term Motor Learning
Nico Lehmann, Norman Aye, Jörn Kaufmann, Hans-Jochen Heinze, Emrah Düzel, Gabriel Ziegler, Marco Taubert
Journal of Neuroscience 13 December 2023, 43 (50) 8637-8648; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0537-23.2023
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • brain microstructure
  • diffusion
  • MRI
  • motor learning
  • neuroplasticity
  • NODDI
  • skill acquisition

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Articles

  • Neural sensitivity to radial frequency patterns in the visual cortex of developing macaques
  • Unveiling cortical criticality changes along the prodromal to the overt continuum of alpha-synucleinopathy
  • Stress-sensitization of neurons in the dentate gyrus is dependent on neuronal interleukin-1 receptor signaling and is associated with increased synaptic plasticity, perineuronal nets, and excitatory/inhibitory input imbalance.
Show more Research Articles

Development/Plasticity/Repair

  • Developmental Changes in Brain Cellular Membrane and Energy Metabolism: A Multi-Occasion 31P Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy Study
  • The Epigenetic Reader PHF23 Is Required for Embryonic Neurogenesis
  • Microglia Support Both the Singular Form of LTP Expressed by the Lateral Perforant Path and Episodic Memory
Show more Development/Plasticity/Repair
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Accessibility
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.