Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Research Articles, Behavioral/Cognitive

Effects of Context Changes on Memory Reactivation

Şahcan Özdemir, Yağmur Damla Şentürk, Nursima Ünver, Can Demircan, Christian N. L. Olivers, Tobias Egner and Eren Günseli
Journal of Neuroscience 4 September 2024, 44 (36) e2096232024; https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2096-23.2024
Şahcan Özdemir
1Department of Psychology, Sabancı University, Istanbul 34956, Turkey
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Şahcan Özdemir
Yağmur Damla Şentürk
1Department of Psychology, Sabancı University, Istanbul 34956, Turkey
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Nursima Ünver
1Department of Psychology, Sabancı University, Istanbul 34956, Turkey
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Can Demircan
1Department of Psychology, Sabancı University, Istanbul 34956, Turkey
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Christian N. L. Olivers
2Department of Experimental and Applied Psychology, Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam 1081 BT, the Netherlands
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Christian N. L. Olivers
Tobias Egner
3Department of Psychology & Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, North Carolina 27708
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Tobias Egner
Eren Günseli
1Department of Psychology, Sabancı University, Istanbul 34956, Turkey
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Eren Günseli
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

While the influence of context on long-term memory (LTM) is well documented, its effects on the interaction between working memory (WM) and LTM remain less understood. In this study, we explored these interactions using a delayed match-to-sample task, where participants (6 males, 16 females) encountered the same target object across six consecutive trials, facilitating the transition from WM to LTM. During half of these target repetitions, the background color changed. We measured the WM storage of the target using the contralateral delay activity in electroencephalography. Our results reveal that task-irrelevant context changes trigger the reactivation of long-term memories in WM. This reactivation may be attributed to content–context binding in WM and hippocampal pattern separation.

  • context
  • learning
  • long-term memory
  • memory reactivation
  • working memory

Significance Statement

Understanding the mechanisms of memory updating in response to changing contexts is vital because context plays a pivotal role in shaping long-term memories. This study demonstrates, for the first time, that an irrelevant context change triggers the reactivation of learned memories in visual working memory. This observation underscores the importance of multimemory interactions during context updating. Challenging traditional memory models that postulate mandatory reactivation of long-term memories upon each use, our results instead reveal a selective reactivation process, especially during transitions to new environments. This finding elucidates the adaptive nature of memories and enhances our understanding of memory storage and retrieval processes.

Introduction

Working memory (WM) is critical in storing novel task rules and their associated information (Baddeley, 2003, 2010; Carlisle et al., 2011; Cowan, 2017). When used repeatedly, these representations transfer to long-term memory (LTM; Anderson, 1983; Logan, 1988; Carlisle et al., 2011; Gunseli et al., 2014a,b, 2016). LTM is strongly intertwined with the context in which memories are formed and utilized. For example, retrieval is more successful when it shares the same context with encoding (Tulving and Thomson, 1973; Godden and Baddeley, 1975; Robin et al., 2016). Changes in context also determine the temporal structure of episodic memories by generating event boundaries at context transitions (Clewett and Davachi, 2017; Wang and Egner, 2022; Güler et al., 2023, 2024; Nolden et al., 2024). These findings suggest that context strongly influences how an item is preserved in LTM.

Although the effects of context change on LTM encoding and retrieval are well established, its influence on the interaction between WM and LTM remains unclear. For instance, when people are required to switch between different task rules, the associated rule-updating process results in a reactivation of task-relevant information in WM (Şentürk et al., 2024). By contrast, it is not currently known whether a change in context would result in a similar WM updating process as a change in the task rule. There are several reasons why context changes may have this effect on WM. First, reactivating information in WM upon a context change may be critical for individuating similar memories from different contexts, possibly by associating each memory with a unique context or via hippocampal pattern separation. Hippocampal pattern separation enhances the individuation of memories via dissociating activity patterns of similar memories (O’Reilly and McClelland, 1994; Yassa and Stark, 2011; Amer and Davachi, 2023). This process can occur through cortical activity (Wimber et al., 2015), which is a feature of WM storage (Fuster and Alexander, 1971; Funahashi et al., 1993; Vogel and Machizawa, 2004; Thrower et al., 2023). Second, reactivating memories upon context switches could be one of the contributing factors to the segmented nature of episodic memories (Sols et al., 2017; Güler et al., 2023). Given this intimate relationship between context, task-relevant items, and task rules, we hypothesized that context switches, akin to task switches, should trigger the reactivation of long-term memories in WM to adjust to the novel context.

To investigate how context change affects the interplay between WM and LTM, we employed a measure of memory reactivation and assessed how it responded to contextual changes. Participants performed a recognition task in which the target object was repeated across trials. During this repetition, a task-irrelevant context (the background color) occasionally changed. We used the contralateral delay activity (CDA), which has become a widely used electroencephalography (EEG) index of active WM storage (Vogel et al., 2005; Luria et al., 2016). This first of all allowed us to assess learning and transfer to LTM, as this has been shown to be accompanied by a decline in CDA with increasing repetition of the same item across trials (Carlisle et al., 2011; Gunseli et al., 2014a,b; Reinhart and Woodman, 2014; Reinhart et al., 2014, 2016; Grubert et al., 2016; Şentürk et al., 2024). Second, and most important, the CDA allowed us to test our main hypothesis: considering that LTM is reactivated in WM to adapt to changes in the environment (Artuso and Palladino, 2011 Borders et al., 2022; Şentürk et al., 2024) and that adaptation processes like hippocampal pattern separation may occur via cortical reactivation associated with WM (Wimber et al., 2015), we hypothesized that context shifts would trigger the reactivation of information handed off to LTM back in WM. If so, this should become evident as a rebound in the CDA for the already-learned item. In contrast, if WM is not context-sensitive, then information handed off to LTM should remain in LTM without being reactivated in WM despite contextual shifts.

Materials and Methods

Participants

This study was preregistered on OSF (https://osf.io/wmk5q?view_only=None). The preregistration includes the study's experimental design, sampling plan, hypotheses, and analysis plan. Twenty-nine students from Sabancı University participated for course credits in this study, and seven participants were excluded from further analysis due to exclusion criteria (explained below). Data collection was conducted following our lab's guidelines to minimize COVID-19 transmission risk (Hasşerbetçi and Günseli, 2021). The analyses were conducted with 22 participants (Meanage = 21.7; SD = 2.9), 6 males and 16 females. We determined the target participant number based on our previous work that used a similar experimental design (Şentürk et al., 2024). Effect sizes from four studies that used CDA as a memory load measure were averaged (Gunseli et al., 2014b; Berggren and Eimer, 2016; Reinhart et al., 2016; Xie and Zhang, 2018). Following the guidelines by Schönbrodt and Wagenmakers (2018) and Dienes (2021) and the associated R package by Schönbrodt and Stefan (2019; github.com/nicebread/BFDA), a sequential design approach was conducted. Cauchy distribution as an uninformed and objective distribution was chosen as prior with half of the estimated effect size as the scaling parameter (Dienes, 2021). We calculated the minimum number of participants as 20 by estimating the number of participants needed for a power of 0.90 and a false positive rate of 0.02 under the estimated effect size d = 1.12. The number of participants as a stopping rule was estimated as 70 with the smallest effect size in the abovementioned studies, which is d = 0.48. Bayes factor (BF) was planned to be calculated first when the number of participants reaches 20 and then aimed to be calculated after every five new participants.

Data collection was decided to continue until the BF is between 1/6 and 6 or the number of participants reaches 70. We collected 22 participants in total after exclusion. Because we were late in checking the statistical analyses of 20 participants, we exceeded the specified number of 20 without calculating the BF. We realized the mistake with 22 participants. We first calculated the BF for the first 20 participants, and the result was above 6, and then we calculated the BF again with all 22 participants, and the results were still similar (above 6), so we stopped the data collection at 22 participants.

Since color served as context in this study, before the experiment, each participant took an online Ishihara color blindness test, which is suggested to be as efficient as traditional paper tests (Marey et al., 2015). Participants viewed plates, which consist of solid circles of various colors and sizes that form a number or a pattern. They indicated the number written on the plate or counted the number of lines on the pattern. If they respond correctly to <13 plates out of 38, they are excluded from the experiment due to red–green color vision deficiency (Marey et al., 2015).

After the EEG data artifact rejection process (see below), participants who had <60 nonrejected trials per condition or with accuracies <80% were excluded from further analysis. New participants were recruited to replace excluded participants. This study was approved by the Sabancı University Ethical Committee. All participants signed a consent form to participate in the study.

Ethics statement

This study was performed in line with the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki. The ethics approval was granted by the Sabancı University Research Ethics Committee.

Stimuli

Images that were used in the current study (Şentürk et al., 2024) were brought together with additionally collected images to create a set of 2,880 images of real-world objects (Konkle et al., 2010; Konkle and Oliva, 2012; Konkle and Caramazza, 2013; Google Images) in total. Images were resized to contain approximately the same number of nontransparent pixels. Half of the images were selected as target objects, and the other half were selected as nontarget. Target objects were separated into two groups, animate and inanimate. Animate and inanimate groups contained 60 object categories, each category consisting of 12 pictures, thus making 1,440 target objects in total. Each object (7.1° × 7.1°) was presented once as a target throughout the experiment. Each category became a target twice. Two objects from each object category were used as targets during the experiment.

Each trial was presented with a color as a context. Context color could be either red (HSL, 228, 44, 146) or green (H, 71; S, 67; L, 145). The context color was presented as a rectangular frame surrounding the center of the screen (16.7° × 10°). The experiment had 1,440 trials. Each target object was repeated for six trials in a row. Through these repetition series, the context could change in the first and/or fifth repetition. Participants viewed the experiment 85 cm away from the computer screen. The background color of the experiment was gray. The location cue was a vertically halved, bicolored circle (0.35° × 0.35°), with one side being navy blue (H, 240; S, 100; L, 25) and the other side orange (H, 38,5; S, 100; L, 50). For a given participant, a particular color indicated the side of the screen on which the target was presented, and it was counterbalanced across participants. The experiment is designed and conducted with PsychoPy (Peirce et al., 2019).

Design and procedure

Trial design

The trial design is depicted in Figure 1. The task was to indicate if the probe item matches the target item. Each trial started with a presentation of the context color for 1,250 ms. Then, the location cue appeared for a jittered duration of 800–1,400 ms and remained on the screen until the probe was at the center of the screen. Following the location cue, two objects were presented. One of the objects was presented at the left and one to the right of the location cue, for 500 ms. Participants were instructed to memorize the target presented on the cued side. They were instructed to fixate on the location cue. Next, there was a retention interval of 900 ms during which the cue remained on the screen. After the retention interval, the probe was shown at the center of the screen. On each side of the probe, there were two response labels, “Same” and “Different” (0.7°). Participants pressed the left or right arrow key on a Turkish QWERTY computer keyboard to indicate their response (Fig. 1; e.g., the left arrow to respond Same and the right arrow to respond Different). The locations of each response label were counterbalanced across participants. The probe remained on the screen until response or up to 1,600 ms. Participants received visual feedback upon their response (“correct” or “incorrect”) or after 1,600 ms (“miss”). The feedback was shown at the center of the screen for 300 ms (5° × 5°). Lastly, a blank intertrial interval was jittered between 300 and 700 ms. The context color frame was presented in the background (16.7° × 10°) throughout the trial except the intertrial interval.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Illustration of the experimental procedure. Each trial will start with the presentation of context, i.e., the background color, which will be filled in the real experiment but shown here only as a frame for illustration purposes. Then, participants will see the location cue that indicates which object will be the target object. Following the location cue, the target object was shown at the indicated side, and the nontarget object was at the contralateral side. After a brief retention interval, participants will be shown a probe object and will indicate if it is the same as the target.

Trial distribution and block design

On each trial, the probe and the target were from the same object category. For each condition, the target was equally likely to be on the left or right on the target display. The target type was equally likely to be animate or inanimate. The experiment began with a practice session of 25 trials minimum. The objects used during practice weren't shown in the main experiment. The practice session was repeated until participants achieved at least 80% accuracy. The experimental session was divided into 38–42 blocks of ∼40 trials. The block and trial numbers varied to ensure each block starts with a new context and a new target. At the end of each block, participants were informed about their accuracy and were able to take a self-paced break.

Behavioral analysis

Our main hypotheses concern the EEG data and are not directly related to the behavioral performance pattern. However, as in previous studies using this protocol, we expected to observe a decrease in reaction time with target repetition (Carlisle et al., 2011; Gunseli et al., 2014a). To test this, we performed a Bayesian paired-sample t test to compare reaction times of first and fifth target repetitions in old-context trials.

EEG recording and registered analysis

The EEG was recorded from 32 sintered AG/AgCl electrodes. The electrodes were positioned at international 10/20 system sites and mounted in an elastic cap using Brain Products actiCHamp (actiCHamp Plus, Brain Products). The vertical EOG electrodes were located at 2 cm above and below the right eye, and the horizontal EOG electrodes were located at 1 cm lateral to the external canthi. We performed the analysis of the EEG data using MATLAB (2022), the EEGLAB toolbox (Delorme and Makeig, 2004), and the custom code. Trials containing ocular artifacts that are recorded by EOG and EEG noise such as blocking, muscle noise, saturation, etc. were detected manually via visual inspection. Trials with such artifacts were excluded from further analysis. We applied a filter to EEG data by IIR Butterworth filter with a bandpass of 0.1–40 Hz using the pop_eegfiltnew.m function of EEGLAB. The online reference electrode was located in the right mastoid, and then the data were rereferenced off-line to the average of left and right mastoids. A baseline period of 200 ms prior to the stimulus onset was included in the ERP analysis.

We used the CDA amplitude as a validated tool to assess WM storage based on four lines of findings. First, previous research showed that CDA scales with the number of items kept in WM and reaches an asymptote at individuals’ WM capacity (Vogel and Machizawa, 2004; Vogel et al., 2005; Luria et al., 2016). Second, the CDA has been shown to reflect the number of items in WM and not their complexity (Ikkai et al., 2010). Third, the CDA decreases when the same item is repeatedly stored in WM, reflecting the handoff of the item's representation from WM to LTM (Carlisle et al., 2011; Gunseli et al., 2014a,b; Reinhart and Woodman, 2014; Reinhart et al., 2014, 2016; Grubert et al., 2016; Şentürk et al., 2024). Thus, it is a suitable tool to measure WM involvement in storing items. Fourth and last, the CDA was found to increase following task rule changes (Şentürk et al., 2024) and cues that signal high reward (Reinhart and Woodman, 2014), deeming it possible to observe a similar increase following context change.

The CDA was computed at P7/8, PO3/4, PO7/8, and O1/2 electrodes (Vogel and Machizawa, 2004; Vogel et al., 2005; Ikkai et al., 2010; Günseli et al., 2019) by subtracting the activity of ipsilateral channels from the contralateral ones relative to the target position between 400 and 1,400 ms after the target onset (retention interval). To test if new targets are stored in WM (Carlisle et al., 2011; Gunseli et al., 2014a,b; Reinhart et al., 2016), the CDA in new target trials were compared with zero using two one-sample t tests, one for new contexts and the other for old contexts.

Next, to test if repeated targets are being handed off from WM to LTM (Carlisle et al., 2011; Gunseli et al., 2014a,b; Reinhart and Woodman, 2014; Reinhart et al., 2014, 2016; Grubert et al., 2016; Şentürk et al., 2024), we compared the first and the fifth target repetitions using a Bayesian paired-sample t test. For this analysis, we used old-context trials only. To assess if this difference showed a linear or a quadratic CDA trend through a target repetition series (Fig. 2), a trend analysis with repeated-measure ANOVA was conducted. Last and importantly, to assess whether a context change results in the targets stored in LTM being reactivated in WM, we used a Bayesian paired-sample t test to compare the CDA in new versus old-context trials only for the fifth target repetition where a handoff to LTM is expected based on previous literature (Carlisle et al., 2011; Gunseli et al., 2014a,b; Reinhart et al., 2016).

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Behavioral results. A, The accuracy increased with target repetition. B, The reaction time remained unchanged across target repetitions. Neither were affected by context changes.

Exploratory analysis

We calculated the N2pc, which is an index of attentional selection (Eimer, 1996; Hickey et al., 2009). Similar to CDA, N2pc was calculated as a difference between contralateral and ipsilateral activity regarding the target position, time-locked to the onset of the memory item. This difference was calculated between 250 and 350 ms based on visual exploration of our data and our previous study with a similar design (Şentürk et al., 2024). Time–frequency analysis was conducted to explore the involvement of attentional processes in memory reactivation (Fukuda and Woodman, 2017; Günseli et al., 2019). Bilateral and contralateral alpha-band suppressions were analyzed in the time course between 500 and 1,200 ms. The same channels with the CDA analysis (P7/8, PO7/8, and O1/2) were used in the calculation. We analyzed the power of frequencies between 4 and 50 Hz to see if the effects we observed were alpha band specific or propagated to other frequencies. To calculate bilateral alpha-band suppression, we defined our frequencies between 8 and 12 Hz (Woodman et al., 2012; Günseli et al., 2019) on a logarithmic scale. For each frequency, a sinusoid (ei2ft) was created; then these sinusoids were converted to Morlet wavelets by being tapered with a Gaussian [n (e-t2/2s2; s is the width of the Gaussian; s = /(2f) denotes for number of cycles created for wavelet)]. We padded zero to the beginning and the end of our data as half of the length of our Morlet wavelets. Our epoched data were rearranged as one continuous EEG data. Fast Fourier transform (FFT) was applied to both the EEG and Morlet waves. The dot product of the Fourier-transformed EEG data and Fourier-transformed Morlet wavelet was calculated for each frequency. Then inverse FFT was applied to each dot product. With this procedure, the EEG data became convoluted for each Morlet wavelet. Then, we performed baseline normalization and decibel (dB) conversion. Each baseline was calculated by averaging the power activity between 500 and 200 ms before the memory item onset of all trials. The power activity in each trial was divided by this baseline and converted to dB. Then, for the analysis regarding the alpha-band suppression, we averaged the dB values between 8–12 and 400–1,200 ms, over trials for each condition. We have chosen a slightly shorter time window compared with the CDA calculation to prevent probe-related activity from contaminating the power due to the temporal smearing caused by convolution.

A similar procedure was applied to calculate the contralateral alpha suppression. However, for contralateral alpha suppression, the baseline was calculated between 500 and 200 ms before the location cue rather than the memory onset. Since the location cue was given before the memory representation, lateralization can be expected in the baseline time window that reflects the expected attention (Ikkai et al., 2016). This might result in the cancellation of lateralization in the retention interval due to baseline removal with a lateralized baseline. This difference in baseline time range was applied to get a clear baseline without lateralization. This baseline normalization was applied to each condition separately, unlike the bilateral time–frequency decomposition. Then the power values from the selected channels (P7/8, PO7/8, and O1/2) that are contralateral to the target item position were subtracted from the ipsilateral channels (Günseli et al., 2019).

Results

Behavioral results

Reaction times

There was anecdotal evidence for no difference in reaction times across Target Repetition 1 and Target Repetition 5 (old-context TR1 vs old-context TR5, BF10 = 0.669; t(21) = 1.597; p = 0.125). Moreover, there was also anecdotal evidence for equal reaction times at the new and old-context trials’ first repetitions (BF10 = 0.225; t(21) = 0.132; p = 0.896). Also, there was anecdotal evidence for an increase in RT with a change in context at the fifth repetition (BF10 = 1.536; t(21) = 2.164; p = 0.042). When we conducted a trend analysis with repeated-measure ANOVA, repetition showed no linear contrast (t(105) = −1.357; p = 0.178). These results suggest that RT was mostly unaffected by target repetitions and contextual shifts.

Accuracy

We found strong evidence for an increase in accuracy with repetition (old-context TR1 vs old-context TR5, BF10 = 91.247; t(21) = −4.265; p < 0.001; linear contrast with repetition, t(105) = 5.923; p < 0.001). Also, there was anecdotal evidence for equal accuracy at the fifth repetition between change in context (M = 0.96; SD = 0.02) and old context (M = 0.96; SD = 0.02; BF10 = 0.243; t(21) = 0.432; p = 0.67). Thus, while accuracy improved across target repetitions, it was mostly unaffected by context shifts.

EEG results

CDA

We compared the CDA amplitudes of the first repetition trials against 0 to test whether there is a CDA when a novel object is presented. As depicted in Figure 3A, the first repetition trials’ CDA values were more negative than 0 (old context, BF10 = 34,470; t = −7.134; p < 0.001; new context, BF10 = 43,375; t(21) = −7.252; p < 0.001). Then we compared the first and fifth repetitions of old-context trials to test the transition of the item from WM to LTM. Results showed strong evidence that the CDA amplitude of the fifth repetition was different from the first (BF10 = 2,406; t(21) = −5.815; p < 0.001), which suggests the representation of the target item was transferred to LTM with repetition of the same task and item (linear trend, t(105) = 6.859; p < 0.001).

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

CDA. A, CDA changes through repetitions. CDA amplitude decreased with target repetition. CDA was recovered at the fifth repetition when there was a contextual change. B, Contralateral–ipsilateral waveforms relative to the target side for repeated and switched context trials at the fifth target repetition. The shaded areas refer to the time windows for N2pc and CDA. The recovery with a context change was present for the CDA but not the N2pc. The error bars represent the standard error of the mean for the context repeat versus switch condition difference.

Then, we compared the fifth repetition trial regarding whether there was a change in context or not. When there was a change in the background color, we observed strong evidence for a recovery in the CDA (BF10 = 8.858; t(21) = −3.131; p = 0.005; Fig. 3B). We conclude that a change in context triggers the reactivation of the task-relevant item in WM. Lastly, we tested if a similar increase is present for novel targets. The CDA for novel targets did not differ for old-context and new-context trials (BF10 = 0.326). This finding suggests that a CDA increase via a contextual shift at TR5 is unlikely to represent a nonreactivation-related cognitive boost, as a similar shift should have been present for contextual shifts at TR1.

N2pc

For a new target, there was strong evidence for an N2pc (BF10 = 767.442; t(21) = −5.269; p < 0.001) suggesting that the memory item was attended at the memory display. N2pc didn't show any changes regarding the contextual changes. However, with the first repetition, we observed a strong increase in N2pc (BF10 = 55.982; t(21) = 4.033; p < 0.001; Fig. 3A). After this increase, it didn't show any changes due to context changes (BF10 = 0.240; t(21) = 0.401; p = 0.692).

Alpha-band suppression

The alpha-band suppression provided a pattern similar to the CDA (Fig. 4C). For a new target, there was strong evidence for a bilateral alpha-band suppression (BF10 = 5,899.886; t(21) = −6.250; p < 0.001). Moreover, there was a decrease with the repetition of the same target (old-context TR1 vs old-context TR5, BF10 = 396.46; t(21) = −4.957; p < 0.001), which was further supported by the linear trend observed with repetition (ANOVA linear contrast, t(105) = 8.023; p < 0.001). Lastly, the comparison between old-context and new-context trials at the fifth repetition showed that the alpha-band suppression recovered (BF10 = 1,830; t(21) = −5.683; p < 0.001). These results suggested that overall attentional resources declined with repetition and recovered with a contextual change (Reinhart and Woodman, 2014; Şentürk et al., 2024). This increase in alpha suppression could reflect memory reactivation of the task-relevant item or the novel context (Fukuda and Woodman, 2017) given that the global alpha suppression explored here is not specific to the hemisphere that represents the memory item.

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Exploratory analysis. A, Changes in N2pc through repetitions. N2pc showed a strong increase with the first repetition but responded neither to repetition nor context changes. B, Contralateral alpha suppression through repetitions. Contralateral alpha suppression was stable across repetitions and context changes. C, Bilateral alpha suppression through repetitions. Bilateral alpha suppression decreased with target repetition and increased with a context change at the fifth target repetition. D, Broadband (4–50 Hz) power at TR5, separately for new- versus old-context conditions.

Contralateral alpha-band suppression

We analyzed the spatial lateralization in the alpha-band suppression regarding the object position (Fig. 4E). For a new target, there was strong evidence for a contralateral alpha-band suppression (BF10 = 29.585; t(21) = −3.727; p = 0.001) suggesting that the memory item was attended during its storage within WM. Alpha-band suppression showed moderate evidence for no change regarding the repetition of the target or contextual changes (BF10 = 0.223; t(21) = 0.048; p = 0.96). This result suggests that participants continued to attend to the memory item's location across repetitions. This contrast between CDA and lateral alpha is in line with our previous findings and highlights a dissociation between selective attention and storage within WM (van Driel et al., 2017; Günseli et al., 2019; Hakim et al., 2019; Şentürk et al., 2024).

EEG behavioral relations

We examined whether the reactivation in WM predicts any behavioral performance of the participants. To achieve this, we subtracted the CDA amplitudes at the fifth repetition old-context trials from the fifth repetition new-context trials. Similarly, we subtracted the reaction times and accuracies at the fifth repetition of old-context trials from the fifth repetition of new-context trials. Then we checked whether the changes in CDA due to contextual changes predict any behavioral performance by analyzing correlations. CDA differences with contextual changes predicted neither a difference in accuracy nor in reaction times (CDA-RT, Pearson's r = −0.267; p = 0.23; CDA-accuracy, Pearson's r = −0.087; p = 0.701).

Discussion

We examined whether task-irrelevant context changes cause memory reactivation. Our EEG index of WM, the CDA, diminished for repeated targets, replicating previous work on the transfer from WM to LTM (Carlisle et al., 2011; Reinhart and Woodman, 2014; Gunseli et al., 2014a,b; Grubert et al., 2016; Alfandari et al., 2019; Şentürk et al., 2024). Importantly, with a change in context, the CDA reemerged, indicating that task-irrelevant context changes elicited the reactivation of task-relevant information in WM.

WM minimizes processing costs whenever possible (Stokes, 2015; Draschkow et al., 2021; Mızrak and Oberauer, 2021; Chota et al., 2023; Yucel et al., 2023; de Jong et al., 2024). In our experiment, memory reactivation was not associated with a behavioral benefit but only with a metabolic cost, reflected in increased CDA. This metabolic cost without behavioral benefits suggests that memory reactivation is not strategic but involuntary.

Why are memories reactivated following context changes?

Reinhart and colleagues found that memories are reactivated when participants were instructed to improve performance (Reinhart and Woodman, 2014) or signaled high reward (Reinhart et al., 2016), suggesting high stakes facilitate memory reactivation. Mızrak and Oberauer (2021) suggested reactivation is reserved for memories that partially overlap with former memories, aligning with WM's role in resolving proactive interference (Engle, 2002). We recently demonstrated that LTMs are reactivated in WM when there is a change in task rules. In the present study, we show that changes in task-irrelevant information also evoke memory reactivation. Together with Şentürk et al. (2024), our findings suggest memory reactivation helps adapt to novel settings, irrespective of stakes or proactive interference.

We propose two adaptive functions of memory reactivation. The first is pattern separation. The hippocampus separates similar events to prevent interference (O’Reilly and McClelland, 1994; Yassa and Stark, 2011; Amer and Davachi, 2023). Encountering the same item in a different context might signal it as distinct, encouraging pattern separation, which could benefit from cortical reinstatement associated with WM (Wimber et al., 2015). The second mechanism might be contextual binding. Previous studies found that spatial, color, or temporal context affects behavioral performance, reflecting their storage even when irrelevant (Oberauer and Vockenberg, 2009; Artuso and Palladino, 2011; Cai et al., 2018, 2022). If WM establishes item-context bindings by default, context change might trigger the reactivation of an item in WM to establish its binding with the.

The independence of attention and storage in WM

In our study, contralateral alpha-band suppression did not differ across repetitions or contextual changes. Typically, contralateral alpha suppression is associated with the allocation of spatial attention (Woodman et al., 2022). Given the decline in the CDA index of WM storage, one would have expected a simultaneous decline in contralateral alpha suppression, supporting the view of WM as a manifestation of endogenous attention (Gazzaley and Nobre, 2012; Kiyonaga and Egner, 2013). However, the dissociation between CDA and contralateral alpha suppression implies a differentiation between attention and WM storage (Günseli et al., 2019) and demonstrates that memory reactivation does not require spatial attention.

Eliminating alternative explanations

In the present study, we interpreted the decline in CDA as indicative of a transition to LTM (Carlisle et al., 2011; Gunseli et al., 2014a,b; Reinhart and Woodman, 2014; Reinhart et al., 2014, 2016; Grubert et al., 2016; Şentürk et al., 2024). Accordingly, we took the resurgence of the CDA as a sign of reactivating an LTM back in WM (Reinhart et al., 2014, 2016; Şentürk et al., 2024). Below, we will discuss and eliminate five alternative explanations.

The first possibility is that the decreasing CDA via target repetition reflects a decline in attention to the repeated item rather than a transition to LTM. However, this is contradicted by the absence of a decline in contralateral alpha suppression and N2pc, indices of selective attention in perception and visual WM (Eimer, 1996; Hickey et al., 2009; Foster and Awh, 2019; Woodman et al., 2022). This dissociation is consistent with our previous work showing that CDA is sensitive to storage in WM and mostly unaffected by attention, which is tracked via contralateral alpha power suppression (van Driel et al., 2017; Günseli et al., 2019; Hakim et al., 2019). Thus, diminishing attention to the memory item likely does not account for the CDA pattern. Even if the CDA reflected decreasing attention, our interpretation of a decline in active maintenance holds, as a decrease in attention coupled with an increase in accuracy would not be possible without LTM.

The second alternative is that the increase in CDA with a new context reflects increased WM activation due to the enhanced processing of the memory display rather than retrieval from LTM. However, the lack of a corresponding increase in N2pc and contralateral alpha suppression, indices of selective attention (Eimer, 1996; Hickey et al., 2009; Foster and Awh, 2019; Woodman et al., 2022), argues against stronger encoding, as the latter requires stronger attention (Treisman and Geffen, 1967; Posner, 1980; Mangun, 1995; Reynolds and Chelazzi, 2004; Boynton, 2005; Carrasco, 2018). Importantly, whether the source is stronger encoding or retrieval from LTM, WM representations were reactivated with a context change despite the item being available in LTM. Thus, our interpretation of a context-driven activation of WM holds, regardless of whether this activation comes from reencoding or retrieving the item from LTM.

The third alternative is that the CDA increase with a context change reflects increased arousal or attention to the task. However, this is unlikely for three reasons. First, task rules are abstract representations, and arousal is a global state, neither of which would be lateralized. Therefore, they cannot explain a larger CDA, which is lateralized relative to the memory item. In line with this, previous studies found the CDA to be immune to confounds from global effects like arousal or attention (Ikkai et al., 2010; Feldmann-Wüstefeld et al., 2018). Second, previous studies found that the CDA is unaffected by the difficulty of the task the item is stored for (Ikkai et al., 2010; Gunseli et al., 2014a,b; Luria et al., 2016; Şentürk et al., 2024), making it reliable for assessing representation-specific activity. Third, if the CDA increase was due to increased attention to the task, this should have been evident for context changes at Item Repetition 1, which was not the case (Şentürk et al., 2024). Therefore, a global increase in attention or arousal are unlikely candidates for explaining the CDA increase with context change.

The fourth concern regarding the CDA decline as transfer to LTM might be the lack of an effect of target repetition on reaction times. However, this was neither unexpected nor crucial. First, the decline in reaction time over target repetitions is task-dependent, being strongest in effortful search tasks and absent in recognition tasks (Carlisle et al., 2011; Gunseli et al., 2014a,b). In the present study, we replicated the latter. Second, accuracy improved with repeated targets, implying learning (Olson et al., 2005; Adam and Vogel, 2018). Third, our primary outcome measure, CDA, has been consistently shown to index WM load (Vogel and Machizawa, 2004; McCollough et al., 2007; Ikkai et al., 2010; Kang and Woodman, 2014; Luria et al., 2016; Adam et al., 2018; Feldmann-Wüstefeld et al., 2018; Günseli et al., 2019; Hakim et al., 2019; Roy and Faubert, 2022), thus serving as a more direct indicator of the memory representation's status than reaction time, and substantial evidence now correlates the decline in CDA with learning (Carlisle et al., 2011; Gunseli et al., 2014a,b; Reinhart et al., 2014; Reinhart and Woodman, 2014; Grubert et al., 2016; Şentürk et al., 2024). Memory performance not worsening despite weakening neural WM indices supports LTM taking over given that forgetting would predict declining performance.

Besides the clear decrease in CDA with repetition, we observed a recovery in CDA at the sixth compared with that at the fifth repetition. This raises the question of why participants allocate more WM resources to maintaining the same information after transferring it to LTM. However, prior research has demonstrated CDA recovery at the end of the learning curve, indicating preparedness for a new item (Carlisle et al., 2011; Reinhart and Woodman, 2014; Gunseli et al., 2014a,b; Şentürk et al., 2024). In our study, the fixed number of six repetitions provides participants with an expectation of a new item. Thus, the CDA increase on Repetition 6 likely reflects preparatory WM activation. Importantly, this observation does not invalidate our core finding: the CDA on Repetition 5 differs between old and new-context trials, showing that individuals rely on LTM when the same context repeats and on WM when the context switches.

Conclusion

In summary, our findings demonstrate that contextual shifts prompt memory reactivation. Items transferred to LTM through repeated storage are reactivated in WM upon encountering a task-irrelevant context change. Given the neural costs of this reactivation and the lack of observable behavioral benefits, this suggests that context-dependent reactivation is involuntary. Accordingly, we propose that memory reactivation in response to environmental changes occurs automatically, even when the context change is not directly related to the task at hand.

Data Availability

All data and materials are shared publicly on Open Science (https://osf.io/xhrb9/?view_only=6ce0380db484406ea2070d8dbbf6ba93). Data are shared anonymously without any risk of lack of privacy. All codes of the study are shared publicly on Open Science (https://osf.io/xhrb9/?view_only=6ce0380db484406ea2070d8dbbf6ba93).

Footnotes

  • This work was funded by the Scientific and Technological Council of Türkiye (TÜBİTAK) Grant (122K700) awarded to E.G. We thank Azra Duru Erdem, Hande Altunbaş, and Öykü Özdemir for their contributions to data collection.

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Şahcan Özdemir at sahcan.ozdemir{at}sabanciuniv.edu or Eren Günseli at eren.gunseli{at}sabanciuniv.edu.

SfN exclusive license.

References

  1. ↵
    1. Adam KC,
    2. Robison MK,
    3. Vogel EK
    (2018) Contralateral delay activity tracks fluctuations in working memory performance. J Cogn Neurosci 30:1229–1240. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01233 pmid:29308988
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Adam KC,
    2. Vogel EK
    (2018) Improvements to visual working memory performance with practice and feedback. PLoS One 13:e0203279. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0203279 pmid:30161210
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Alfandari D,
    2. Belopolsky AV,
    3. Olivers CN
    (2019) Eye movements reveal learning and information-seeking in attentional template acquisition. Vis Cogn 27:467–486. https://doi.org/10.1080/13506285.2019.1636918
    OpenUrl
  4. ↵
    1. Amer T,
    2. Davachi L
    (2023) Extra-hippocampal contributions to pattern separation. Elife 12:e82250. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.82250 pmid:36972123
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Anderson JR
    (1983) A spreading activation theory of memory. J Verbal Learn Verbal Behav 22:261–295. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(83)90201-90203
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  6. ↵
    1. Artuso C,
    2. Palladino P
    (2011) Content–context binding in verbal working memory updating: on-line and off-line effects. Acta Psychol 136:363–369. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.01.001
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Baddeley A
    (2003) Working memory: looking back and looking forward. Nat Rev Neurosci 4:829–839. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1201
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Baddeley A
    (2010) Working memory. Curr Biol 20:R136–R140. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2009.12.014
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Berggren N,
    2. Eimer M
    (2016) Does contralateral delay activity reflect working memory storage or the current focus of spatial attention within visual working memory? J Cogn Neurosci 28:2003–2020. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01019
    OpenUrl
  10. ↵
    1. Borders AA,
    2. Ranganath C,
    3. Yonelinas AP
    (2022) The hippocampus supports high precision binding in visual working memory. Hippocampus 32:217–230. https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.23401
    OpenUrl
  11. ↵
    1. Boynton GM
    (2005) Attention and visual perception. Curr Opin Neurobiol 15:465–469. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2005.06.009
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Cai Y,
    2. Fulvio JM,
    3. Samaha J,
    4. Postle BR
    (2022) Context binding in visual working memory is reflected in bilateral event-related potentials, but not in contralateral delay activity. eNeuro 9:ENEURO.0207-22.2022. https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0159-24.2024 pmid:38719456
    OpenUrlPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Cai Y,
    2. Yu Q,
    3. Sheldon AD,
    4. Postle BR
    (2018) The role of location-context binding in nonspatial visual working memory. Technical report, bioRxiv.
  14. ↵
    1. Carlisle NB,
    2. Arita JT,
    3. Pardo D,
    4. Woodman GF
    (2011) Attentional templates in visual working memory. J Neurosci 31:9315–9322. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1097-11.2011 pmid:21697381
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  15. ↵
    1. Carrasco M
    (2018) How visual spatial attention alters perception. Cogn Process 19:77–88. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-018-0883-4 pmid:30062667
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Chota S,
    2. Gayet S,
    3. Kenemans JL,
    4. Olivers CNL,
    5. Van der Stigchel S
    (2023) A matter of availability: sharper tuning for memorized than for perceived stimulus features. Cereb Cortex 33:7608–7618. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhad064 pmid:37005059
    OpenUrlPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Clewett D,
    2. Davachi L
    (2017) The ebb and flow of experience determines the temporal structure of memory. Curr Opin Behav Sci 17:186–193. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.08.013 pmid:29276730
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Cowan N
    (2017) The many faces of working memory and short-term storage. Psychon Bull Rev 24:1158–1170. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-016-1191-6
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. de Jong J,
    2. Wilhelm S,
    3. Akyürek EG
    (2024) Adaptive forgetting speed in working memory. Psychon Bull Rev. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-024-02507-2
  20. ↵
    1. Delorme A,
    2. Makeig S
    (2004) EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J Neurosci Methods 134:9–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Dienes Z
    (2021) How to use and report Bayesian hypothesis tests. Psychol Conscious 8:9–26. https://doi.org/10.1037/cns0000258
    OpenUrl
  22. ↵
    1. Draschkow D,
    2. Kallmayer M,
    3. Nobre AC
    (2021) When natural behavior engages working memory. Curr Biol 31:869–874.e5. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2020.11.013 pmid:33278355
    OpenUrlPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Eimer M
    (1996) The N2pc component as an indicator of attentional selectivity. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 99:225–234. https://doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(96)95711-9
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Engle RW
    (2002) Working memory capacity as executive attention. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 11:19–23. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00160
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  25. ↵
    1. Feldmann-Wüstefeld T,
    2. Vogel EK,
    3. Awh E
    (2018) Contralateral delay activity indexes working memory storage, not the current focus of spatial attention. J Cogn Neurosci 30:1185–1196. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01271 pmid:29694260
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Foster JJ,
    2. Awh E
    (2019) The role of alpha oscillations in spatial attention: limited evidence for a suppression account. Curr Opin Psychol 29:34–40. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.11.001 pmid:30472541
    OpenUrlPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Fukuda K,
    2. Woodman GF
    (2017) Visual working memory buffers information retrieved from visual long-term memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 114:5306–5311. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1617874114 pmid:28461479
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  28. ↵
    1. Funahashi S,
    2. Chafee MV,
    3. Goldman-Rakic PS
    (1993) Prefrontal neuronal activity in rhesus monkeys performing a delayed anti-saccade task. Nature 365:753–756. https://doi.org/10.1038/365753a0
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    1. Fuster JM,
    2. Alexander GE
    (1971) Neuron activity related to short-term memory. Science 173:652–654. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.173.3997.652
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  30. ↵
    1. Gazzaley A,
    2. Nobre AC
    (2012) Top-down modulation: bridging selective attention and working memory. Trends Cogn Sci 16:129–135. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.014 pmid:22209601
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Godden DR,
    2. Baddeley AD
    (1975) Context-dependent memory in two natural environments: on land and underwater. Br J Psychol 66:325331. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1975.tb01468.x
    OpenUrl
  32. ↵
    1. Grubert A,
    2. Carlisle NB,
    3. Eimer M
    (2016) The control of single-color and multiple-color visual search by attentional templates in working memory and in long-term memory. J Cogn Neurosci 28:1947–1963. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01020
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  33. ↵
    1. Güler B,
    2. Adıgüzel Z,
    3. Uysal B,
    4. Günseli E
    (2024) Discrete memories of a continuous world: a working memory perspective on event segmentation. Curr Opin Behav Sci 6:100145. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.crbeha.2023.100145
    OpenUrl
  34. ↵
    1. Güler B,
    2. Serin F,
    3. Gunseli E
    (2023) Prediction error is out of context: the dominance of contextual stability in segmenting episodic events.
  35. ↵
    1. Günseli E,
    2. Fahrenfort JJ,
    3. van Moorselaar D,
    4. Daoultzis KC,
    5. Meeter M,
    6. Olivers CNL
    (2019) EEG dynamics reveal a dissociation between storage and selective attention within working memory. Sci Rep 9:13499. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-49577-0 pmid:31534150
    OpenUrlPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Gunseli E,
    2. Meeter M,
    3. Olivers CNL
    (2014a) Is a search template an ordinary working memory? Comparing electrophysiological markers of working memory maintenance for visual search and recognition. Neuropsychologia 60:29–38. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.05.012
    OpenUrl
  37. ↵
    1. Gunseli E,
    2. Olivers CNL,
    3. Meeter M
    (2014b) Effects of search difficulty on the selection, maintenance, and learning of attentional templates. J Cogn Neurosci 26:2042–2054. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00600
    OpenUrl
  38. ↵
    1. Gunseli E,
    2. Olivers CN,
    3. Meeter M
    (2016) Task-irrelevant memories rapidly gain attentional control with learning. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 42:354–362. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000134
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  39. ↵
    1. Hakim N,
    2. Adam KC,
    3. Gunseli E,
    4. Awh E,
    5. Vogel EK
    (2019) Dissecting the neural focus of attention reveals distinct processes for spatial attention and object-based storage in visual working memory. Psychol Sci 30:526–540. doi:10.1177/0956797619830384
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Hasşerbetçi S,
    2. Günseli E
    (2021) EEG çalışmalarında viral Enfeksiyon Bulaşma Riskini Azaltma Yönergeleri. Turk J Public Health 19:182–195. https://doi.org/10.20518/tjph.840203
    OpenUrl
  41. ↵
    1. Hickey C,
    2. Di Lollo V,
    3. McDonald JJ
    (2009) Electrophysiological indices of target and distractor processing in visual search. J Cogn Neurosci 21:760–775. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21039
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. Ikkai A,
    2. Dandekar S,
    3. Curtis CE
    (2016) Lateralization in alpha-band oscillations predicts the locus and spatial distribution of attention. PLoS One 11:e0154796. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0154796 pmid:27144717
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Ikkai A,
    2. McCollough AW,
    3. Vogel EK
    (2010) Contralateral delay activity provides a neural measure of the number of representations in visual working memory. J Neurophysiol 103:1963–1968. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00978.2009 pmid:20147415
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    1. Kang M-S,
    2. Woodman GF
    (2014) The neurophysiological index of visual working memory maintenance is not due to load dependent eye movements. Neuropsychologia 56:63–72. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.12.028 pmid:24440409
    OpenUrlPubMed
  45. ↵
    1. Kiyonaga A,
    2. Egner T
    (2013) Working memory as internal attention: toward an integrative account of internal and external selection processes. Psychon Bull Rev 20:228–242. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-012-0359-y pmid:23233157
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    1. Konkle T,
    2. Brady TF,
    3. Alvarez GA,
    4. Oliva A
    (2010) Conceptual distinctiveness supports detailed visual long-term memory for real-world objects. J Exp Psychol Gen 139:558–578. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019165 pmid:20677899
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    1. Konkle T,
    2. Caramazza A
    (2013) Tripartite organization of the ventral stream by animacy and object size. J Neurosci 33:10235–10242. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0983-13.2013 pmid:23785139
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  48. ↵
    1. Konkle T,
    2. Oliva A
    (2012) A real-world size organization of object responses in occipitotemporal cortex. Neuron 74:1114–1124. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2012.04.036 pmid:22726840
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    1. Logan GD
    (1988) Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychol Rev 95:492–527. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.95.4.492
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  50. ↵
    1. Luria R,
    2. Balaban H,
    3. Awh E,
    4. Vogel EK
    (2016) The contralateral delay activity as a neural measure of visual working memory. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 62:100–108. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.01.003 pmid:26802451
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    1. Mangun GR
    (1995) Neural mechanisms of visual selective attention. Psychophysiology 32:4–18. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1995.tb03400.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    1. Marey HM,
    2. Semary NA,
    3. Mandour SS
    (2015) Ishihara electronic color blindness test: an evaluation study. Ophthalmic Res 3:67–75. https://doi.org/10.9734/OR/2015/13618
    OpenUrl
  53. ↵
    1. McCollough AW,
    2. Machizawa MG,
    3. Vogel EK
    (2007) Electrophysiological measures of maintaining representations in visual working memory. Cortex 43:77–94. doi:10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70447-7
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    1. Mızrak E,
    2. Oberauer K
    (2021) Working memory recruits long-term memory when it is beneficial: evidence from the Hebb effect. J Exp Psychol Gen 151:763–780. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000934
    OpenUrl
  55. ↵
    1. Nolden S,
    2. Turan G,
    3. Güler B,
    4. Günseli E
    (2024) Prediction error and event segmentation in episodic memory. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 157:105533. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2024.105533
    OpenUrl
  56. ↵
    1. Oberauer K,
    2. Vockenberg K
    (2009) Updating of working memory: lingering bindings. Q J Exp Psychol 62:967–987. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210802372912
    OpenUrl
  57. ↵
    1. Olson IR,
    2. Jiang Y,
    3. Moore KS
    (2005) Associative learning improves visual working memory performance. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 31:889–900. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.31.5.889
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  58. ↵
    1. O’Reilly RC,
    2. McClelland JL
    (1994) Hippocampal conjunctive encoding, storage, and recall: avoiding a trade-off. Hippocampus 4:661–682. https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.450040605
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    1. Peirce J,
    2. Gray JR,
    3. Simpson S,
    4. MacAskill M,
    5. Höchenberger R,
    6. Sogo H,
    7. Kastman E,
    8. Lindeløv JK
    (2019) Psychopy2: experiments in behavior made easy. Behav Res Methods 51:195–203. doi:10.3758/s13428-018-01193-y
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  60. ↵
    1. Posner MI
    (1980) Orienting of attention. Q J Exp Psychol 32:3–25. https://doi.org/10.1080/00335558008248231
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  61. ↵
    1. Reinhart RM,
    2. Carlisle NB,
    3. Woodman GF
    (2014) Visual working memory gives up attentional control early in learning: ruling out interhemispheric cancellation. Psychophysiology 51:800–804. doi:10.1111/psyp.12217
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  62. ↵
    1. Reinhart RMG,
    2. McClenahan LJ,
    3. Woodman GF
    (2016) Attention’s accelerator. Psychol Sci 27:790–798. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797616636416 pmid:27056975
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  63. ↵
    1. Reinhart RMG,
    2. Woodman GF
    (2014) High stakes trigger the use of multiple memories to enhance the control of attention. Cereb Cortex 24:2022–2035. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bht057 pmid:23448876
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  64. ↵
    1. Reynolds JH,
    2. Chelazzi L
    (2004) Attentional modulation of visual processing. Annu Rev Neurosci 27:611–647. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.26.041002.131039
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  65. ↵
    1. Robin J,
    2. Wynn J,
    3. Moscovitch M
    (2016) The spatial scaffold: the effects of spatial context on memory for events. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 42:308–315. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000167
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  66. ↵
    1. Roy Y,
    2. Faubert J
    (2022) Is the contralateral delay activity (CDA) a robust neural correlate for visual working memory (VWM) tasks? A reproducibility study. Psychophysiology 60: e14180. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.14180 pmid:36124370
    OpenUrlPubMed
  67. ↵
    1. Schönbrodt FD,
    2. Wagenmakers E-J
    (2018) Bayes factor design analysis: planning for compelling evidence. Psychon Bull Rev 25:128–142. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-017-1230-y
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  68. ↵
    1. Schönbrodt FD,
    2. Stefan AM
    (2019) BFDA: an R package for Bayes factor design analysis (version 0.5.0). Retrieved from https://github.com/nicebread/BFDA
  69. ↵
    1. Şentürk YD,
    2. Ünver N,
    3. Demircan C,
    4. Egner T,
    5. Günseli E
    (2024) The reactivation of task rules triggers the reactivation of task-relevant items. Cortex 171:465–480. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2023.10.024
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  70. ↵
    1. Sols I,
    2. DuBrow S,
    3. Davachi L,
    4. Fuentemilla L
    (2017) Event boundaries trigger rapid memory reinstatement of the prior events to promote their representation in long-term memory. Curr Biol 27:3499–3504.e4. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2017.09.057 pmid:29129536
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  71. ↵
    1. Stokes MG
    (2015) Activity-silent’ working memory in prefrontal cortex: a dynamic coding framework. Trends Cogn Sci 19:394–405. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.05.004 pmid:26051384
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  72. ↵
    1. Thrower L,
    2. Dang W,
    3. Jaffe RG,
    4. Sun JD,
    5. Constantinidis C
    (2023) Decoding working memory information from neurons with and without persistent activity in the primate prefrontal cortex. J Neurophysiol 130:1392–1402. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00290.2023 pmid:37910532
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  73. ↵
    1. Treisman A,
    2. Geffen G
    (1967) Selective attention: perception or response? Q J Exp Psychol 19:1–17. https://doi.org/10.1080/14640746708400062
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  74. ↵
    1. Tulving E,
    2. Thomson DM
    (1973) Encoding specificity and retrieval processes in episodic memory. Psychol Rev 80:352–373. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0020071
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  75. ↵
    1. van Driel J,
    2. Gunseli E,
    3. Meeter M,
    4. Olivers CNL
    (2017) Local and interregional alpha EEG dynamics dissociate between memory for search and memory for recognition. NeuroImage 149:114–128. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.01.031
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  76. ↵
    1. Vogel EK,
    2. Machizawa MG
    (2004) Neural activity predicts individual differences in visual working memory capacity. Nature 428:748–751. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature02447
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  77. ↵
    1. Vogel EK,
    2. McCollough AW,
    3. Machizawa MG
    (2005) Neural measures reveal individual differences in controlling access to working memory. Nature 438:500–503. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature04171
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  78. ↵
    1. Wang YC,
    2. Egner T
    (2022) Switching task sets creates event boundaries in memory. Cognition 221:104992. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104992
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  79. ↵
    1. Wimber M,
    2. Alink A,
    3. Charest I,
    4. Kriegeskorte N,
    5. Anderson MC
    (2015) Retrieval induces adaptive forgetting of competing memories via cortical pattern suppression. Nat Neurosci 18:582–589. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3973 pmid:25774450
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  80. ↵
    1. Woodman GF,
    2. Vogel EK,
    3. Luck SJ
    (2012) Flexibility in visual working memory: accurate change detection in the face of irrelevant variations in position. Vis Cogn 20:1–28. https://doi.org/10.1080/13506285.2011.630694 pmid:22287933
    OpenUrlPubMed
  81. ↵
    1. Woodman GF,
    2. Wang S,
    3. Sutterer DW,
    4. Reinhart RMG,
    5. Fukuda K
    (2022) Alpha suppression indexes a spotlight of visual-spatial attention that can shine on both perceptual and memory representations. Psychon Bull Rev 29:681–698. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-021-02034-4 pmid:34877635
    OpenUrlPubMed
  82. ↵
    1. Xie W,
    2. Zhang W
    (2018) Familiarity speeds up visual short-term memory consolidation: electrophysiological evidence from contralateral delay activities. J Cogn Neurosci 30:1–13. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01188
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  83. ↵
    1. Yassa MA,
    2. Stark CEL
    (2011) Pattern separation in the hippocampus. Trends Neurosci 34:515–525. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2011.06.006 pmid:21788086
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  84. ↵
    1. Yucel D,
    2. Ataseven N,
    3. Todorova L,
    4. Güler B,
    5. Fukuda K,
    6. Gunseli E
    (2023) Increased reliance on long-term memory when anticipating attentional guidance.
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 44 (36)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 44, Issue 36
4 Sep 2024
  • Table of Contents
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Masthead (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Effects of Context Changes on Memory Reactivation
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Effects of Context Changes on Memory Reactivation
Şahcan Özdemir, Yağmur Damla Şentürk, Nursima Ünver, Can Demircan, Christian N. L. Olivers, Tobias Egner, Eren Günseli
Journal of Neuroscience 4 September 2024, 44 (36) e2096232024; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2096-23.2024

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Effects of Context Changes on Memory Reactivation
Şahcan Özdemir, Yağmur Damla Şentürk, Nursima Ünver, Can Demircan, Christian N. L. Olivers, Tobias Egner, Eren Günseli
Journal of Neuroscience 4 September 2024, 44 (36) e2096232024; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2096-23.2024
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Significance Statement
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Conclusion
    • Data Availability
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • context
  • learning
  • long-term memory
  • memory reactivation
  • working memory

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Articles

  • CaMKIIβ-mediated phosphorylation enhances protein stability of spastin to promote neurite outgrowth
  • Vocal error monitoring in the primate auditory cortex
  • EEG Correlates of Active Removal from Working Memory
Show more Research Articles

Behavioral/Cognitive

  • Neural Distinction between Visual Word and Object Recognition: An fMRI Study Using Pictographs
  • Attentional Precursors of Errors Predict Error-Related Brain Activity
  • Directed Neural Network Dynamics in Sensorimotor Integration: Divergent Roles of Frontal Theta Band Activity Depending on Age
Show more Behavioral/Cognitive
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Accessibility
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.