Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Featured ArticleResearch Articles, Behavioral/Cognitive

Seven Tesla Evidence for Columnar and Rostral–Caudal Organization of the Human Periaqueductal Gray Response in the Absence of Threat: A Working Memory Study

Alexandra K. Fischbach, Ajay B. Satpute, Karen Quigley, Philip A. Kragel, Danlei Chen, Marta Bianciardi, Larry Wald, Tor D. Wager, Ji-Kyung Choi, Jiahe Zhang, Lisa Feldman Barrett and Jordan E. Theriault
Journal of Neuroscience 26 June 2024, 44 (26) e1757232024; https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1757-23.2024
Alexandra K. Fischbach
1Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, Massachusetts 02115
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Alexandra K. Fischbach
Ajay B. Satpute
1Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, Massachusetts 02115
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Ajay B. Satpute
Karen Quigley
1Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, Massachusetts 02115
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Philip A. Kragel
2Department of Psychology, Emory University, Atlanta, Georgia 30322
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Philip A. Kragel
Danlei Chen
1Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, Massachusetts 02115
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Danlei Chen
Marta Bianciardi
3Department of Radiology, Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Charlestown, Massachusetts 02129
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Marta Bianciardi
Larry Wald
3Department of Radiology, Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Charlestown, Massachusetts 02129
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Larry Wald
Tor D. Wager
4Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Dartmouth College, Hanover, New Hampshire 03755
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Tor D. Wager
Ji-Kyung Choi
5Department of Surgery, University of California, San Francisco, California 94143
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Jiahe Zhang
1Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, Massachusetts 02115
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Lisa Feldman Barrett
1Department of Psychology, Northeastern University, Boston, Massachusetts 02115
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Jordan E. Theriault
3Department of Radiology, Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Charlestown, Massachusetts 02129
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

The periaqueductal gray (PAG) is a small midbrain structure that surrounds the cerebral aqueduct, regulates brain–body communication, and is often studied for its role in “fight-or-flight” and “freezing” responses to threat. We used ultra-high-field 7 T fMRI to resolve the PAG in humans and distinguish it from the cerebral aqueduct, examining its in vivo function during a working memory task (N = 87). Both mild and moderate cognitive demands elicited spatially similar patterns of whole-brain blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) response, and moderate cognitive demand elicited widespread BOLD increases above baseline in the brainstem. Notably, these brainstem increases were not significantly greater than those in the mild demand condition, suggesting that a subthreshold brainstem BOLD increase occurred for mild cognitive demand as well. Subject-specific masks were group aligned to examine PAG response. In PAG, both mild and moderate demands elicited a well-defined response in ventrolateral PAG, a region thought to be functionally related to anticipated painful threat in humans and nonhuman animals—yet, the present task posed only the most minimal (if any) “threat,” with the cognitive tasks used being approximately as challenging as remembering a phone number. These findings suggest that the PAG may play a more general role in visceromotor regulation, even in the absence of threat.

  • brainstem
  • high resolution
  • neuroimaging
  • periaqueductal gray
  • working memory

Significance Statement

The periaqueductal gray (PAG) is thought to control survival-related behavior and is typically studied using experiments that manipulate threat. Others have proposed that the PAG plays a more general role in bodily regulation, but studies examining PAG function outside of threat-based experimental contexts are rare. We used high-resolution fMRI to examine PAG response in humans during a working memory task, which involves minimal threat. Moderate cognitive demands elicited a well-defined response in ventrolateral PAG, a functional subregion thought to coordinate a “freezing” response to threat. A task where threat is minimal elicited a clear fMRI response in one of the most well-known survival circuits in the brain, which suggests the PAG supports a more general function in brain–body coordination.

Introduction

Historically, the periaqueductal gray (PAG) has been identified as a key brain structure mediating the “fight-or-flight” and “freezing” responses (for review, see Bandler, 1988; Bandler et al., 1991a), and this functional association of the PAG with threat responses continues to be articulated in recent reviews (Mobbs et al., 2015, 2020; Silva and McNaughton, 2019). Direct chemical stimulation of the PAG elicits a diverse range of motor behaviors and physiological responses—including, in cats, immobility, pupil dilation, vocalization (e.g., hissing or howling), alert positions (Bandler and Carrive, 1988), running and jumping (Zhang et al., 1990), and hypertension and tachycardia (Abrahams et al., 1960), all of which are generally understood as part of the “fight-or-flight” and “freezing” responses (Bandler et al., 1991b; Bandler and Depaulis, 1991; Fanselow, 1994). Likewise, in humans, the PAG is most commonly studied for its role in threat (Coker-Appiah et al., 2013; Hermans et al., 2013; Lindner et al., 2015; Weis et al., 2020; Zhou et al., 2021; Wang et al., 2022; Weis et al., 2022). Deep brain stimulation of the human PAG elicits a desire to escape (Amano et al., 1978), feelings of fear and impending death, and autonomic changes including hyperventilation and increased heart rate (Nashold et al., 1969). Brain imaging studies in humans observed that blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) signal intensity increases in the PAG when participants were chased through a virtual maze (i.e., approached by a simulated predator; Mobbs et al., 2007) and when exposed to a learned cue signaling the onset of an unpleasant respiratory restriction (Faull et al., 2016, 2019). The interpretation of the PAG as a primary region for coordinating survival-based responses has been used to unify these many observations under a single coherent function (Fanselow, 1991; Silva et al., 2016), in which the PAG was thought to play a central role in an evolutionarily ancient “fear circuit” (Panksepp, 2011; Silva et al., 2016; LeDoux and Daw, 2018; McNaughton and Corr, 2018; Lu et al., 2022; Vázquez-León et al., 2023).

The hypothesis that the PAG is a center for survival-based responses unifies a great deal of evidence, but not all evidence. For example, the PAG has been observed to play a functional role in bodily processes at moments when threat is absent (for review, see Carrive, 1993), including control of the heart (M. A. Gray et al., 2009; Wager et al., 2009b; Pereira et al., 2010; Hermans et al., 2013; Hohenschurz-Schmidt et al., 2020), blood pressure control (Green et al., 2005), cardiorespiratory coordination (Dampney et al., 2013), bladder control (Blok et al., 1997; Blok and Holstege, 1998), rapid eye movement sleep (J. Lu et al., 2006), body temperature (Zhang et al., 1997; Chen et al., 2002), trigeminovascular contributions to migraine headache (Knight and Goadsby, 2001), communication (e.g., the vocalization of songbirds; Haakenson et al., 2020), mating behavior (e.g., the control of male copulatory behavior in quails; Carere et al., 2007), and feeding behaviors (e.g., the motivational drive to hunt in mice; Mota-Ortiz et al., 2012; Tryon and Mizumori, 2018). Furthermore, the PAG has extensive connectivity with regions across the entire neuraxis, from the spinal cord and the brainstem to the cerebral cortex, placing it at a critical integration point in brain–body communication (for review, see Carrive and Morgan, 2012). Given this evidence, we hypothesized that the well-known involvement of PAG in response to predatory threat is consistent with a more fundamental function: the PAG may coordinate and regulate internal bodily (visceromotor) systems in service of efficient regulation of the body (called allostasis). On this interpretation, threat responses represent only one of many contexts where the autonomic nervous system, the endocrine system, the immune system, and tissues of the body must be coordinated with each other and with motor movements (for review, see Benarroch, 2012; Motta et al., 2017). If this is correct, and the PAG does play a more general regulatory role (rather than a threat-/survival-specific role), then organized patterns of PAG response may be observed in task-based contexts involving effort, but minimal threat.

Critically, well-known columnar functional distinctions within the PAG set some expectations for how its response is organized. Within the PAG, prior work has identified dorsomedial (dmPAG), dorsolateral (dlPAG), lateral (lPAG), and ventrolateral (vlPAG) functional columns (Bandler et al., 1991a; Carrive, 1993). In the context of threat-/survival-based research, dl/lPAG is thought to support active coping “fight-or-flight” responses to predatory threats, whereas dmPAG is thought to support similar responses to aggressive conspecifics, and vlPAG is thought to support passive coping or “freezing” responses to painful threats (Zhang et al., 1990; Morgan et al., 1998; Gross and Canteras, 2012). For example, stimulating dl/lPAG, in rodents and cats, increases cardiac rates and respiratory rates (RRs; Bandler et al., 1991c; Carrive and Bandler, 1991; Depaulis et al., 1992; Zhang et al., 2005; Subramanian et al., 2008) and elicits explosive running and jumping (Fardin et al., 1984; Zhang et al., 1990; Bandler et al., 1991c; Morgan et al., 1998). In contrast, stimulating vlPAG decreases cardiac rates and RRs (Bandler et al., 1991c; Carrive and Bandler, 1991; Depaulis et al., 1992; Lovick, 1992; Subramanian et al., 2008) and induces an absence of movement in cats (Zhang et al., 1990) and “freezing” behavior in rats (Carrive et al., 1997; Morgan et al., 1998). In humans, anticipated breathlessness increases the BOLD signal in vlPAG (Faull et al., 2016, 2019; Tinoco Mendoza et al., 2023), whereas subcutaneous (deep muscle) and cutaneous pain increases the BOLD signal in vlPAG and lPAG, respectively (Tinoco Mendoza et al., 2023). Observations that correspond to this functional localization along PAG columns would increase our confidence in an observed pattern of results, given that these functional distinctions have been strongly established in prior work.

In the present study, we examined changes in PAG activity in the context of a mildly-to-moderately challenging N-back working memory task, which aims to simulate energetic, cognitive, and physiological demands within the range of those typically encountered in daily life. Participants observed a sequence of letters and responded with a key press when a letter was repeated, either from the prior trial (1-back) or three trials prior (3-back; Fig. 1). This task required that participants simultaneously attend to and remember target stimuli while inhibiting or completing task-relevant motor responses. The 1-back task could be conceptually described as easier than remembering a phone number, and so we consider it to fall well outside the traditional survival-based contexts in which PAG function has previously been tested.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Schematic overview of the working memory N-back task. An example of a 1-back (mild cognitive demand) and 3-back (moderate cognitive demand) task showing a sequence of trials. For every trial, a single letter was presented at the center of the visual field for 1 s. Following the 1 s stimuli presentation, participants were given an additional 1 s to respond before the next trial began. Participants were instructed to respond with a button press if the target stimulus matched the stimulus n (either 1 or 3) positions back.

Imaging resolution in 1.5 and 3 T MRI scanners is insufficient to resolve small brainstem nuclei, due to both low signal-to-noise ratios (SNRs) and voxel resolution. In contrast, ultra-high resolution 7 T imaging, used in the present work, provides a substantial increase in the SNR (Linnman et al., 2012; Satpute et al., 2013). An earlier report included the first 24 participants from the current dataset (all the participants who were available for analysis at that time) and used univariate and multivariate methods to distinguish PAG BOLD responses to 1-back (mild cognitive demand) and 3-back (moderate cognitive demand) conditions (Kragel et al., 2019). This earlier study reported greater BOLD signal intensity increases in the 3-back (vs the 1-back) condition, which were localized to the right rostral vlPAG. The present work builds on this earlier report, with a sample size (N = 87) that affords the power to identify functional distinctions among PAG columns. We also improved our localization procedure, identifying PAG columnar subregions by their radial degree. As in prior work (Kragel et al., 2019), we aligned subject-specific PAG masks to a group template to estimate the voxelwise BOLD response during the N-back task. For PAG columns, tract-tracing techniques in nonhuman animals typically produce symmetrical results (Carrive et al., 1987; Zhang et al., 1990; Carrive and Bandler, 1991; Li et al., 1993; Sandkühler and Herdegen, 1995; Holstege et al., 1997; Jansen et al., 1998; Paredes et al., 2000), and so we expected that PAG activity would be symmetric along the PAG midline. We recorded cardiac interbeat interval (IBI) and RR to examine whether columnar organization supported visceromotor functions and whether this visceromotor support could possibly account for any task-elicited changes observed during the working memory task.

Materials and Methods

Participants

One hundred forty participants were recruited from the Greater Boston area and provided informed consent in accordance with guidelines set forth by the Partners’ Healthcare Institutional Review Board. Consented participants completed two scanning sessions and were paid $150 for each scan (i.e., $300 upon study completion). Eighteen participants either withdrew or ended the MRI scan session before starting the working memory task. Nineteen participants were excluded due to poor image quality (e.g., large artifacts compromising analysis), as assessed by visual inspection in Mango version 4.1 (RRID: SCR_009603). Sixteen participants were excluded for excessive head motion [>0.5 mm framewise displacement in >20% of repetition times (TRs) in the run; see below, MRI preprocessing]. Our final sample consisted of 87 participants (Mage = 27.0 years; SDage = 6.1 years; 39 female, 47 male, 1 nonresponse). For self-reported ethnicity, 13% of participants identified as Hispanic. For self-reported race, 62% identified as White, 25% identified as Asian, and 9% identified as Black. Of the 87 participants, three did not provide a response for race, and one did not provide a response for ethnicity. The participant sample was generally highly educated (24% had some graduate education, 25% had completed college/university, 37% had some college/university, 10% had completed high school, and 2% had not completed high school). Peripheral physiological recordings were inspected for artifacts (see below, Peripheral physiological recording and quality assessment), and only nonartifactual recordings were included in analyses. Of the 87 participants with usable fMRI data, 59 had artifact-free cardiac data, and 47 had artifact-free respiratory data.

All participants were between 18 and 40 years old, right-handed, fluent English speakers, with normal or corrected to normal vision and no known neurological or psychiatric illnesses. Participants were excluded from participating if they were pregnant, were claustrophobic, or had any metal implants that could cause harm during scanning.

Experimental paradigm

Participants completed a visual N-back working memory task during fMRI scanning, based on the experimental design of prior studies (Kirchner, 1958; Gevins and Cutillo, 1993; Gray et al., 2003; Wager and Smith, 2003; van Ast et al., 2016). On each trial, a letter (Q, W, R, S, or T) was presented at the center of the visual field for 1 s followed by a blank screen for 1 s (Fig. 1). Participants were instructed to respond with a button press when the letter on the screen matched the letter presented n trials ago where n was either 1 or 3. The task was administered during a single scanning run, across 12 blocks, each consisting of 10 trials (120 trials total). Each block began with a 3 s cue indicating the current trial type (1-back or 3-back). The 1-back and 3-back blocks were presented in 1-3-3-1 or 3-1-1-3 order (counterbalanced across participants), and each block was followed by 25 s of resting fixation. Stimuli were classified into two categories; a “target” was a stimulus that met the N-back criteria for a match (e.g., N-back either one or three trials ago), and all other stimuli were classified as “nontargets.” The task had fixed proportions of 20% targets and 80% nontargets. Of the nontargets, 12.5% were classified as lure trials—i.e., 2-back matches in the 1-back blocks and 2- or 4-back matches in the 3-back blocks. The proportion of lure trials was the same for both 1-back and 3-back blocks.

The task was administered in MATLAB (MathWorks; RRID:SCR_001622), using the Psychophysics Toolbox extensions (Kleiner, 2007; RRID:SCR_002881). Stimuli were made visible to participants by projecting them onto a mirror attached to the head coil. Responses were recorded using an MR-compatible button box.

To familiarize participants with the task, participants completed a practice N-back task session on a laptop immediately before the scan session. These practice sessions mirrored the task used during scanning, including blocks of 1-back and 3-back trials, for a total of 48 trials.

MRI data acquisition

Gradient-echo planar imaging (EPI) BOLD-fMRI was performed on a 7 T Siemens MRI scanner at the Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging at Massachusetts General Hospital. The scanner was built by Magnex Scientific, with the MRI console, gradient and gradient drivers, and patient table provided by Siemens. Functional images were acquired using the GRAPPA-EPI (generalized autocallibrating partially parallel aquisitions) sequence: echo time = 28 ms; TR = 2.34 s; flip angle = 75°; slice orientation = transversal (axial), anterior to posterior phase encoding; voxel size = 1.1 mm isotropic; gap between slices = 0 mm; number of axial slices = 123; field of view = 205 × 205 mm2; GRAPPA acceleration factor = 3; echo spacing = 0.82 ms; bandwidth = 1,414 Hz per pixel; and partial Fourier in the phase encode direction = 7/8. A custom-built 32-channel radiofrequency coil head array was used for reception. Radiofrequency transmission was provided by a detunable bandpass birdcage coil.

Structural images were acquired using a T1-weighted EPI sequence, selected so that functional and structural images had similar spatial distortions, which facilitated coregistration and subsequent normalization of data to the MNI space (Renvall et al., 2016). Structural scan parameters were as follows: echo time = 22 ms; TR = 8.52 s; flip angle = 90°; slice orientation = transversal (axial); voxel size = 1.1 mm isotropic; gap between slices = 0 mm; number of axial slices = 126; field of view = 205 × 205 mm2; GRAPPA acceleration factor = 3; echo spacing = 0.82 ms; bandwidth = 1,414 Hz per pixel; and partial Fourier in the phase encode direction = 6/8.

MRI preprocessing

Functional and structural MRI data were preprocessed using the fmriprep pipeline, version 1.1.2 (Esteban et al., 2019, 2020; RRID: SCR_016216), a Nipype-based tool (Gorgolewski et al., 2011; Esteban et al., 2020; RRID: SCR_002502). Full pipeline details can be found at https://fmriprep.org/en/1.1.2/workflows.html. Spatial normalization of anatomical data to the 2009c ICBM 152 Nonlinear Asymmetrical template (Fonov et al., 2009; RRID: SCR_008796) was performed through nonlinear registration with the antsRegistration tool of ANTs version 2.1.0 (Avants et al., 2008; RRID: SCR_004757), using brain-extracted versions of both T1w (T1-weighted) volume and template. Brain tissue segmentation of the cerebrospinal fluid (CSF), white matter (WM), and gray matter (GM) was performed on the brain-extracted T1w using FAST in FSL version 5.0.9 (Zhang et al., 2001; RRID: SCR_002823). Functional data were slice time corrected using 3dTshift from AFNI version 16.2.07 (Cox, 1996; RRID: SCR_005927) and motion corrected using MCFLIRT (FSL version 5.0.9; Jenkinson et al., 2002). This was followed by coregistration to the corresponding T1w using boundary-based registration (Greve and Fischl, 2009) with nine degrees of freedom, using FLIRT (FMRIB’s Linear Image Registration Tool; FSL version 5.0.9; Jenkinson and Smith, 2001; Jenkinson et al., 2002). Motion-correcting transformations, BOLD-to-T1w transformation, and T1w-to-template (MNI) warp were concatenated and applied in a single step using antsApplyTransforms (ANTs version 2.1.0) using Lanczos interpolation. Physiological noise regressors were extracted using the aCompCor method (Muschelli et al., 2014), taking the top five principal components from subject-specific CSF and WM masks, where the masks were generated by thresholding the WM/CSF masks derived from FAST at 99% probability, constraining the CSF mask to the ventricles (using the ALVIN mask; Kempton et al., 2011), and eroding the WM mask using the binary_erosion function in SciPy version 1.6 (Virtanen et al., 2020). Framewise displacement (Power et al., 2014) was calculated for each functional run using the implementation of Nipype. Many internal operations of fmriprep use Nilearn (Abraham et al., 2014; RRID: SCR_001362), principally within the BOLD-processing workflow. For all participants, the quality of brain masks, tissue segmentation, and MNI registration was visually inspected for errors using the HTML figures provided by the fmriprep pipeline.

fMRI analysis

To estimate BOLD signal intensity during the working memory N-back task, a general linear model was applied to preprocessed functional time-series (GLM; FSL version 5.0.9). Subject-level models provided beta weight estimates for 1-back and 3-back conditions within each participant, relative to baseline fixation. All trial regressors were aligned to trial onset (duration = 1 s) and convolved with a double gamma hemodynamic response function. Because trials were closely spaced, the convolved 1-back and 3-back regressors approximated a block-based design, meaning that the observed PAG responses should be interpreted as an average across the 1-back/3-back block period (as opposed to more time-sensitive responses to the letter presented on each trial). Subject-level nuisance regressors included a run intercept, motion (i.e., translation/rotation in x/y/z planes), aCompCor components (five CSF, five WM; Muschelli et al., 2014), a discrete cosine transformation set with a minimum period of 264 s, and spike regressors (>0.5 mm framewise displacement; Satterthwaite et al., 2013). To assess multicollinearity, variance inflation factors (VIFs) were inspected for 1-back (VIFmean = 1.33; VIFSD = 0.16) and 3-back regressors (VIFmean = 1.48; VIFSD = 0.28), and were considered to be within an acceptable range (i.e., < 5; Mumford et al., 2015). Group-level analyses included whole-brain contrasts and analyses within PAG voxels (see below, PAG identification and alignment). Whole-brain contrasts computed z-scores relative to 0 for subject-level beta weights after smoothing with a 1 mm FWHM Gaussian kernel. Thresholds for z-score maps were corrected for multiple comparisons using threshold-free cluster-enhancement (TFCE; α < 0.05; Smith and Nichols, 2009).

PAG identification and alignment

The PAG was identified and aligned across subjects using a semiautomated iterative procedure (for details, see Kragel et al., 2019), which built on manual PAG alignment methods previously used in 7 T studies of the PAG (for details, see Satpute et al., 2013). For each subject, a subject-specific mask of the cerebral aqueduct was created by identifying high-variance voxels in the region. Following this, subject-specific PAG masks were created by dilating the aqueduct mask (two voxels, i.e., 2.2 mm) and masking (1) voxels in the original subject-specific aqueduct mask, (2) voxels not in a subject-specific GM segmentation (>50% probability), and (3) any voxels outside the target range (MNI, −22 > y > −42; z > −14). This created a hollow, cylindrical PAG mask for each subject, which were aligned and warped to a group-average mask using DARTEL (differomorphic anatomical registration using exponentiated Lie algebra; Ashburner, 2007). Subject-group transformations were applied to whole-brain contrast images to align subject-level functional data to the common PAG-aligned space. For all analyses within the PAG, voxels were resampled to 0.5 mm isotropic space (from 1.1 mm isotropic native space), masked with the group-aligned PAG mask, and smoothed to 1 mm.

Each voxel within the group-aligned PAG mask was tagged according to (1) its radial degree from the PAG midline, (2) its position along the rostral–caudal PAG axis, and (3) its radial distance from the center of the cylinder. Figure 2B depicts the “unrolled” PAG, with radial degrees on the x-axis, and rostral–caudal position on the y-axis. Figure 2C depicts the “unrolled” PAG “sliced” according to the radial distance from the center of the cylinder. The rostral–caudal position was calculated using the principal component analysis (PCA) on voxelwise x/y/z coordinates, and the radial degree and distance were calculated using a geometric transformation of those PCA coordinates, giving degree relative to the volumetric y-axis (at 0°) and cylinder center (at radial distance = 0). For visualization, Figure 2 depicts the functional columns within the PAG (Fig. 2); however, all group-level analyses were performed using either voxelwise data (Figs. 2, 5A) or bilateral ROIs aggregating across radial degrees or the rostral–caudal axis (Fig. 5B; see below, Linear mixed-effects model). The ventral most 90° of the PAG was excluded from analysis, given that it contains other subcortical structures (e.g., dorsal raphe).

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

PAG identification, segmentation, and visualization. A, Sagittal and coronal perspectives on the human PAG. For the purposes of visualization, functional columns within the PAG are colored according to the legend. The ventral 90° of the cylinder contains the dorsal raphe and was excluded from analysis (B) The PAG is a 3D structure and is difficult to visualize. To simplify the presentation of results, we labeled each voxel with its position on the rostral–caudal axis and its radial degree from the volumetric y-axis. Plotting the rostral–caudal position (y-axis) by the radial degree (x-axis) flattens the PAG into a 2D surface. This convention will be used in later figures. C, The flattened map of PAG voxels can be simplified further by taking radial slices within the PAG. Each voxel was labeled with its radial distance from the center of the cylinder, and the radial distance is plotted panels depicting 1 mm bins. For example, the 0–1 mm panel shows voxels abutting the cerebral aqueduct, and the 3–4 mm panel shows voxels at the outer periphery of the PAG. As in (B), the y-axis illustrates the longitudinal (rostral–caudal) axis of the PAG. Within each bin, radial degrees are plotted on the x-axis.

Linear mixed-effects model

Voxel beta weights within the PAG group-aligned mask were submitted to a linear mixed-effects model to summarize the spatial distribution of BOLD signal changes. PAG voxel beta weights were averaged within four ranks of the rostral–caudal position and 10 ranks of the radial degree (Fig. 5B). Each rostral–caudal rank consisted of a 3.5 mm segment along the 14 mm rostral–caudal positional axis, and each radial degree rank consisted of a bilateral average of 13.5° sections of the PAG—i.e., the first radial degree rank included 0–13.5° in the left and right PAG, while the last radial degree rank included 121.5–135° in the left and right PAG. Given this, our mixed-effects model assumes bilaterality in any effect along the PAG radial degree (an assumption we confirmed by visual inspection of the voxelwise pattern of results; Fig. 5A).

The mixed-effects model included fixed effects of condition (1-back/3-back), rostral–caudal rank, PAG degree rank, and all interactions. The model included by-participant random intercepts and by-participant random slopes for all fixed effects (Barr et al., 2013). Analyses were conducted in R (R version 3.6.2; R Core Team, 2016) using the lmer4 package (Bates et al., 2015), with degrees of freedom approximated by the Satterthwaite method, as implemented in the lmerTest package (Kuznetsova et al., 2017). The code was entered as follows:model<−lmer(averagedPAGBOLD∼condition*rostral–caudalposition*PAGdegree+(condition*rostral–caudalposition*PAGdegree|participant),control=lmerControl(optimizer

= “bobyqa”, optCtrl = list(maxfun = 2e5))).

Peripheral physiological recording and quality assessment

Peripheral physiological measures were collected at 1 kHz using an ADInstruments PowerLab data acquisition system with MR-compatible sensors and LabChart software. Data were continuously acquired throughout the entire scan session and partitioned for alignment with fMRI data using experimenter annotations in LabChart and scanner TR events. A piezoelectric pulse transducer (PowerLab, ADInstruments) measured the heart rate from the left index fingertip. A respiratory belt with a piezoelectric transducer (UFI) measured RR and was placed around the chest at the lower end of the sternum.

Physiological time-series data were visually inspected for quality in Biopac Systems’ AcqKnowledge data acquisition and analysis software and in custom visualizations using R (R version 3.6.2; R Core Team, 2016) and the ggplot2 package (version 3.2.1; Wickham, 2009). Time-series were classified as high quality (i.e., few or no motion artifacts), noisy (i.e., IBI, motion artifacts affected <50% of each 1 min segment; RR, large, frequent motion artifacts or RRs outside of a typical range of 7–20 breaths per min), and unusable (i.e., IBI, containing excessive artifacts with few reliably detectable peaks; RR, containing low amplitudes that could not be distinguished from noise). Among the 87 participants with usable fMRI data, cardiac data was high quality (i.e., all 1 min segments were sufficiently free of motion to easily identify peaks in the IBI signal) for 59 participants, noisy for 12, unusable for 14, and missing for 2. Among the 87 participants with usable fMRI data, respiratory data was high quality for 47 participants, noisy for 20, unusable for 14, and missing for 6. Only high-quality data were used for cardiac (IBI) and respiratory (RR) analyses.

Peripheral physiological preprocessing and analysis

Physiological time-series data were analyzed using MATLAB toolboxes and custom R scripts. The heart rate was calculated using the PhysIO MATLAB toolbox (Kasper et al., 2017), which used a 0.3–9 Hz bandpass filter, and identified peaks using a two-pass process. The first-pass estimated run average heart rate (inverse of IBI) detects peaks exceeding 40% of normalized amplitude, assuming a minimum peak spacing <90 beats per minute. Moreover, the first-pass peaks were used to create an averaged template, and the first-pass estimated average heart rate was used as a prior to detect peaks on the second-pass peaks (for more details, see Kasper et al., 2017). PhysIO pipeline-detected peaks were compared with peaks identified in Biopac by trained coders. Discrepancies between the two methods were rare (0.5% of peaks across all runs and participants) and occurred very rarely in the dataset scored by a more experienced coder (0.323% of peaks) compared with the dataset scored by a less experienced coder (0.878% of peaks). To reduce drift in the cardiac signal, heart rates were smoothed with a 6 s rolling average and down-sampled into scan TR. Upon completion of preprocessing, we then converted the heart rate into IBI, as IBI displays a more linear relationship with underlying autonomic cardiac inputs than the heart rate (Berntson et al., 1995). After converting HR (heart rate) to IBI, cardiac data were analyzed exclusively using IBI. RRs were calculated using custom R scripts. A 1 Hz low-pass filter was applied to the respiratory time-series, and local peaks were identified in a sliding 500 ms window. Small excursions (those <0.5 SD of average respiratory belt tension across the entire run) were deemed too small to meet criteria as a respiratory peak or trough. To maintain consistency between preprocessing of IBI and RR, RRs were also smoothed with a 6 s rolling average and down-sampled into scan TR. Down-sampling IBI and RR into scan TR resulted in a total of 263 captured instances of physiological activity for each variable (IBI, RR) in every participant; averages were then computed for each participant and for each resting fixation, 1-back and 3-back task epoch (see above, Experimental paradigm). Using the averaged task epoch values, change scores were computed as the difference in physiological activity (IBI, RR) between the resting fixation and the 1-back and 3-back task epochs.

Results

Performance and reaction time increased in the 1-back versus 3-back working memory task

As expected, and consistent with the earlier work using a small subset of this sample, performance (i.e., hit rates) was higher for 1-back trials (M = 0.99; SD = 0.02) than for 3-back trials [M = 0.89; SD = 0.08; t(85) = 12.18; p < 1e−16; d = 1.31]. Likewise, reaction times for correct responses were higher for 1-back trials (M = 0.76 s; SD = 0.16 s) than for 3-back trials [M = 0.99 s; SD = 0.22 s; t(85) = 13.75; p < 1e−16; d = 1.48]. Reaction times for incorrect responses did not significantly differ between 1-back and 3-back conditions.

Peripheral physiology was affected by 1-back and 3-back working memory conditions

Group-level changes in peripheral physiology (IBI and RR) were compared with the resting baseline during 1-back and 3-back task epochs. Although previous analyses using a subset of the present sample did not observe any change in peripheral physiology from baseline (Kragel et al., 2019), we did observe changes in both 1-back and 3-back conditions (Fig. 3). In the 1-back condition, across participants, IBI increased (i.e., cardio-deceleration occurred), and reparatory rate increased compared with the resting baseline [IBI: t(58) = 3.64, p < 0.0006, d = 0.47; RR: t(46) = 4.75, p < 1e−5, d = 0.69]. In the 3-back condition, IBI decreased (i.e., cardio-acceleration occurred) and RR increased compared with the resting baseline [IBI: t(58) = 3.82, p < 0.0003, d = 0.50; RR: t(46) = 9.07, p < 1e−12, d = 1.32]. Relative to the 1-back condition, the 3-back IBI was shorter [t(58) = 5.21; p < 1e−6; d = 0.68], and the 3-back respiration rate was faster [t(46) = 6.07; p < 1e−7; d = 0.89]. Thus, both mildly and moderately difficult cognitive tasks elicited physiological changes: they both elicited increased RRs, and they elicited opposite cardiac responses, with the mildly difficult 1-back reducing the heart rate and the moderately difficult 3-back increasing it.

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Task-dependent physiological changes from baseline. Group means and 95% confidence intervals are plotted in red for IBI and RR (respiration rate; see above, peripheral physiological recording and quality assessment). Black dots illustrate individual mean change across all 1-back (left) or 3-back (right) task periods. Note that a decrease in IBI reflects an increase in the heart rate. *** p < 0.001.

Whole-brain contrast elicits expected pattern of cortical results and widespread brainstem activity in the 3-back condition

Whole-brain group-level analyses for 1-back, 3-back, and 3-back > 1-back contrasts were performed to ensure that cortical results were consistent with observations in prior work (Kragel et al., 2019). Group-level analyses were performed on first-level contrasts after applying a transformation to align the subject-specific PAG masks (see Materials and Methods, PAG identification and alignment), The 3-back > 1-back contrast was consistent with prior work, with bilateral responses observed in the inferior frontal junction, intraparietal sulcus, dorsal anterior insula, supplemental motor area, posterior parietal cortex, and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Fig. 4A). Notably, unlike in Kragel et al. (2019), we did not observe a suprathreshold response in the PAG for the 3-back > 1-back contrast. However, the 3-back > baseline contrast did elicit suprathreshold activity throughout the brainstem, including within the PAG (Fig. 4B).

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Group-level patterns in whole-brain and brainstem contrasts. A, Whole-brain contrasts for 1-back > fixation, 3-back > fixation, and 3-back > 1-back contrasts. Subject-level estimates were smoothed with a 1 mm FWHM Gaussian kernel and submitted to a group-level GLM. Thresholding was performed using threshold-free cluster enhancement (α < 0.05; Smith and Nichols, 2009). The 1-back and 3-back > fixation contrasts generally showed a similar pattern within the cortex, with the exception in the posterior insula and posterior parietal cortex, where no 1-back response exceeding the TFCE threshold was observed. Cortical results for the 3-back > 1-back contrast were similar to those observed in prior work (Kragel et al., 2019), except that no 3-back > 1-back difference was observed in the PAG. B, The 3-back > fixation contrast elicited widespread BOLD activity in the brainstem and in a number of voxels (marked in black) within the group-aligned PAG mask (marked in white). Notably, there was no 3-back > 1-back difference observed in the PAG or brainstem, suggesting that subtle changes above baseline were present in the 1-back condition as well. Voxelwise analyses within the PAG (Fig. 5) allowed us to examine this further. All subject-level contrasts were warped to a common PAG mask template to align PAG voxels at the group level (see Materials and Methods, PAG identification and alignment).

The 1-back and 3-back (vs baseline) contrasts elicited similar patterns of BOLD response across the cortex (Fig. 4A), meaning that the 3-back > 1-back contrast estimates were generally caused by a difference in the intensity of the BOLD signal increase/decrease between the conditions. In other words, a similar set of brain regions responded to 1-back and 3-back conditions, but the magnitude of increase/decrease was greater for the 3-back condition (with the exceptions of the posterior parietal cortex and posterior insula, where no suprathreshold differences between the 1-back and resting baseline were observed; Fig. 4A). The fact that the 3-back condition elicited brainstem and PAG responses that exceeded baseline, but that did not differ from the 1-back response, suggested the presence of a subthreshold PAG response in the 1-back < baseline contrast as well. Given that the cortical response in 1-back and 3-back conditions were similar in their topographic organization, but different in their intensities, it was possible that a similar topographic similarity would be observed in the PAG as well. Extracting and analyzing the voxelwise topography of the BOLD response in the PAG allowed us to test this hypothesis.

The voxelwise topography of the PAG BOLD response for 1-back and 3-back working memory conditions is strongly correlated

Voxelwise z-scores for the 1-back and 3-back > baseline contrasts were plotted on a 2D surface to visualize the topography of the PAG BOLD response (Fig. 5A). Visual inspection of these results suggested that the 1-back and 3-back contrasts did elicit a PAG BOLD response that was spatially similar, but different in intensity, as in the cortical pattern of results. Supporting this topographic similarity, voxelwise z-scores were highly correlated between 1-back and 3-back contrasts; r = 0.76 (0.75, 0.77; bootstrapped 95% CI, 10,000 samples; Fig. 5A). To model the topographic pattern of the PAG response, we assigned two ordinal ranks to each PAG voxel: a rostral–caudal rank (four levels, from caudal to rostral poles of the PAG; Fig. 5B) and a radial degree rank (10 levels, from dorsomedial to ventrolateral PAG, averaging bilaterally; Fig. 5B; see Materials and Methods, Linear mixed-effects model). These ordinal ranks, along with the N-back condition (1-back, 3-back) and all interactions, were submitted to a linear mixed-effects model (including subject-level intercepts and all random slopes; Barr et al., 2013) to model the spatial topography of the PAG BOLD beta weights in both N-back conditions.

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

Patterns of group-level PAG BOLD signal intensity elicited by the 1-back/3-back task. A, Left: Voxelwise topography of the PAG BOLD response (z-scored) during 1-back (left) and 3-back (right) conditions. Upper panels display radial slices of the PAG cylinder (each 1 mm thick; Fig. 2). The y-axis illustrates the voxel position along the rostral–caudal axis (14 mm in length). The x-axis illustrates the voxel position in ±135° from the brain midline. The heat map illustrates the group-level z-scored BOLD signal intensity relative to baseline fixation between task blocks. Voxels were interpolated to 0.5 mm isotropic, from 1.1 mm native space, and smoothed with a 1 mm FWHM Gaussian kernel. Right: Beta weights for all 1-back PAG voxels (y-axis) plotted against beta weights for all 3-back PAG voxels (x-axis). A Pearson’s correlation coefficient and 95% confidence interval were calculated using 10,000 bootstrapped samples. Across voxels, 1-back and 3-back z-scores were highly correlated, similar to 1-back and 3-back cortical responses, as reported in Fig. 4. B, To model PAG topography in the 1-back and 3-back conditions, ordinal ranks were assigned to voxels according to their position on the rostral–caudal axis (4 ranks, left) and radial degree (10 ranks, right). Radial degree ranks were bilateral, meaning that our analysis assumed symmetry in any PAG response [which is visually supported by the topography in panel (a)]. In each subject, voxels in each combination of rostral–caudal and degree ranks were averaged to produce 40 estimates for each subject to be used in the mixed-effects model. C, Subject-level beta weights were averaged and plotted (with 95% confidence intervals) for each combination of rostral–caudal and degree ranks. The y-axis, unlike in the plots above, depicts group-averaged beta weights (in arbitrary units, a.u.), and the x-axis depicts the radial degree rank (i.e., bilateral dorsomedial to ventrolateral regions, moving from left to right). In both 1-back (left) and 3-back (right) conditions, the rostral end of the PAG showed a general BOLD increase above fixation, whereas the caudal end of the PAG showed a dorsomedial decrease and increased BOLD response in ventrolateral areas. These plotted averages are consistent with the results of the mixed effects model (see Results).

The main effect of the N-back condition was nonsignificant, [F(1,86) = 2.12; p < 0.15], consistent with the failure to observe any 3-back > 1-back difference within the PAG in the whole-brain contrast (Fig. 4B). The main effect of the rostral–caudal rank was significant, [F(1,86) = 52.61; p < 1e−9], but was qualified by an interaction with the N-back condition, [F(1,86) = 7.88; p < 0.007], suggesting that the linear increase along the rostral–caudal axis of the PAG occurred in both N-back conditions, but was greater in the 3-back condition than in the 1-back condition (Fig. 5C; y-axis increases from pink to green categories). The main effect of the degree rank was also significant, [F(1,86) = 41.92; p < 1e−8], and was also qualified by an interaction with the N-back condition [F(1,86) = 4.62; p < 0.035], suggesting that the linear increase from the dorsomedial to ventrolateral PAG occurred in both N-back conditions, but was greater in the 3-back condition than in the 1-back condition (Fig. 5C; y-axis increases from left to right along the x-axis). The magnitude of the linear increase from the dorsomedial to ventrolateral PAG also increased from the caudal to rostral end of the PAG (Fig. 5C), as indicated by an interaction between rostral–caudal rank and degree rank, [F(1,86) = 26.03; p < 1e−5], but no significant three-way interaction with the N-back condition was observed [F(1,86) = 3.64; p > 0.05], meaning that the spatial topography of the BOLD response in 1-back and 3-back conditions was similar along the dimensions of the rostral–caudal axis and radial degrees, and that the conditions did differ in the magnitude of change in the BOLD response along either spatial dimension of the PAG (Fig. 5C).

Task-elicited changes in the peripheral physiological response correlate with PAG voxels, but the voxelwise topography of correlation is weakly related to task-elicited BOLD responses

The PAG is implicated in physiological regulation, and we expected that task-elicited changes in the PAG BOLD signal would correlate with changes (from baseline) in cardiac IBI and RR. To identify voxels that were associated with the magnitude of task-elicited physiological change from baseline, in each voxel, subject-level beta weights for 1-back and 3-back > baseline contrasts were correlated with subject-level changes in IBI and RR (Fig. 6A). The voxelwise spatial topography of the correlation coefficients was less clearly distinguishable along the expected organizational pattern of functional PAG columns than in the 1-back and 3-back > baseline contrasts (Fig. 5A). To directly compare these two patterns of voxelwise topography (i.e., voxelwise correlations with physiological change, Fig. 6A, and voxelwise task-elicited BOLD increases/decreases, Fig. 5A), we plotted the estimates against each other (Fig. 6B). We observed moderate correlations across PAG voxels between task-elicited BOLD and its correlation with changes in cardiac IBI (Fig. 6B, top), but weaker or negative correlations with change in RR (Fig. 6B, bottom). In all cases, the similarity in topography was much weaker than the similarity between the PAG BOLD signals elicited in the 1-back and 3-back tasks.

Figure 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 6.

Voxelwise correlation with task-elicited physiological changes. A, Voxelwise topography of between-subject correlations between either cardiac IBI (top) or RR (bottom) and estimated voxelwise PAG BOLD signal intensity (i.e., beta weight) elicited by the 1-back (left) and 3-back (right) task. Upper panels display radial slices of the PAG cylinder (each 1 mm thick; Fig. 2). The y-axis illustrates the voxel position along the rostral–caudal axis (14 mm in length). The x-axis illustrates the voxel position in ±135° from the brain midline. The heat map illustrates Pearson’s correlation coefficients. Correlations were calculated across all subjects with artifact-free physiological data (cardiac IBI = 69; RR = 47). B, Voxelwise BOLD-physiological change correlation coefficients for IBI change (y-axis, top) and RR change (y-axis bottom) are plotted against contrast beta weights for all 1-back PAG voxels (x-axis, left) and 3-back PAG voxels (x-axis, right). Pearson’s correlation coefficients and 95% confidence intervals were calculated using 10,000 bootstrapped samples. Correlations across voxels were nonzero, but substantially lower than the correlation between the 1-back and 3-back voxelwise topography of BOLD response reported in Fig. 5.

Discussion

Our results suggest that, in humans, nonpainful and nonthreatening tasks that elicit mild physiological changes elicit a spatially organized pattern of BOLD response in the PAG. Further, the moderate and mild working memory demands of the 3-back and 1-back tasks elicited nearly identical patterns of BOLD response across the PAG—the 3-back response magnified the intensity of response, but did not change its voxelwise topography. In both 1-back and 3-back conditions, BOLD increases were observed along the entire rostral–caudal axis in the vlPAG, suggesting that the BOLD response to the cognitive task is organized along the previously known anatomical boundaries of PAG functional columns (for review, see Carrive and Morgan, 2012).

It was surprising that light cognitive exertion would elicit a localized BOLD response in the vlPAG, as previous studies have associated the function of the vlPAG with survival-related stimuli, including stimuli eliciting “freezing” behavior (in rats; Carrive et al., 1997; Morgan et al., 1998), and anticipated breathlessness or subcutaneous pain in humans (Faull et al., 2016; Tinoco Mendoza et al., 2023). Given that the present work observed a clearly defined pattern of BOLD increase in the vlPAG in response to a task approximately as easy as remembering a phone number, we posit that the PAG plays a more general functional role than just supporting survival-related behavior (e.g., housing “fear” or “survival” circuits; Panksepp, 2011; Silva et al., 2016; LeDoux and Daw, 2018; McNaughton and Corr, 2018; Lu et al., 2022; Vázquez-León et al., 2023)—consistent with accumulating evidence that the PAG helps coordinate and regulate the internal (visceromotor) systems of the body (for similar perspectives, see Benarroch, 1993, 2001; Faull et al., 2019). In other words, although prior work has observed associations between PAG activity and “fight-or-flight” or “freezing” behavior in the context of threat, the overall function of the PAG may be better described by its support for autonomic regulation in many situations, even in contexts where survival-related threat is absent, such as the working memory context used in the present work.

Within the past decade, significant progress has been made in imaging the human PAG (Coker-Appiah et al., 2013; Hermans et al., 2013; Lindner et al., 2015; Weis et al., 2020; Zhou et al., 2021; Wang et al., 2022; Weis et al., 2022; Tinoco Mendoza et al., 2023); however, because the PAG has been associated with survival-related behavior, most experiments continue to explore its function in these threat-based contexts. In contrast, the N-back task used in the present work involves cognitive demands that are within the range of what is experienced in daily life and provides a context in which to probe the PAG BOLD response during less affectively evocative task conditions. Successful performance in a N-back working memory task requires integrating exteroceptive (e.g., visual stimuli) and interoceptive sensory signals (e.g., signals of energy status from the body), maintaining those signals across trials (i.e., working memory), mobilizing energy to meet changing task demands (e.g., as reflected in changes in cardiorespiratory reactivity; Thayer et al., 2012; Dampney et al., 2013; Berntson et al., 2017; Hoemann et al., 2020), maintaining goal-directed behavior (e.g., allocating attentional resources), and finally, generating and executing a motor plan (e.g., pushing a button to make a response). These brain functions support successful working memory performance, but they also support other complex, goal-directed sensorimotor behaviors. Indeed, these are the basic operations that are needed to perform many laboratory psychological tasks and engage in real-world motivated performance (i.e., situations that are goal-relevant to the performer, require instrumental cognitive responses, and are active rather than passive; Blascovich and Mendes, 2000). From this perspective, the working memory task is a motivated performance task, similar to other motivated performance tasks that have elicited a PAG BOLD response under more stressful conditions (e.g., social stress during preparation for public speaking; Wager et al., 2009a,b; Theriault et al., under review). The N-back task, then, in addition to being regarded as a prototypical “cognitive” task, can also be thought of as a task that drives the brain to mobilize resources to meet task demands—i.e., to engage in allostatic regulation (Barrett, 2017; Sennesh et al., 2022). The PAG is anatomically well positioned to contribute to this allostatic function—especially given the its anatomical position at the convergence of ascending viscerosensory signals from the body and descending visceromotor signals for controlling the body (for review, see Carrive and Morgan, 2012).

In sum, our observation that both mildly and moderately challenging cognitive tasks elicited similar patterns of BOLD response in the PAG is consistent with the emerging perspective that the PAG serves a general integrative function—as opposed to a survival-specific function—and may support the integration of bottom-up sensory signals from the body with top-down visceromotor control signals from the brain (for review, see Bandler et al., 2000; Benarroch, 2012; Gianaros and Wager, 2015; Koutsikou et al., 2017). This perspective was hindered by the rarity of studies probing PAG function outside of threat-related contexts, and so future work would benefit from exploring PAG function in naturalistic conditions, and in other experimental contexts, such as the N-back task, that drive the brain to mobilize resources for action. Such studies could identify subcortical responses that might be missed in experimental contexts that exclusively study rare and intense events that trigger a “fight-or-flight” response. Indeed, the fact that moderate cognitive demand elicited a BOLD increase across the entire brainstem (including the PAG; Fig. 4B) suggests that there is a great deal to learn about brainstem and subcortical contributions to cognition.

Footnotes

  • This work was supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health (U01 CA193632, R01 AG071173, R01 MH113234, R21 MH129902), the National Science Foundation (BCS 1947972), the U.S. Army Research Institute for the Behavioral and Social Sciences (W911NF-16-1-019), and the Unlikely Collaborators Foundation. The views, opinions, and/or findings contained in this manuscript are those of the authors and shall not be construed as an official Department of the Army position, policy, or decision, unless so designated by other documents, nor do they necessarily reflect the views of the Unlikely Collaborators Foundation.

  • ↵*L.F.B. and J.E.T. contributed equally to this work.

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Jordan Theriault at jtheriault2{at}mgh.harvard.edu.

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.

References

  1. ↵
    1. Abraham A,
    2. Pedregosa F,
    3. Eickenberg M,
    4. Gervais P,
    5. Mueller A,
    6. Kossaifi J,
    7. Gramfort A,
    8. Thirion B,
    9. Varoquaux G
    (2014) Machine learning for neuroimaging with scikit-learn. Front Neuroinform 8:1–10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fninf.2014.00014 pmid:24600388
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Abrahams VC,
    2. Hilton SM,
    3. Zbrożyna A
    (1960) Active muscle vasodilatation produced by stimulation of the brain stem: its significance in the defence reaction. J Physiol 154:491–513. https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1960.sp006593 pmid:13681101
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Amano K,
    2. Tanikawa T,
    3. Iseki H,
    4. Kawabatake H,
    5. Notani M,
    6. Kawamura H,
    7. Kitamura K
    (1978) Single neuron analysis of the human midbrain tegmentum. Stereotact Funct Neurosurg 41:66–78. https://doi.org/10.1159/000102402
    OpenUrl
  4. ↵
    1. Ashburner J
    (2007) A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. NeuroImage 38:95–113. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.007
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Avants BB,
    2. Epstein CL,
    3. Grossman M,
    4. Gee JC
    (2008) Symmetric diffeomorphic image registration with cross-correlation: evaluating automated labeling of elderly and neurodegenerative brain. Med Image Anal 12:26–41. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.media.2007.06.004 pmid:17659998
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Bandler R
    (1988) Brain mechanisms of aggression as revealed by electrical and chemical stimulation: suggestion of a central role for the midbrain periaqueductal grey region. In: Progress in psychobiology and physiological psychology (Epstein N, Morrison AR, eds), Vol. 13, pp 67–154. New York: Academic Press.
    OpenUrl
  7. ↵
    1. Bandler R,
    2. Carrive P
    (1988) Integrated defence reaction elicited by excitatory amino acid microinjection in the midbrain periaqueductal grey region of the unrestrained cat. Brain Res 439:95–106. https://doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(88)91465-5
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Bandler R,
    2. Carrive P,
    3. Depaulis A
    (1991a) Emerging principles of organization of the midbrain periaqueductal gray matter. In: The midbrain periaqueductal gray matter: functional, anatomical, and neurochemical organization (Depaulis A, Bandler R, eds), pp 1–8. New York: Springer US.
  9. ↵
    1. Bandler R,
    2. Carrive P,
    3. Zhang SP
    (1991b) Integration of somatic and autonomic reactions within the midbrain periaqueductal grey: Viscerotopic, somatotopic and functional organization. In: Prog Brain Res (Holstege G, ed), Vol. 87, pp 269–305. New York: Elsevier.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Bandler R,
    2. Carrive P,
    3. Zhang SP
    (1991c) Integration of somatic and autonomic reactions within the midbrain periaqueductal grey: viscerotopic, somatotopic and functional organization. Prog Brain Res 87:269–305. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0079-6123(08)63056-3
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Bandler R,
    2. Depaulis A
    (1991) Midbrain periaqueductal gray control of defensive behavior in the cat and the rat. In: The midbrain periaqueductal gray matter: functional, anatomical, and neurochemical organization (Depaulis A, Bandler R, eds), pp 175–198. New York: Springer US.
  12. ↵
    1. Bandler R,
    2. Keay KA,
    3. Floyd N,
    4. Price J
    (2000) Central circuits mediating patterned autonomic activity during active vs. passive emotional coping. Brain Res Bull 53:95–104. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0361-9230(00)00313-0
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Barr DJ,
    2. Levy R,
    3. Scheepers C,
    4. Tily HJ
    (2013) Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: keep it maximal. J Mem Lang 68:255–278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001 pmid:24403724
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Barrett LF
    (2017) The theory of constructed emotion: an active inference account of interoception and categorization. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 12:1–23. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw154 pmid:27798257
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Bates D,
    2. Mächler M,
    3. Bolker B,
    4. Walker S
    (2015) Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J Stat Softw 67:Article 1. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01
    OpenUrl
  16. ↵
    1. Benarroch EE
    (1993) The central autonomic network: functional organization, dysfunction, and perspective. Mayo Clin Proc 68:988–1001. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0025-6196(12)62272-1
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Benarroch EE
    (2001) Pain-autonomic interactions: a selective review. Clin Auton Res 11:343–349. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02292765
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Benarroch EE
    (2012) Periaqueductal gray: an interface for behavioral control. Neurology 78:210–217. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0b013e31823fcdee
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  19. ↵
    1. Berntson GG,
    2. Cacioppo JT,
    3. Quigley KS
    (1995) The metrics of cardiac chronotropism: biometric perspectives. Psychophysiology 32:162–171. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1995.tb03308.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Berntson GG,
    2. Quigley KS,
    3. Norman GJ,
    4. Lozano DL
    (2017) Cardiovascular psychophysiology. In: Handbook of psychophysiology (Cacioppo JT, Tassinary G, Berntson GG, eds), Ed 4, pp 183–216. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  21. ↵
    1. Blascovich J,
    2. Mendes WB
    (2000) Challenge and threat appraisals: the role of affective cues. In: Feeling and thinking: the role of affect in social cognition (Forgas JP, ed), pp 59–82. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  22. ↵
    1. Blok BFM,
    2. Holstege G
    (1998) The central nervous system control of micturition in cats and humans. Behav Brain Res 92:119–125. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4328(97)00184-8
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Blok BF,
    2. Willemsen AT,
    3. Holstege G
    (1997) A PET study on brain control of micturition in humans. Brain 120:111–121. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/120.1.111
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Carere C,
    2. Ball GF,
    3. Balthazart J
    (2007) Sex differences in projections from preoptic area aromatase cells to the periaqueductal gray in Japanese quail. J Comp Neurol 500:894–907. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.21210
    OpenUrlPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Carrive P
    (1993) The periaqueductal gray and defensive behavior: functional representation and neuronal organization. Behav Brain Res 58:27–47. https://doi.org/10.1016/0166-4328(93)90088-8
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Carrive P,
    2. Bandler R
    (1991) Viscerotopic organization of neurons subserving hypotensive reactions within the midbrain periaqueductal grey: a correlative functional and anatomical study. Brain Res 541:206–215. https://doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(91)91020-2
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Carrive P,
    2. Dampney RA,
    3. Bandler R
    (1987) Excitation of neurones in a restricted portion of the midbrain periaqueductal grey elicits both behavioural and cardiovascular components of the defence reaction in the unanaesthetised decerebrate cat. Neurosci Lett 81:273–278. https://doi.org/10.1016/0304-3940(87)90395-8
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    1. Carrive P,
    2. Leung P,
    3. Harris J,
    4. Paxinos G
    (1997) Conditioned fear to context is associated with increased Fos expression in the caudal ventrolateral region of the midbrain periaqueductal gray. Neuroscience 78:165–177. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0306-4522(97)83047-3
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    1. Carrive P,
    2. Morgan MM
    (2012) Periaqueductal gray. In: The human nervous system (Paxinos G, Mai JK, eds), Ed 2, pp 367–400. London: Academic Press.
  30. ↵
    1. Chen X-M,
    2. Nishi M,
    3. Taniguchi A,
    4. Nagashima K,
    5. Shibata M,
    6. Kanosue K
    (2002) The caudal periaqueductal gray participates in the activation of brown adipose tissue in rats. Neurosci Lett 331:17–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3940(02)00757-7
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Coker-Appiah DS,
    2. White SF,
    3. Clanton R,
    4. Yang J,
    5. Martin A,
    6. Blair RJR
    (2013) Looming animate and inanimate threats: the response of the amygdala and periaqueductal gray. Soc Neurosci 8:621–630. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2013.839480 pmid:24066700
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. Cox RW
    (1996) AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput Biomed Res 29:162–173. https://doi.org/10.1006/cbmr.1996.0014
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Dampney RAL,
    2. Furlong TM,
    3. Horiuchi J,
    4. Iigaya K
    (2013) Role of dorsolateral periaqueductal grey in the coordinated regulation of cardiovascular and respiratory function. Auton Neurosci 175:17–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.autneu.2012.12.008
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Depaulis A,
    2. Keay KA,
    3. Bandler R
    (1992) Longitudinal neuronal organization of defensive reactions in the midbrain periaqueductal gray region of the rat. Exp Brain Res 90:307–318. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00227243
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Esteban O, et al.
    (2019) fMRIPrep: a robust preprocessing pipeline for functional MRI. Nat Methods 16:111–116. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41592-018-0235-4 pmid:30532080
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Esteban O, et al.
    (2020) fMRIPrep: A robust preprocessing pipeline for functional MRI [Computer software]. Zenodo. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.4252786
  37. ↵
    1. Fanselow MS
    (1991) The midbrain periaqueductal gray as a coordinator of action in response to fear and anxiety. In: The midbrain periaqueductal gray matter (Depaulis A, Bandler R, eds), pp 151–173. New York: Springer US.
  38. ↵
    1. Fanselow MS
    (1994) Neural organization of the defensive behavior system responsible for fear. Psychon Bull Rev 1:429–438. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03210947
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Fardin V,
    2. Oliveras J-L,
    3. Besson J-M
    (1984) A reinvestigation of the analgesic effects induced by stimulation of the periaqueductal gray matter in the rat. I. The production of behavioral side effects together with analgesia. Brain Res 306:105–123. https://doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(84)90360-3
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Faull OK,
    2. Jenkinson M,
    3. Ezra M,
    4. Pattinson KT
    (2016) Conditioned respiratory threat in the subdivisions of the human periaqueductal gray. eLife 5:e12047. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.12047 pmid:26920223
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. Faull OK,
    2. Subramanian HH,
    3. Ezra M,
    4. Pattinson KTS
    (2019) The midbrain periaqueductal gray as an integrative and interoceptive neural structure for breathing. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 98:135–144. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.12.020
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. Fonov V,
    2. Evans A,
    3. McKinstry R,
    4. Almli C,
    5. Collins D
    (2009) Unbiased nonlinear average age-appropriate brain templates from birth to adulthood. NeuroImage 47:S102. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119(09)70884-5
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  43. ↵
    1. Gevins A,
    2. Cutillo B
    (1993) Spatiotemporal dynamics of component processes in human working memory. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 87:128–143. https://doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(93)90119-g
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    1. Gianaros PJ,
    2. Wager TD
    (2015) Brain–body pathways linking psychological stress and physical health. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 24:313–321. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721415581476 pmid:26279608
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    1. Gorgolewski K,
    2. Burns CD,
    3. Madison C,
    4. Clark D,
    5. Halchenko YO,
    6. Waskom ML,
    7. Ghosh SS
    (2011) Nipype: a flexible, lightweight and extensible neuroimaging data processing framework in Python. Front Neuroinform 5:1–15. https://doi.org/10.3389/fninf.2011.00013 pmid:21897815
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    1. Gray JR,
    2. Chabris CF,
    3. Braver TS
    (2003) Neural mechanisms of general fluid intelligence. Nat Neurosci 6:316–322. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1014
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    1. Gray MA,
    2. Rylander K,
    3. Harrison NA,
    4. Wallin BG,
    5. Critchley HD
    (2009) Following one’s heart: cardiac rhythms gate central initiation of sympathetic reflexes. J Neurosci 29:1817–1825. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3363-08.2009 pmid:19211888
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  48. ↵
    1. Green AL,
    2. Wang S,
    3. Owen SLF,
    4. Xie K,
    5. Liu X,
    6. Paterson DJ,
    7. Stein JF,
    8. Bain PG,
    9. Aziz TZ
    (2005) Deep brain stimulation can regulate arterial blood pressure in awake humans. NeuroReport 16:1741–1745. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.wnr.0000183904.15773.47
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    1. Greve DN,
    2. Fischl B
    (2009) Accurate and robust brain image alignment using boundary-based registration. NeuroImage 48:63–72. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.060 pmid:19573611
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    1. Gross CT,
    2. Canteras NS
    (2012) The many paths to fear. Nat Rev Neurosci 13:Article 9. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3301
    OpenUrl
  51. ↵
    1. Haakenson CM,
    2. Balthazart J,
    3. Ball GF
    (2020) Effects of inactivation of the periaqueductal gray on song production in testosterone-treated male canaries (Serinus canaria). eNeuro 7:ENEURO.0048-20.2020. https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0048-20.2020 pmid:32737183
    OpenUrlPubMed
  52. ↵
    1. Hermans EJ,
    2. Henckens MJAG,
    3. Roelofs K,
    4. Fernández G
    (2013) Fear bradycardia and activation of the human periaqueductal grey. NeuroImage 66:278–287. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.10.063
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    1. Hoemann K,
    2. Khan Z,
    3. Feldman MJ,
    4. Nielson C,
    5. Devlin M,
    6. Dy J,
    7. Barrett LF,
    8. Wormwood JB,
    9. Quigley KS
    (2020) Context-aware experience sampling reveals the scale of variation in affective experience. Sci Rep 10:Article 1. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-69180-y pmid:32719368
    OpenUrlPubMed
  54. ↵
    1. Hohenschurz-Schmidt DJ, et al.
    (2020) Linking pain sensation to the autonomic nervous system: the role of the anterior cingulate and periaqueductal gray resting-state networks. Front Neurosci 14:1–15.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    1. Holstege G,
    2. Kerstens L,
    3. Moes MC,
    4. VanderHorst VGJM
    (1997) Evidence for a periaqueductal gray–nucleus retroambiguus–spinal cord pathway in the rat. Neuroscience 80:587–598. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0306-4522(97)00061-4
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  56. ↵
    1. Jansen ASP,
    2. Farkas E,
    3. Mac Sams J,
    4. Loewy AD
    (1998) Local connections between the columns of the periaqueductal gray matter: a case for intrinsic neuromodulation. Brain Res 784:329–336. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-8993(97)01293-6
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  57. ↵
    1. Jenkinson M,
    2. Bannister P,
    3. Brady M,
    4. Smith S
    (2002) Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. NeuroImage 17:825–841. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1132
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  58. ↵
    1. Jenkinson M,
    2. Smith S
    (2001) A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images. Med Image Anal 5:143–156. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1361-8415(01)00036-6
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    1. Kasper L, et al.
    (2017) The PhysIO toolbox for modeling physiological noise in fMRI data. J Neurosci Methods 276:56–72. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2016.10.019
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  60. ↵
    1. Kempton MJ, et al.
    (2011) A comprehensive testing protocol for MRI neuroanatomical segmentation techniques: evaluation of a novel lateral ventricle segmentation method. NeuroImage 58:1051–1059. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.06.080 pmid:21835253
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  61. ↵
    1. Kirchner WK
    (1958) Age differences in short-term retention of rapidly changing information. J Exp Psychol 55:352–358. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0043688
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  62. ↵
    1. Kleiner M
    (2007) What’s new in Psychtoolbox-3? 89.
  63. ↵
    1. Knight YE,
    2. Goadsby PJ
    (2001) The periaqueductal grey matter modulates trigeminovascular input: a role in migraine? 11Presented in part at the Xth International Headache Congress, Barcelona, Spain, 24–26 June 1999 [Knight and Goadsby, Cephalalgia 19 (1999) 315]. Neuroscience 106:793–800. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0306-4522(01)00303-7
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  64. ↵
    1. Koutsikou S,
    2. Apps R,
    3. Lumb BM
    (2017) Top down control of spinal sensorimotor circuits essential for survival. J Physiol 595:4151–4158. https://doi.org/10.1113/JP273360 pmid:28294351
    OpenUrlPubMed
  65. ↵
    1. Kragel PA,
    2. Bianciardi M,
    3. Hartley L,
    4. Matthewson G,
    5. Choi J-K,
    6. Quigley KS,
    7. Wald LL,
    8. Wager TD,
    9. Feldman Barrett L,
    10. Satpute AB
    (2019) Functional involvement of human periaqueductal gray and other midbrain nuclei in cognitive control. J Neurosci 39:6180–6189. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2043-18.2019 pmid:31160537
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  66. ↵
    1. Kuznetsova A,
    2. Brockhoff PB,
    3. Christensen RHB
    (2017) Lmertest package: tests in linear mixed effects models. J Stat Softw 82:Article 1. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v082.i13
    OpenUrl
  67. ↵
    1. LeDoux J,
    2. Daw ND
    (2018) Surviving threats: neural circuit and computational implications of a new taxonomy of defensive behaviour. Nat Rev Neurosci 19:Article 5. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2018.22
    OpenUrl
  68. ↵
    1. Li Y-Q,
    2. Takada M,
    3. Shinonaga Y,
    4. Mizuno N
    (1993) Direct projections from the midbrain periaqueductal gray and the dorsal raphe nucleus to the trigeminal sensory complex in the rat. Neuroscience 54:431–443. https://doi.org/10.1016/0306-4522(93)90264-G
    OpenUrlPubMed
  69. ↵
    1. Lindner K,
    2. Neubert J,
    3. Pfannmöller J,
    4. Lotze M,
    5. Hamm AO,
    6. Wendt J
    (2015) Fear-potentiated startle processing in humans: parallel fMRI and orbicularis EMG assessment during cue conditioning and extinction. Int J Psychophysiol 98:535–545. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.02.025
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  70. ↵
    1. Linnman C,
    2. Moulton EA,
    3. Barmettler G,
    4. Becerra L,
    5. Borsook D
    (2012) Neuroimaging of the periaqueductal gray: state of the field. NeuroImage 60:505–522. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.095 pmid:22197740
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  71. ↵
    1. Lovick TA
    (1992) Inhibitory modulation of the cardiovascular defence response by the ventrolateral periaqueductal grey matter in rats. Exp Brain Res 89:133–139. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00229010
    OpenUrlPubMed
  72. ↵
    1. Lu B, et al.
    (2022) Neuronal electrophysiological activities detection of defense behaviors using an implantable microelectrode array in the dorsal periaqueductal gray. Biosensors 12:193. https://doi.org/10.3390/bios12040193 pmid:35448253
    OpenUrlPubMed
  73. ↵
    1. Lu J,
    2. Sherman D,
    3. Devor M,
    4. Saper CB
    (2006) A putative flip–flop switch for control of REM sleep. Nature 441:Article 7093. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature04767
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  74. ↵
    1. McNaughton N,
    2. Corr PJ
    (2018) Survival circuits and risk assessment. Curr Opin Behav Sci 24:14–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.01.018
    OpenUrl
  75. ↵
    1. Mobbs D,
    2. Hagan CC,
    3. Dalgleish T,
    4. Silston B,
    5. Prévost C
    (2015) The ecology of human fear: survival optimization and the nervous system. Front Neurosci 9:1–22.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  76. ↵
    1. Mobbs D,
    2. Headley DB,
    3. Ding W,
    4. Dayan P
    (2020) Space, time, and fear: survival computations along defensive circuits. Trends Cogn Sci 24:228–241. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2019.12.016
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  77. ↵
    1. Mobbs D,
    2. Petrovic P,
    3. Marchant JL,
    4. Hassabis D,
    5. Weiskopf N,
    6. Seymour B,
    7. Dolan RJ,
    8. Frith CD
    (2007) When fear is near: threat imminence elicits prefrontal-periaqueductal gray shifts in humans. Science 317:1079–1083. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1144298 pmid:17717184
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  78. ↵
    1. Morgan MM,
    2. Whitney PK,
    3. Gold MS
    (1998) Immobility and flight associated with antinociception produced by activation of the ventral and lateral/dorsal regions of the rat periaqueductal gray. Brain Res 804:159–166. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-8993(98)00669-6
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  79. ↵
    1. Mota-Ortiz SR,
    2. Sukikara MH,
    3. Bittencourt JC,
    4. Baldo MV,
    5. Elias CF,
    6. Felicio LF,
    7. Canteras NS
    (2012) The periaqueductal gray as a critical site to mediate reward seeking during predatory hunting. Behav Brain Res 226:32–40. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2011.08.034
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  80. ↵
    1. Motta SC,
    2. Carobrez AP,
    3. Canteras NS
    (2017) The periaqueductal gray and primal emotional processing critical to influence complex defensive responses, fear learning and reward seeking. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 76:39–47. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.10.012
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  81. ↵
    1. Mumford JA,
    2. Poline J-B,
    3. Poldrack RA
    (2015) Orthogonalization of regressors in fMRI models. PLoS ONE 10:e0126255. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0126255 pmid:25919488
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  82. ↵
    1. Muschelli J,
    2. Nebel MB,
    3. Caffo BS,
    4. Barber AD,
    5. Pekar JJ,
    6. Mostofsky SH
    (2014) Reduction of motion-related artifacts in resting state fMRI using aCompCor. NeuroImage 96:22–35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.028 pmid:24657780
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  83. ↵
    1. Nashold BS,
    2. Wilson WP,
    3. Slaughter DG
    (1969) Sensations evoked by stimulation in the midbrain of man. J Neurosurg 30:14–24. https://doi.org/10.3171/jns.1969.30.1.0014
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  84. ↵
    1. Panksepp J
    (2011) The basic emotional circuits of mammalian brains: do animals have affective lives? Neurosci Biobehav Rev 35:1791–1804. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.08.003
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  85. ↵
    1. Paredes J,
    2. Winters RW,
    3. Schneiderman N,
    4. McCabe PM
    (2000) Afferents to the central nucleus of the amygdala and functional subdivisions of the periaqueductal gray: neuroanatomical substrates for affective behavior. Brain Res 887:157–173. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-8993(00)02972-3
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  86. ↵
    1. Pereira EAC,
    2. Lu G,
    3. Wang S,
    4. Schweder PM,
    5. Hyam JA,
    6. Stein JF,
    7. Paterson DJ,
    8. Aziz TZ,
    9. Green AL
    (2010) Ventral periaqueductal grey stimulation alters heart rate variability in humans with chronic pain. Exp Neurol 223:574–581. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.expneurol.2010.02.004
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  87. ↵
    1. Power JD,
    2. Mitra A,
    3. Laumann TO,
    4. Snyder AZ,
    5. Schlaggar BL,
    6. Petersen SE
    (2014) Methods to detect, characterize, and remove motion artifact in resting state fMRI. NeuroImage 84:320–341. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.048 pmid:23994314
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  88. ↵
    R Core Team (2016) R: A language and environment for statistical computing [Computer software]. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org
  89. ↵
    1. Renvall V,
    2. Witzel T,
    3. Wald LL,
    4. Polimeni JR
    (2016) Automatic cortical surface reconstruction of high-resolution T1 echo planar imaging data. NeuroImage 134:338–354. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.004 pmid:27079529
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  90. ↵
    1. Sandkühler J,
    2. Herdegen T
    (1995) Distinct patterns of activated neurons throughout the rat midbrain periaqueductal gray induced by chemical stimulation within its subdivisions. J Comp Neurol 357:546–553. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.903570406
    OpenUrlPubMed
  91. ↵
    1. Satpute AB,
    2. Wager TD,
    3. Cohen-Adad J,
    4. Bianciardi M,
    5. Choi J-K,
    6. Buhle JT,
    7. Wald LL,
    8. Barrett LF
    (2013) Identification of discrete functional subregions of the human periaqueductal gray. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 110:17101–17106. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1306095110 pmid:24082116
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  92. ↵
    1. Satterthwaite TD, et al.
    (2013) An improved framework for confound regression and filtering for control of motion artifact in the preprocessing of resting-state functional connectivity data. NeuroImage 64:240–256. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.08.052 pmid:22926292
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  93. ↵
    1. Sennesh E,
    2. Theriault J,
    3. Brooks D,
    4. van de Meent J-W,
    5. Barrett LF,
    6. Quigley KS
    (2022) Interoception as modeling, allostasis as control. Biol Psychol 167:108242. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2021.108242 pmid:34942287
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  94. ↵
    1. Silva BA,
    2. Gross CT,
    3. Gräff J
    (2016) The neural circuits of innate fear: detection, integration, action, and memorization. Learn Mem 23:544–555. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.042812.116 pmid:27634145
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  95. ↵
    1. Silva C,
    2. McNaughton N
    (2019) Are periaqueductal gray and dorsal raphe the foundation of appetitive and aversive control? A comprehensive review. Prog Neurobiol 177:33–72. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2019.02.001
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  96. ↵
    1. Smith SM,
    2. Nichols TE
    (2009) Threshold-free cluster enhancement: addressing problems of smoothing, threshold dependence and localisation in cluster inference. NeuroImage 44:83–98. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.061
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  97. ↵
    1. Subramanian HH,
    2. Balnave RJ,
    3. Holstege G
    (2008) The midbrain periaqueductal gray control of respiration. J Neurosci 28:12274–12283. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4168-08.2008 pmid:19020021
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  98. ↵
    1. Thayer JF,
    2. Åhs F,
    3. Fredrikson M,
    4. Sollers JJ,
    5. Wager TD
    (2012) A meta-analysis of heart rate variability and neuroimaging studies: implications for heart rate variability as a marker of stress and health. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 36:747–756. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.11.009
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  99. ↵
    1. Theriault JE,
    2. Fischbach A,
    3. Kragel PA,
    4. Wager TD,
    5. Wald LL,
    6. Bianciardi M,
    7. Quigley KS,
    8. Barrett LF,
    9. Satpute AB
    (under review) Distinct spatial patterns of neural and physiology-related activity in the human periaqueductal grey during anticipatory social stress.
  100. ↵
    1. Tinoco Mendoza FA,
    2. Hughes TES,
    3. Robertson RV,
    4. Crawford LS,
    5. Meylakh N,
    6. Macey PM,
    7. Macefield VG,
    8. Keay KA,
    9. Henderson LA
    (2023) Detailed organisation of the human midbrain periaqueductal grey revealed using ultra-high field magnetic resonance imaging. NeuroImage 266:119828. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.119828
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  101. ↵
    1. Tryon VL,
    2. Mizumori SJY
    (2018) A novel role for the periaqueductal gray in consummatory behavior. Front Behav Neurosci 12:178–178. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00178 pmid:30210313
    OpenUrlPubMed
  102. ↵
    1. van Ast VA,
    2. Spicer J,
    3. Smith EE,
    4. Schmer-Galunder S,
    5. Liberzon I,
    6. Abelson JL,
    7. Wager TD
    (2016) Brain mechanisms of social threat effects on working memory. Cereb Cortex 26:544–556. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhu206 pmid:25249408
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  103. ↵
    1. Vázquez-León P,
    2. Miranda-Páez A,
    3. Valencia-Flores K,
    4. Sánchez-Castillo H
    (2023) Defensive and emotional behavior modulation by serotonin in the periaqueductal gray. Cell Mol Neurobiol 43:1453–1468. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10571-022-01262-z
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  104. ↵
    1. Virtanen P, et al.
    (2020) Scipy 1.0: fundamental algorithms for scientific computing in Python. Nat Methods 17:261–272. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41592-019-0686-2 pmid:32015543
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  105. ↵
    1. Wager TD,
    2. Smith EE
    (2003) Neuroimaging studies of working memory: a meta-analysis. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 3:255–274. https://doi.org/10.3758/cabn.3.4.255
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  106. ↵
    1. Wager TD,
    2. van Ast VA,
    3. Hughes BL,
    4. Davidson ML,
    5. Lindquist MA,
    6. Ochsner KN
    (2009a) Brain mediators of cardiovascular responses to social threat, Part II: prefrontal-subcortical pathways and relationship with anxiety. NeuroImage 47:836–851. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.044 pmid:19465135
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  107. ↵
    1. Wager TD,
    2. Waugh CE,
    3. Lindquist M,
    4. Noll DC,
    5. Fredrickson BL,
    6. Taylor SF
    (2009b) Brain mediators of cardiovascular responses to social threat: Part I: reciprocal dorsal and ventral sub-regions of the medial prefrontal cortex and heart-rate reactivity. NeuroImage 47:821–835. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.043 pmid:19465137
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  108. ↵
    1. Wang S,
    2. Veinot J,
    3. Goyal A,
    4. Khatibi A,
    5. Lazar SW,
    6. Hashmi JA
    (2022) Distinct networks of periaqueductal gray columns in pain and threat processing. NeuroImage 250:118936. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.118936
    OpenUrl
  109. ↵
    1. Weis CN,
    2. Bennett KP,
    3. Huggins AA,
    4. Parisi EA,
    5. Gorka SM,
    6. Larson C
    (2022) A 7-tesla MRI study of the periaqueductal gray: resting state and task activation under threat. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 17:187–197. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsab085 pmid:34244809
    OpenUrlPubMed
  110. ↵
    1. Weis C,
    2. Bennett K,
    3. Huggins A,
    4. Parisi E,
    5. Larson C
    (2020) Dynamic functional connectivity of the periaqueductal grey in response to predictable and unpredictable threat using 7-tesla MRI. Biol Psychiatry 87:S164–S165. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2020.02.436
    OpenUrl
  111. ↵
    1. Wickham H
    (2009) Ggplot2: elegant graphics for data analysis. New York: Springer.
  112. ↵
    1. Zhang SP,
    2. Bandler R,
    3. Carrive P
    (1990) Flight and immobility evoked by excitatory amino acid microinjection within distinct parts of the subtentorial midbrain periaqueductal gray of the cat. Brain Res520:73–82. https://doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(90)91692-a
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  113. ↵
    1. Zhang Y,
    2. Brady M,
    3. Smith S
    (2001) Segmentation of brain MR images through a hidden Markov random field model and the expectation-maximization algorithm. IEEE Trans Med Imaging 20:45–57. https://doi.org/10.1109/42.906424
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  114. ↵
    1. Zhang W,
    2. Hayward LF,
    3. Davenport PW
    (2005) Respiratory muscle responses elicited by dorsal periaqueductal gray stimulation in rats. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 289:R1338–R1347. https://doi.org/10.1152/ajpregu.00828.2004
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  115. ↵
    1. Zhang YH,
    2. Hosono T,
    3. Yanase-Fujiwara M,
    4. Chen XM,
    5. Kanosue K
    (1997) Effect of midbrain stimulations on thermoregulatory vasomotor responses in rats. J Physiol 503:177–186. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7793.1997.177bi.x pmid:9288685
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  116. ↵
    1. Zhou F,
    2. Zhao W,
    3. Qi Z,
    4. Geng Y,
    5. Yao S,
    6. Kendrick KM,
    7. Wager TD,
    8. Becker B
    (2021) A distributed fMRI-based signature for the subjective experience of fear. Nat Commun 12:Article 1. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-021-26977-3 pmid:34789745
    OpenUrlPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 44 (26)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 44, Issue 26
26 Jun 2024
  • Table of Contents
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Masthead (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Seven Tesla Evidence for Columnar and Rostral–Caudal Organization of the Human Periaqueductal Gray Response in the Absence of Threat: A Working Memory Study
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Seven Tesla Evidence for Columnar and Rostral–Caudal Organization of the Human Periaqueductal Gray Response in the Absence of Threat: A Working Memory Study
Alexandra K. Fischbach, Ajay B. Satpute, Karen Quigley, Philip A. Kragel, Danlei Chen, Marta Bianciardi, Larry Wald, Tor D. Wager, Ji-Kyung Choi, Jiahe Zhang, Lisa Feldman Barrett, Jordan E. Theriault
Journal of Neuroscience 26 June 2024, 44 (26) e1757232024; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1757-23.2024

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Seven Tesla Evidence for Columnar and Rostral–Caudal Organization of the Human Periaqueductal Gray Response in the Absence of Threat: A Working Memory Study
Alexandra K. Fischbach, Ajay B. Satpute, Karen Quigley, Philip A. Kragel, Danlei Chen, Marta Bianciardi, Larry Wald, Tor D. Wager, Ji-Kyung Choi, Jiahe Zhang, Lisa Feldman Barrett, Jordan E. Theriault
Journal of Neuroscience 26 June 2024, 44 (26) e1757232024; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1757-23.2024
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Significance Statement
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • brainstem
  • high resolution
  • neuroimaging
  • periaqueductal gray
  • working memory

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Articles

  • Regional Excitatory–Inhibitory Balance Relates to Self-Reference Effect on Recollection via the Precuneus/Posterior Cingulate Cortex–Medial Prefrontal Cortex Connectivity
  • Attention Alters Population Spatial Frequency Tuning
  • Sequence Diversity and Encoded Enzymatic Differences of Monocistronic L1 ORF2 mRNA Variants in the Aged Normal and Alzheimer's Disease Brain
Show more Research Articles

Behavioral/Cognitive

  • Attention Alters Population Spatial Frequency Tuning
  • Complex Impact of Stimulus Envelope on Motor Synchronization to Sound
  • The Molecular Substrates of Second-Order Conditioned Fear in the Basolateral Amygdala Complex
Show more Behavioral/Cognitive
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Accessibility
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.