Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Research Articles, Development/Plasticity/Repair

Making Connections: Neurodevelopmental Changes in Brain Connectivity After Adverse Experiences in Early Adolescence

Ayla Pollmann, Remo Sasso, Kathryn Bates and Delia Fuhrmann
Journal of Neuroscience 21 February 2024, 44 (8) e0991232023; https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0991-23.2023
Ayla Pollmann
1Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King’s College London, London SE5 8AF, United Kingdom
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Ayla Pollmann
Remo Sasso
2School of Electronic Engineering and Computer Science, Queen Mary University of London, London E1 4NS, United Kingdom
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Kathryn Bates
1Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King’s College London, London SE5 8AF, United Kingdom
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Delia Fuhrmann
1Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King’s College London, London SE5 8AF, United Kingdom
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Delia Fuhrmann
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Adverse childhood experiences have been linked to detrimental mental health outcomes in adulthood. This study investigates a potential neurodevelopmental pathway between adversity and mental health outcomes: brain connectivity. We used data from the prospective, longitudinal Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study (N ≍ 12.000, participants aged 9–13 years, male and female) and assessed structural brain connectivity using fractional anisotropy (FA) of white matter tracts. The adverse experiences modeled included family conflict and traumatic experiences. K-means clustering and latent basis growth models were used to determine subgroups based on total levels and trajectories of brain connectivity. Multinomial regression was used to determine associations between cluster membership and adverse experiences. The results showed that higher family conflict was associated with higher FA levels across brain tracts (e.g., t(3) = −3.81, β = −0.09, pbonf = 0.003) and within the corpus callosum (CC), fornix, and anterior thalamic radiations (ATR). A decreasing FA trajectory across two brain imaging timepoints was linked to lower socioeconomic status and neighborhood safety. Socioeconomic status was related to FA across brain tracts (e.g., t(3) = 3.44, β = 0.10, pbonf = 0.01), the CC and the ATR. Neighborhood safety was associated with FA in the Fornix and ATR (e.g., t(1) = 3.48, β = 0.09, pbonf = 0.01). There is a complex and multifaceted relationship between adverse experiences and brain development, where adverse experiences during early adolescence are related to brain connectivity. These findings underscore the importance of studying adverse experiences beyond early childhood to understand lifespan developmental outcomes.

  • adverse childhood experiences
  • adverse adolescent experiences
  • brain connectivity
  • family conflict
  • fractional anisotropy
  • K-means clustering
  • latent basis growth models
  • neighborhood safety
  • socioeconomic status
  • traumatic experiences

Significance Statement

There is a compelling link between youth adversity and various detrimental outcomes, including reduced mental health, socioeconomic status, and even life expectancy. One potential pathway for the lifelong consequences of adversity could be neurodevelopment in adolescence, but few studies have tested this directly. This study investigates a potential neurodevelopmental pathway between adversity and mental health outcomes: brain connectivity. We explored the relationship between adverse experiences during early adolescence (ages 9–13 years) and individual differences in neurodevelopmental trajectories. Our results provide novel evidence demonstrating that adverse experiences during adolescence are related to changes in brain connectivity. They highlight the need to consider environmental influences on development during adolescence, a unique period of protracted biological, social, and cognitive changes.

Introduction

Almost two-thirds of young people worldwide experience at least one form of adverse childhood experiences (ACEs) before the age of eighteen (Cicchetti, 2016; Carlson et al., 2020). Adversities include experiences such as abuse, neglect, and household dysfunction (Felitti et al., 1998; Dube et al., 2003). Previous research has shown that there is a compelling link between youth adversity and a variety of detrimental outcomes, including reduced mental health, socioeconomic status, and even life expectancy (Felitti et al., 1998; BPS, 2019; Hughes et al., 2020; Liao et al., 2021). However, the pathways by which adversity influences development are still to be understood (Huang et al., 2012).

Neurological changes are thought to play an important role in the developmental pathways after adversity (Short and Baram, 2019). Brain development is a multifaceted process driven by genes and shaped by the environment (Lim et al., 2020). Psychopathology literature on early adversity shows that childhood adversity and genetic vulnerability are associated with structural and functional brain development, potentially contributing to mental health issues later in development (Tian et al., 2020). For example, adverse events may accelerate the onset of bipolar disorder by disrupting white matter integrity and structural connectivity (Benedetti et al., 2014). Thus, neurological changes are an essential pathway by which adversity is thought to drive development (Short and Baram, 2019). However, the specific ways adverse experiences connect to detrimental health outcomes still need to be investigated.

Brain regions are connected through white matter tracts; pathways transmitting neuronal signals and influence a variety of phenotypes (Johansen-berg et al., 2010). Adverse experiences are thought to shape the development of brain connectivity (Chahal et al., 2022). Previous research suggests that changes in the connection of brain regions rather than changes within individual regions drive developmental outcomes (Fornito and Harrison, 2012). Investigating changes in white matter tracts connecting different brain regions is particularly important. Studies have shown that childhood maltreatment is associated with altered microstructural white matter development, which may increase the vulnerability to mental health issues (De Bellis et al., 2002; Paul et al., 2008; Huang et al., 2012; Lu et al., 2013). Specifically, adversity appears to be associated with reduced fractional anisotropy (FA) in large white matter bundles (Huang et al., 2012; Tendolkar et al., 2018; Lim et al., 2019, 2020). A meta-analysis by Lim et al. (2020) examined structural connectivity in maltreated children and found reduced FA, among other tracts, in the corpus callosum (CC), the fornix, and the anterior thalamic radiation. However, structural connectivity changes after adversity in youth remain little studied.

With magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) techniques, it is possible to investigate anatomical brain regions and these connecting pathways (Sporns, 2013). Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) is an MRI technique that allows for the exploration of the microstructural properties of white matter tracts. In DTI, tissue water diffusion is used to illustrate the microstructural architecture of membranes in the brain (Alexander et al., 2007; Soares et al., 2013). FA data is the most commonly used DTI measure (Beaulieu, 2002). It is commonly assumed that white matter volumes increase during childhood and adolescence (Paus, 2010; Brain Development Cooperative Group, 2012). Lower levels of FA are typically thought to indicate lower integrity of white matter microstructure due to changes in myelin insulation or axonal injury (Fields, 2008; Thomason and Thompson, 2011; Sheikh et al., 2014; McCarthy-Jones et al., 2018), and lower FA levels might be linked to adverse experiences (Dufford and Kim, 2017; Assari and Boyce, 2021).

While evidence shows clear links between adversity in childhood and neurodevelopmental changes, it is not yet clear how adversity shapes brain development during adolescence (Huang et al., 2012). As children transition into adolescence, they go through social changes (e.g., increasing reliance on peers) and biological changes (e.g., puberty) that may make them more susceptible to external influences (Andersen and Teicher, 2008; Kolb, 2009). For this reason, adolescence is thought to be a sensitive period for development (Fuhrmann et al., 2015). Adverse adolescent experiences (AAEs) differ from ACEs in at least three key ways. (1) They are broader than ACEs (including factors such as romantic relationships or school environment; Pollmann et al., 2022). (2) Environmental influences in adolescence tend to show more pronounced individual differences than in childhood (Fuhrmann et al., 2015), potentially because adolescents have greater autonomy than children in choosing and exploring their environment. (3) Adolescents are a heterogeneous group, and we, therefore, also expect levels of individual variability in adverse experiences. Studies on individual differences in neurodevelopment during adolescence are currently relatively scarce (Foulkes and Blakemore, 2018). However, there is some evidence for qualitatively different subgroups showing different trajectories of white matter microstructure development (Bathelt et al., 2019). A key aim of this study is to investigate how such potential individual differences relate to adverse experiences. We do this by testing for potential subgroups with different neurodevelopmental trajectories across adolescence.

This study will contribute to our understanding of the neurodevelopmental pathways that may follow adverse experiences during adolescence. We used latent basis growth models (LBGM) and K-means clustering to examine structural brain development in a data-driven way and examine how developmental trajectories are linked to adverse experiences. We used the prospective, longitudinal Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study (N ≍ 12.000) to examine white matter connectivity in adolescents exposed to adversity at two timepoints. Our rationale for selecting these adversity measures is as follows. Firstly, because of the design of the study, we were restricted to adversity measures available in the ABCD sample at baseline. Secondly, we wanted to focus on adversities that theory suggests specifically to be relevant for adolescence (such as the school environment; Eccles and Roeser, 2011), instead ofACEs (such as parental mental health issues; Felitti et al., 1998), which have been widely investigated (Boullier and Blair, 2018).

We hypothesize that (1) white matter connectivity (measured in FA) increases from 9 to 13 years. We additionally expect (2) that there will be individual differences in trajectories of white matter connectivity (measured in FA trajectory). These individual differences in adolescent structural brain connectivity development are reflected in subpopulations of developmental trajectories. Alongside differences in global brain connectivity patterns, we expect differences in the CC, fornix, and anterior thalamic radiations (ATR) based on the meta-analysis by Lim et al. (2020). To balance granularity with generalizability in our design, we included the global measure of connectivity, to capture potential overall effects, as well as the localized measures, to investigate the connectivity of specific regions. In the last analysis step, we explored the relationship between brain connectivity subpopulations and adverse AAEs.

Method

Cohort

This study used data from the prospective, longitudinal ABCD study (N ≍ 12.000, participants aged 9–13 years; Karcher and Barch, 2021). We used data collected at baseline (referred to as imaging timepoint 1) and 2-year follow-up (referred to as imaging timepoint 2). The sample included 48% female participants and 52% male. The largest ethic group came from a white ethnic background (48%); for detailed demographic information, see extended data: https://osf.io/fv5ey. For more information on the cohort, see the ABCD website.

Adversity measures

Adverse life experiences at age 9–11 years were modeled using five scales, all assessed at baseline. Where necessary, scales were reverse coded where relevant such that higher scores reflect higher adversity:

School satisfaction

School satisfaction was measured using the youth-reported 12-item PhenX “ABCD School Risk and Protective Factors Survey” (SRPF). Three subscales were assessed: school environment, school involvement, and school disengagement. Response items ranged on a Likert scale from 1 (“No!”) to 4 (“Yes!”). Previous work has shown that the reliability of the school environment scale (six items, α = 0.60) and the school involvement scale (four items, α = 0.64) is moderately high. However, the alienation/disengagement from school scale is low (α = 0.21) as only two items of this scale were included by ABCD (Anon, 2016; Zucker et al., 2018; Gonzalez et al., 2021).

Family conflict

Family conflict was assessed using the self-reported binary nine-item family conflict subscale from the PhenX Family Environment Scale (FES) based on Moos and Moos (1994), which has moderately high reliability (α = 0.68). Items included anger, criticism, and physical abuse (Gonzalez et al., 2021).

Neighborhood safety

ABCD included one self-reported PhenX Neighbourhood Safety/Crime Survey item: “My neighbourhood is safe from crime.” Response options ranged from 1 (“Strongly disagree”) to 5 (“Strongly agree”) (Echeverria et al., 2004; Mujahid et al., 2007, Anon, 2016).

Socioeconomic status

Participants’ socioeconomic status (SES) was examined using the parental level of education item in the parent-reported “ABCD Longitudinal Parent Demographics Survey.” Possible educational levels ranged from 0 (“Never attended/Kindergarten only”) to 21 (“Doctoral degree”).

Traumatic experiences

An additional analysis was conducted in which parents reported traumatic experiences of their offspring in the “ABCD Parent Diagnostic Interview for DSM-5 (KSADS) Adversity Events” scale. This scale is part of a post-traumatic stress disorder module based on the Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia (KSADS) for the DSM-5 (Kobak et al., 2013; Kaufman et al., 2021). This 15-item binary scale included adverse experiences such as injury, war/terrorism, violence, sexual abuse, and death of a loved one (Barch et al., 2018; Hoffman et al., 2019).

Structural brain connectivity

We examined brain development trajectories by modeling FA data in canonical white matter tracts using DTI measures provided by ABCD and based on an ABCD-specific atlas: AtlasTrack (Hagler et al., 2009; Holland and Dale, 2011), at two imagining timepoints. We inspected four FA measures: FA of the CC, the fornix, anterior thalamic radiation (ATR), and the total FA across all tracts. Data from the fornix and anterior thalamic radiation were averaged across hemispheres. To ensure the reliability and quality control of the imaging data, the ABCD Data Analytics and Informatics Core (DAIC) equalizes MRI acquisition across sites and establishes standardized image processing and extraction for all measures prior to their release (Saragosa-Harris et al., 2022). For detailed information on the processing steps of the MR data used in ABCD, see Hagler et al. (2019).

Data preparation

Data were analyzed in R version 4.2.2 (R Core Team, 2021) with R studio version 2022.07.2 (RStudio Team, 2021).

Data wrangling for DTI measures

As preregistered, participants were excluded based on imaging quality control, motion control, and manual reviews provided in the release notes of ABCD (imgincl_dmri_include = “0,” imgincl_t1w_include = “0,” and mrif_score > “2”). Full information on inclusion and exclusion criteria can be found in the ABCD release notes and guidelines (Hagler et al., 2019). We additionally excluded participants with z-scores above 5 and participants with more than 70% (or more than 13 items) missing on the adversity and DTI measures (N = 1,072). The final dataset included 16,997 observations across 2 timepoints or 10,808 participants in total. For the number of observations per age, see Table 1 and Extended Data Figure 2.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Average age per imaging timepoint for all DTI tracts in months

Missingness

DTI data were collected twice for most participants: at baseline and/or at 2-year follow-up. After exclusion, there were 10,808 participants, 6,189 participants with 2 timepoints and 4,619 participants with data missing at one timepoint; see extended data for further information: https://osf.io/fv5ey. Overall, there was a missingness of ∼13% across all variables (with average levels per measure varying between 0.01% and 35% missingness).

Powerful statistical approaches to handling missing data, such as multiple imputations, can improve the generalizability of research findings (Little et al., 2014). Therefore, multivariate imputation via chained equations was used for multiple imputations (MICE package; van Buuren and Groothuis-Oudshoorn, 2011). This means that the missing average levels have been imputed using predictive mean matching (PMM; Morris et al., 2014). However, we replicated the complete analysis without imputations as a robustness check (see extended data: https://osf.io/fv5ey).

Analyses

Pre-registration

Before accessing the data, the study was preregistered (link here). We planned to determine average trajectories of brain development using LBGM (Duncan and Duncan, 2009). The latent basis growth model (LBGM) is a type of latent growth curve model (LBGM; Duncan and Duncan, 2009), with which change over time can be modeled without specifying a specific trajectory (McNeish, 2020). Using LBGM, we determined the average trajectories of brain development. We modeled FA data to determine the slope intercept and slope variance to determine an increase in FA and significant individual differences in FA among participants. While the LBGM showed a significant variation in the intercept, the slope variances were nonsignificant. This means that we did not find significant group trajectories of brain connectivity, which prevented the examination of individual differences using LBGM, as planned.

Exploratory analysis

Our subsequent exploratory analysis followed a two-step approach and was not preregistered. (1) We used K-means clustering to determine subgroups based on the total level of brain connectivity at imaging timepoint one plus timepoint two (total level) and the development of connectivity from timepoint one to timepoint two (trajectory). (2) We used these clusters to predict brain trajectory subgroups from adverse experiences using multinomial regression.

K-means clustering

K-means is an unsupervised learning method (i.e., without categorical specifications), and it is an established exploration technique. The advantage of the approach is its simplicity and efficiency, as it minimizes the average squared distance between points in the same cluster and groups objects into clusters (Hartigan and Wong, 1979; Lloyd, 1982; Jain, 2010). In this study, K-means clustering was conducted based on the total level of brain connectivity (total; FA at imaging timepoint 1 + timepoint 2: higher FA total level implies greater connectivity, lower FA total levels imply lower connectivity) and the development of brain connectivity from imaging timepoint one to two (trajectory; FA at timepoint 2 − timepoint 1). We computed the Euclidean distance d between a data point x and the mean μ of a cluster using the elements t1 and t2 , denoting the FA total levels of timepoints 1 and 2, respectively. We performed three analyses: combining the FA total level and trajectory (dVD , Eq. 1) andto disentangle the individual contributions of each, the FA total level individually (dV , Eq. 2) and the FA trajectory individually as robustness checks (dD , Eq. 3):dVD(x,μ)=((xt1+xt2)−(μt1+μt2))2+((xt2−xt1)−(μt2−μt1))2 (1)dV(x,μ)=((xt1+xt2)−(μt1+μt2))2 (2)dD(x,μ)=((xt2−xt1)−(μt2−μt1))2 (3)An optimal number of clusters was determined with the commonly used Elbow and Silhouette methods (Rousseeuw, 1987; Kodinariya and Makwana, 2013; Saptura et al., 2020). These methods showed us an optimal number of clusters (the “elbow”): If there were no distinct trajectories, these methods would show no clear evidence for a particular number of clusters. Additionally, we normalized the data to account for age differences and differences in the time period between the two imaging time points by centring the data around the corresponding mean level.

Multinomial regression analyses

Multinomial logistic regression was used to predict cluster membership based on adversity experiences at baseline. To adjust for multiple comparisons, p-values were Bonferroni-corrected. We corrected for the number of brain regions (four) and adversity scales (five).

Results

This research provides insights into the relationship between adversities and adolescent brain development. We hypothesized (1) increases in white matter connectivity (as measured by FA) from ages 9–13 years. We also predicted that (2) there will be individual differences in adolescent structural brain connectivity trajectories (FA trajectories) reflected in distinct subpopulations. We then predicted group membership with adverse life experiences. We followed a two-step analysis approach: (1) We used K-means clustering to determine subgroups based on the total level of brain connectivity (total level) and the trajectory of connectivity from one imaging timepoint to the next (trajectory), and (2) we used multinomial regression to predict cluster membership from adverse experiences. The code and results of the analyses can be accessed here.

LBGM

We could access only two neuroimaging timepoints per participant at the time of data access. Therefore, we planned to analyze the data by age in months rather than timepoint (as usually, at least three timepoints are required for a latent growth model). We fitted four LBGM across ages 9–13 (split into five timepoints, e.g., one per year of age) to estimate trajectories of FA across four different brain regions. This resulted in high levels of missingness, as each participant had only a maximum of 2 measures across 59 months (or 59 “timepoints”). Results below show the comparative fit index (CFI), root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), and standardized root mean square residual (SRMR). Intercept estimates, variances and covariances for all models can be seen in Table 2. The full results can be found here

All tracts

The all-tract growth model showed an acceptable fit: χ2(7) = 17.937, p = 0.012; CFI = 0.999; RMSEA = 0.012, SRMR = 0.026. There was a significant variation in the intercept, indicating significant individual differences at baseline. However, the slope variances were not significant, and, therefore, there was no significant growth variation in all-tract brain connectivity (e.g., no significant individual differences in trajectories).

CC

The latent basis growth model for the CC also showed an acceptable fit: χ2(7) = 10.493, p = 0.162; CFI = 1; RMSEA = 0.007, SRMR = 0.022. We found a significant variation in the intercept but no significant growth variation in brain connectivity across the CC. Again, indicating individual differences at baseline but not in trajectories.

Fornix

For the fornix, the latent basis growth model did not converge; however, a linear growth model showed an acceptable fit: χ2(10) = 13.690, p = 0.188; CFI = 0.999; RMSEA = 0.006, SRMR = 0.040. There was significant variation in the intercept, hence individual differences at baseline. Still, there was no significant growth variation.

Anterior thalamic radiation

Lastly, for the anterior thalamic radiation, the latent basis growth model showed an acceptable fit again: χ2(7) = 7.938, p = 0.338; CFI = 1; RMSEA = 0.004, SRMR = 0.039. There was significant variation in the intercept; however, there was no growth variation in brain connectivity.

Thus, across all tracts, the CC, fornix, and ATR, there was a significant variation in the intercept at baseline, indicating significant individual differences at baseline (Table 2). However, the slope variances were nonsignificant (Table 2), meaning that models did not identify individual differences in growth trajectories. This prevented the examination of individual differences using growth mixture models, as planned. To check whether this was an artifact of our analysis technique and the resulting high levels of missingness, we opted to use a different, non-preregistered analysis technique that allowed us to examine trajectories with only two timepoints: K-means clustering (Lloyd, 1982; Jain, 2010). Because this analysis was not preregistered and is therefore exploratory, we recommend that the LBGM analysis be replicated once at least three brain imaging data points are available in the ABCD data.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2.

Intercept (i) and slope (s) estimates, variance and covariance per model per brain region; all tracts (AT), corpus callosum (CC), fornix (F), and anterior thalamic radiation (ATR)

Overall, there was a slight increase in FA across all brain tracts, the CC, fornix, and anterior thalamic radiation as shown by significant positive slope estimate across all inspected brain tracts (Fig. 1). This is in line with previous studies (Paus et al., 2008; Lebel and Beaulieu, 2011; Palmer et al., 2021). This increase in FA is thought to reflect microstructural changes such as myelination and changes in axonal density and coherence (Lebel and Deoni, 2018).

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

FA across white matter tracts from age 9 (107 months), age 10 (119 months), age 11 (131 months), age 12 (143 months), to age 13 (155 months) years. A, FA across all white matter tracts. B, FA across the CC. C, FA across the fornix. D, FA across the anterior thalamic radiation.

K-means clustering across all brain tracts

To examine individual differences, we used K-means clustering in an exploratory analysis. We determined subpopulations across the DTI data based on the trajectory and total level of FA across all brain tracts, the CC, the fornix, and the ATR. An overview of all significant cluster differences can be found in Table 3. The full results can be found here.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 3.

Overview of significant cluster difference

All brain tracts—total level and trajectory

For total level and trajectory, the optimal number of clusters based on the Elbow and Silhouette method across all brain tracts was determined to be four. The clustering result can be seen in Figure 2. Cluster 1 can be described as high FA levels that increase over time. It is the largest cluster (N = 3,898). Cluster 2 shows medium FA levels that increase over time (N = 3,876). Cluster 3 shows high FA levels that decrease over time (N = 1,527). Cluster 3 has the most distinct trajectory from the total trajectories. Lastly, Cluster 4 shows low FA levels that increase steeply (N = 1,507; Fig. 2). A multinominal regression was performed to create a model of the relationship between the adversity predictors and cluster membership. This revealed that there is an overall significant difference between clusters for family conflict (χ2(3) = 14.83, pbonf = 0.039) and socioeconomic status (χ2(3) = 24.31, pbonf < 0.001).

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Clustering across all brain tracts using four clusters based on the total level and trajectory of FA data, including a locally estimated scatterplot smoothing (LOESS) line in dark blue. FA refers to FA data. T1 relates to imaging timepoint 1 and T2 to imaging timepoint 2.

The specific comparisons between clusters indicated a difference between Clusters 1 and 2 for family conflict (t(3) = −3.81, β = −0.09, pbonf = 0.003); a higher family conflict was associated with cluster membership in Cluster 1 compared to that in Cluster 2. In other words, higher family conflict was related to Cluster 1 with higher overall FA values compared to Cluster 2 with lower overall FA values (Fig. 2). This is in line with the robustness check we conducted based only on the total FA level, in which cluster membership in the cluster with higher FA levels was significantly associated with family conflict.

There was also a significant difference between Clusters 1 and 3 for SES (t(3) = 3.44, β = 0.10, pbonf = 0.01); lower socioeconomic status associated with cluster membership in Cluster 3 compared to Cluster 1. Therefore, participants in Cluster 3 with a decreasing FA trend between the two imaging timepoints were more likely to have lower SES status compared to Cluster 1 with an increasing FA trend (Fig. 2). Again, these results are reproduced in the robustness check we conducted based only on the trajectory of FA, where cluster membership in the cluster with the decreasing trend of FA values was significantly associated with lower SES.

We conducted an additional analysis, where we included all adversity predictors into one regression model to explore the influence on shared variance on group membership. A multinominal regression was performed to create a model of the relationship between all adversity predictors and cluster membership. This revealed that there is an overall significant difference between clusters for family conflict (χ2(3) = 14.51, pbonf = 0.046) and socioeconomic status (χ2(3) = 21.45, pbonf < 0.001). These results are in line with the previous logistic regression results conducted per adversity predictor.

K-means clustering across the CC

CC—total level and trajectory

For trajectory and total level, the optimal number of clusters based on the Elbow and Silhouette method in the CC was determined to be four, similar to the results of all tracts. The clustering result can be seen in Figure 3. Cluster 1 refers to the largest cluster (N = 4,082) and can be described as high and increasing; Cluster 2 was medium and increasing (N = 3,864), and Cluster 3 had a high and decreasing trend (N = 1,564). Lastly, Cluster 4 was low and steeply increasing (N = 1,298; Fig. 3). Cluster 3 had the most distinct trajectory, similar to all tracts. A multinomial regression was performed to create a model of the relationship between the adversity predictors and cluster membership based on the trajectory and total level of participant FA. The overall measure showed an overall significant difference between clusters for family conflict (χ2(3) = 14.99, pbonf = 0.04) and socioeconomic status (χ2(3) = 23.64, pbonf < 0.001), a result similar to all tracts.

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Clustering in the CC using four clusters based on the total level and trajectory of FA data. FA refers to FA data, including a smooth LOESS line in dark blue. FA refers to FA data. T1 relates to imaging timepoint 1 and T2 to imaging timepoint 2.

The specific cluster comparisons indicated a difference between Clusters 1 and 2 for family conflict (t(3) = −3.80, β = −0.09, pbonf = 0.003). This means that higher family conflict was again predictive of cluster membership in Cluster 1 compared to Cluster 2. Cluster 1, with higher overall FA values compared to Cluster 2, was again related to higher family conflict (Fig. 3). The robustness check we performed based on only the total level of FA replicated these results; cluster membership in the cluster with higher FA levels was significantly associated with family conflict. However, the specific comparisons and robustness check indicated no significant cluster differences of group membership for SES or neighborhood safety.

K-means clustering across the fornix

Fornix—total level and trajectory

Unlike across all tracts and the CC, the optimal number of clusters based on trajectory and total level in the Fornix was determined to be three. Cluster 1 was low and increasing (N = 3,967); Cluster 2 was high and stable (N = 3,664); and Cluster 3 can be described as medium and decreasing (N = 3,177; Fig. 4). Cluster 3 is again the cluster with the trajectory most distinct from the total trajectories. The multinominal regression modeling the relationship between the adversity predictors and cluster membership based on the trajectory and total level of participant FA showed a significant difference between cluster membership for neighborhood safety (χ2(2) = 15.43, pbonf = 0.009).

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Clustering in the Fornix using three clusters based on the total level and trajectory of FA data. FA refers to FA data, including a smooth LOESS line in dark blue. FA refers to FA data. T1 relates to imaging timepoint 1 and T2 to imaging timepoint 2.

Unlike the previous brain regions, the specific comparisons between clusters indicated a difference between Clusters 1 and 3 (t(2) = 3.59, β = 0.09, pbonf = 0.007) for neighborhood safety (Fig. 4). Therefore, lower perceived safety levels in the neighborhood were associated with cluster membership in Cluster 3 compared to Cluster 1. This means that participants in Cluster 1, with an increasing trend of FA values, were more likely to perceive their neighborhoods as safe compared to Cluster 3, with decreasing FA values. Again, these results are reproduced in the robustness check we conducted based only on the trajectory of FA, where cluster membership in the cluster with the decreasing trend of FA values was significantly associated with more (perceived) unsafe neighborhoods.

K-means clustering across the ATR

ATR—total level and trajectory

In the ATR, the optimal number of clusters based on total level and trajectory was determined to be three, similar to the fornix. The clustering result can be seen in Figure 5. Cluster 1 can be described as low and increasing (N = 4,747), Cluster 2 as medium and increasing (N = 3,831), and Cluster 3 as high and decreasing (N = 2,230; Fig. 5). Cluster 3 is again the cluster with the trajectory most distinct from the total trajectories. The multinominal regression of the relationship between the adversity predictors and clusters membership based on the trajectory and total level of participant FA for the first time showed an overall significance for three adversities: family conflict (χ2(2) = 22.62, pbonf < 0.001), neighborhood safety (χ2(2) = 12.23, pbonf = 0.04), and socioeconomic status (χ2(2) = 43.98, pbonf < 0.001).

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

Clustering across the ATR using three clusters based on the total level and trajectory of FA data, including a smooth LOESS line in dark blue. FA refers to FA data. T1 relates to imaging timepoint 1 and T2 to imaging timepoint 2.

The specific comparisons between clusters indicated a difference between Clusters 1 and 2 for family conflict (t(1) = 4.75, β = 0.10 pbonf <0.001); therefore, again, Cluster 2, with higher overall FA values compared to Cluster 1, was related to higher family conflict (Fig. 5). A robustness check based on only the total level of FA replicated these results. Cluster membership in the cluster with higher FA levels was significantly associated with family conflict.

The specific comparisons showed significant differences for cluster membership for both neighborhood safety between Clusters 1 and 3 (t(1) = 3.48, β = 0.09, pbonf <0.001) and SES between Clusters 1 and 3 (t(1) = 6.42, β = 0.16, pbonf < 0.001). This means that a decreasing trend of FA values was associated with more (perceived) dangerous neighborhoods and lower SES compared to Clusters with an increasing trend of FA values. Again, these results are reproduced in the robustness check we conducted based only on the trajectory of FA.

Replication of analysis without imputation

The primary analysis was based on multiple imputations to account for missingness in the data. To check the robustness of our findings, we have replicated our analyses without imputation (see Extended Data: https://osf.io/fv5ey). The relationship between SES and brain connectivity total level and trajectory was mostly replicated. Neighborhood safety remained significantly related to cluster membership for the fornix trajectories. After Bonferroni-correction, family conflict, however, was not significant across all tracts, CC, fornix, and ATR, unlike in the imputed analysis. For this reason, the results for family conflict results should be interpreted with caution.

In summary, neighborhood safety and socioeconomic status predicted different brain connectivity subgroups, while traumatic experiences and school satisfaction did not. Family conflict was predictive of connectivity in some, but not all analyses. Socioeconomic status was related to FA across all tracts, the CC, and the ATR. Neighborhood safety was significant for the fornix and ATR only. While the white matter tracts we discussed here play distinct roles in brain connectivity, there seem to be some similar trends across the examined regions, especially for family conflict. It appears that higher family conflict may be related to a higher overall level of FA, reflecting the relevance of family dynamics on brain development across youth. At the same time, lower socioeconomic status and neighborhood safety were related to a decreasing trend of FA across the two timepoints. The correlation between socioeconomic status and neighborhood safety may reflect the importance of the wider environment during youth.

Discussion

This study explored the relationship between adverse experiences during early adolescence (age 9–13 years) and individual differences in neurodevelopmental trajectories. We used LBGM and K-means clustering to investigate the link between brain connectivity total levels and trajectories with adverse experiences in early adolescence. We found that higher family conflict was related to higher overall FA levels, while lower socioeconomic status and neighborhood safety were related to decreasing FA trends over time. Therefore, this study demonstrates a possible link between adverse experiences and brain connectivity during early adolescence. These results have theoretical and practical implications and enhance our understanding of brain development during adolescence.

Development and trajectories of brain connectivity

Evidence presented here of a significant overall increase in FA from age 9 to 13 years is in line with Hypothesis 1 and previous research, which is thought to reflect increasing white matter myelination and axonal value (McLaughlin et al., 2007; Lebel and Beaulieu, 2011; Palmer et al., 2021). Regarding Hypothesis 2, we found mixed evidence for individual differences in trajectories. While most participants showed an increase of FA between the two imaging timepoints, our clustering analysis revealed that overall and in each of the brain regions, a subgroup of participants (ranging from 14 to 29% of the total sample) showed a decline in FA total level between the two collection timepoints. This could reflect individual differences in brain development in early adolescence. That said, the slope variances of the LBGM were nonsignificant, likely due to high levels of missingness in this analysis approach. Because of this mixed evidence, future studies are needed to scrutinize longitudinal brain development using additional brain imaging data, as two timepoints only capture a short time span, and three or more time points will allow us to better capture brain development trajectories (Parsons and McCormick, 2022). Additionally, we were restricted by the limited availability of adversity scales in the ABCD study. Due to the study design, it was only possible to include adversity measures at baseline. There is a need to examine brain development longitudinally to increase our understanding of the complex relationship between adverse experiences and the brain, and future cohort studies should consider including richer scales exploring adversities during adolescence.

Family conflict and white matter structure

Across all brain tracts, the CC, the fornix, and ATR, family conflict was related to higher FA levels across the two neuroimaging timepoints. Families and care settings are typically the first social environments a person experiences (Ornelas et al., 2007; Vettore et al., 2019), and although peer relationships become increasingly important, adversities in the family environment are still relevant during youth (Pollmann et al., 2022). The family conflict scale in the ABCD study included items related to anger, criticism, and physical abuse in the family: an example item includes “We fight a lot in our family.” Previous research has suggested that abuse in the family could be related to white matter development and alterations in white matter tract integrity in youth (Choi et al., 2009; Huang et al., 2012; Bick et al., 2015). That said, the finding did not replicate in a robustness check, and the effects of family conflict may depend on the missing data handling. These findings should, therefore, be replicated in future studies with more time points.

Socioeconomic status, neighborhood safety, and white matter development

Across all tracts, the CC, and anterior thalamic radiation, lower socioeconomic status was related to a decreasing trend of FA between two timepoints. This finding was largely replicated in our robustness checks. The CC is the largest white matter structure in the brain, and one of its key functions is facilitating communication between the left and right hemispheres (Gazzaniga, 2000). The CC is involved in different complex cognitive functions, such as working memory and even overall intelligence (Aboitiz et al., 1992; Hofer and Frahm, 2006; Luders et al., 2007). The ATR are involved in communicating sensory or motor information from the thalamus (George and Das, 2022). It is associated with movement and implicated in some affective disorders (Lai and Wu, 2014; George and Das, 2022).

Socioeconomic status is often discussed in social science and politics, as it is known to be related to a wide range of important lifespan outcomes (Reiss et al., 2019), including decreased mental and physical health and even earlier death (Felitti et al., 1998). As children transition into adolescence, social and biological changes make them vulnerable to external influences (Andersen and Teicher, 2008; Kolb, 2009). Adolescents from lower SES backgrounds may face more challenges in navigating these changes, such as an increased likelihood of experiencing adverse experiences (Maguire-Jack et al., 2021). Research has shown a link between lower SES or family income and lower FA total levels (Dufford and Kim, 2017; Assari and Boyce, 2021) which, in turn, could be related to individual differences in cognitive flexibility (Ursache et al., 2016).

Lower perceived neighborhood safety was related to a decreasing trend in FA between the two imaging timepoints in the anterior thalamic radiation and the fornix. The fornix facilitates the transfer and processing of information within and between the limbic system and other regions in the brain and has been implicated in cognitive and emotional disorders (Thomas et al., 2011; Senova et al., 2020). Like SES, perceived neighborhood cohesion and safety influence young people's behavior, health, and development (Ingoldsby et al., 2006; De Jesus et al., 2010; Burdette and Needham, 2012). Neighborhood safety may be particularly important in adolescence compared to childhood. Adolescents typically enjoy greater independence and unsupervised time outside their homes (Sawyer et al., 2018). Therefore, living in perceived unsafe neighborhoods increases their likelihood of being exposed to crime and violence (Haynie et al., 2006; McBride Murry et al., 2011).

The relationship between SES and neighborhood safety with white matter trajectories shows a potential pathway by which the environment may influence developmental outcomes such as mental health. However, it is important to note that we cannot rule out potential genetic confounding or gene–environment interactions. For instance, the interaction of an individual's genetic makeup and environmental experiences may impact neurodevelopment (McCrory et al., 2010). Additionally, we note that our conclusions are based on observational evidence. Future research should therefore build on these findings to test potential confounders and whether interventions aimed at improving conditions for disadvantaged youth, both in terms of socioeconomic status and neighborhood safety, might be promising for supporting growth, health, and well-being in adolescence and beyond.

Implications and future directions

This study explored the relationship between adverse experiences during early adolescence and total levels of and trajectories of change in white matter connectivity in the adolescent brain. Most studies to date have looked at average trajectories of brain development and investigations of individual developmental differences are currently lacking often due to the limited availability of longitudinal brain imaging data (Crone and Elzinga, 2015; Foulkes and Blakemore, 2018). When individual variations in structural and functional development are investigated, individual differences are usually found (Mills et al., 2014). This study is one of the first to show that individual brain trajectories in adolescence are linked to adversity. Using K-means clustering and multinomial regression, this study provides preliminary evidence that adversity is associated with brain connectivity during early adolescence (age 9–13 years). Examining average levels of white matter connectivity, and connectivity in the corpus callosum, the fornix, and anterior thalamic radiation, we showed that family conflict was associated with higher overall connectivity levels across all tracts and within multiple specific tracts. Low socioeconomic status and perceived neighborhood safety were associated with decreased connectivity over time, potentially indicating an increasing importance of the wider social environment of brain development during adolescence. Future research should include further comprehensive investigations of additional structural metrics to illustrate brain development and potential opportunities and obstacles of development across the lifespan.

Overall, this study shows a complex and multifaceted relationship between adverse experiences and brain development during early adolescence. Adolescence is a distinct period in life marked by social and biological changes (Sawyer et al., 2018). Understanding how environmental factors like adversity shape brain development during this time of life is therefore important. Our findings have implications for future research on adversity's lifelong consequences: research should not only focus on early life adversity but should also illuminate how adversity shapes lifespan outcomes during later periods of life.

Footnotes

  • A.P. has been funded through a PhD research studentship from the Department of Psychology, King’s College London, and the Cusanuswerk. R.S was funded by Intelligent Games and Games Intelligence CDT (IGGI; EP/S022325/1). D.F. and K.B. were supported by an ESRC Secondary Data Analysis Initiative Grant (ES/T015861/1). For the purposes of open access, the author has applied a Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license to any accepted author manuscript version arising from this submission. Data used in the preparation of this article were obtained from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study (https://abcdstudy.org), held in the NIMH Data Archive (NDA). This is a multisite, longitudinal study designed to recruit more than 10,000 children aged 9–10 years and follow them over 10 years into early adulthood. The ABCD Study is supported by the National Institutes of Health and additional federal partners under the following award numbers: U01DA041048, U01DA050989, U01DA051016, U01DA041022, U01DA051018, U01DA051037, U01DA050987, U01DA041174, U01DA041106, U01DA041117, U01DA041028, U01DA041134, U01DA050988, U01DA051039, U01DA041156, U01DA041025, U01DA041120, U01DA051038, U01DA041148, U01DA041093, U01DA041089, U24DA041123, and U24DA041147. A full list of supporters is available at https://abcdstudy.org/federal-partners.html. A listing of participating sites and a complete listing of the study investigators can be found at https://abcdstudy.org/consortium_members/. ABCD consortium investigators designed and implemented the study and/or provided data but did not necessarily participate in the analysis or writing of this report. This manuscript reflects the views of the authors and may not reflect the opinions or views of the NIH or ABCD consortium investigators. The ABCD data repository grows and changes over time. The ABCD data used in this report came from NIMH Data Archive Digital Object Identifier: 10.15154/1526076. DOIs can be found at: https://nda.nih.gov/study.html?id=1527.

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Ayla Pollmann at ayla.pollmann{at}kcl.ac.uk.

SfN exclusive license.

References

  1. ↵
    Brain Development Cooperative Group (2012) Total and regional brain volumes in a population-based normative sample from 4 to 18 years: the NIH MRI study of normal brain development. Cereb Cortex 22:1–12. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhr018
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Aboitiz F,
    2. Scheibel AB,
    3. Fisher RS,
    4. Zaidel E
    (1992) Fiber composition of the human corpus callosum. Brain Res 598:143–153. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(92)90178-C
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    BPS (2019) Adverse childhood experiences (ACEs) | BPS. Available at: https://www.bps.org.uk/news-and-policy/adverse-childhood-experiences-aces (Accessed June 9, 2022).
  4. ↵
    1. Alexander AL,
    2. Lee JE,
    3. Lazar M,
    4. Field AS
    (2007) Diffusion tensor imaging of the brain. Neurotherapeutics 4:316–329. doi:10.1016/j.nurt.2007.05.011
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Andersen SL,
    2. Teicher MH
    (2008) Stress, sensitive periods and maturational events in adolescent depression. Trends Neurosci 31:183–191. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2008.01.004
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Assari S,
    2. Boyce S
    (2021) Race, socioeconomic status, and cerebellum cortex fractional anisotropy in pre-adolescents. Adolescents 1:70–94. doi:10.3390/adolescents1020007
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  7. ↵
    1. Barch DM
    , et al. (2018) Demographic, physical and mental health assessments in the adolescent brain and cognitive development study: rationale and description. Dev Cogn Neurosci 32:55–66. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2017.10.010
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Bathelt J,
    2. Johnson A,
    3. Zhang M,
    4. Astle DE
    (2019) The cingulum as a marker of individual differences in neurocognitive development. Sci Rep 9:2281. doi:10.1038/s41598-019-38894-z
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  9. ↵
    1. Beaulieu C
    (2002) The basis of anisotropic water diffusion in the nervous system – a technical review. NMR Biomed 15:435–455. doi:10.1002/nbm.782
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Benedetti F,
    2. Bollettini I,
    3. Radaelli D,
    4. Poletti S,
    5. Locatelli C,
    6. Falini A,
    7. Smeraldi E,
    8. Colombo C
    (2014) Adverse childhood experiences influence white matter microstructure in patients with bipolar disorder. Psychol Med 44:3069–3082. doi:10.1017/S0033291714000506
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Bick J,
    2. Zhu T,
    3. Stamoulis C,
    4. Fox NA,
    5. Zeanah C,
    6. Nelson CA
    (2015) Effect of early institutionalization and foster care on long-term white matter development: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Pediatr 169:211–219. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2014.3212
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Boullier M,
    2. Blair M
    (2018) Adverse childhood experiences. Paediatr Child Health 28:132–137. doi:10.1016/j.paed.2017.12.008
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  13. ↵
    1. Burdette AM,
    2. Needham BL
    (2012) Neighborhood environment and body mass index trajectories from adolescence to adulthood. J Adolesc Health 50:30–37. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2011.03.009
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Carlson JS,
    2. Yohannan J,
    3. Darr CL,
    4. Turley MR,
    5. Larez NA,
    6. Perfect MM
    (2020) Prevalence of adverse childhood experiences in school-aged youth: a systematic review (1990–2015). Int J Sch Educ Psychol 8:2–23. doi:10.1080/21683603.2018.1548397
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  15. ↵
    1. Chahal R,
    2. Miller JG,
    3. Yuan JP,
    4. Buthmann JL,
    5. Gotlib IH
    (2022) An exploration of dimensions of early adversity and the development of functional brain network connectivity during adolescence: implications for trajectories of internalizing symptoms. Dev Psychopathol 34:557–571. doi:10.1017/S0954579421001814
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  16. ↵
    1. Choi J,
    2. Jeong B,
    3. Rohan ML,
    4. Polcari AM,
    5. Teicher MH
    (2009) Preliminary evidence for white matter tract abnormalities in young adults exposed to parental verbal abuse. Biol Psychiatry 65:227–234. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.06.022
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Cicchetti D
    (2016) Socioemotional, personality, and biological development: illustrations from a multilevel developmental psychopathology perspective on child maltreatment. Annu Rev Psychol 67:187–211. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033259
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Crone EA,
    2. Elzinga BM
    (2015) Changing brains: how longitudinal functional magnetic resonance imaging studies can inform US about cognitive and social-affective growth trajectories. WIREs Cogn Sci 6:53–63. doi:10.1002/wcs.1327
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  19. ↵
    1. De Bellis MD,
    2. Keshavan MS,
    3. Shifflett H,
    4. Iyengar S,
    5. Beers SR,
    6. Hall J,
    7. Moritz G
    (2002) Brain structures in pediatric maltreatment-related posttraumatic stress disorder: a sociodemographically matched study. Biol Psychiatry 52:1066–1078. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01459-2
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. De Jesus M,
    2. Puleo E,
    3. Shelton RC,
    4. Emmons KM
    (2010) Associations between perceived social environment and neighborhood safety: health implications. Health Place 16:1007–1013. doi:10.1016/j.healthplace.2010.06.005
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Dube SR,
    2. Felitti VJ,
    3. Dong M,
    4. Chapman DP,
    5. Giles WH,
    6. Anda RF
    (2003) Childhood abuse, neglect, and household dysfunction and the risk of illicit drug use: the adverse childhood experiences study. Pediatrics 111:564–572. doi:10.1542/peds.111.3.564
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Dufford AJ,
    2. Kim P
    (2017) Family income, cumulative risk exposure, and white matter structure in middle childhood. Front Hum Neurosci 11:547. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2017.00547
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  23. ↵
    1. Duncan TE,
    2. Duncan SC
    (2009) The ABC’s of LGM: an introductory guide to latent variable growth curve modeling. Soc Personal Psychol Compass 3:979–991. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2009.00224.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Eccles JS,
    2. Roeser RW
    (2011) Schools as developmental contexts during adolescence. J Res Adolesc 21:225–241. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00725.x
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  25. ↵
    1. Echeverria SE,
    2. Diez-Roux AV,
    3. Link BG
    (2004) Reliability of self-reported neighborhood characteristics. J Urban Health 81:682–701. doi:10.1093/jurban/jth151
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Felitti VJ,
    2. Anda RF,
    3. Nordenberg D,
    4. Williamson DF,
    5. Spitz AM,
    6. Edwards V,
    7. Koss MP,
    8. Marks JS
    (1998) Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults. Am J Prev Med 14:245–258. doi:10.1016/S0749-3797(98)00017-8
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Fields RD
    (2008) White matter in learning, cognition and psychiatric disorders. Trends Neurosci 31:361–370. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2008.04.001
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    1. Fornito A,
    2. Harrison B
    (2012) Brain connectivity and mental illness. Front Psychiatry 3:72. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2012.00072
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    1. Foulkes L,
    2. Blakemore S-J
    (2018) Studying individual differences in human adolescent brain development. Nat Neurosci 21:315–323. doi:10.1038/s41593-018-0078-4
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Fuhrmann D,
    2. Knoll LJ,
    3. Blakemore S-J
    (2015) Adolescence as a sensitive period of brain development. Trends Cogn Sci 19:558566. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2015.07.008
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Fuhrmann D,
    2. van Harmelen A-L,
    3. Kievit RA
    (2022) Well-being and cognition are coupled during development: a preregistered longitudinal study of 1,136 children and adolescents. Clin Psychol Sci 10:450–466. doi:10.1177/21677026211030211
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  31. ↵
    1. Gazzaniga MS
    (2000) Cerebral specialization and interhemispheric communication: does the corpus callosum enable the human condition? Brain 123:1293–1326. doi:10.1093/brain/123.7.1293
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. George K,
    2. Das JM
    (2022) Statpearls. StatPearls Publishing.
  33. ↵
    1. Gonzalez R
    , et al. (2021) An update on the assessment of culture and environment in the ABCD study®: emerging literature and protocol updates over three measurement waves. Dev Cogn Neurosci 52:101021. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2021.101021
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  34. ↵
    1. Hagler DJ
    , et al. (2019) Image processing and analysis methods for the adolescent brain cognitive development study. NeuroImage 202:116091. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116091
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Hagler DJ,
    2. Ahmadi ME,
    3. Kuperman J,
    4. Holland D,
    5. McDonald CR,
    6. Halgren E,
    7. Dale AM
    (2009) Automated white-matter tractography using a probabilistic diffusion tensor atlas: application to temporal lobe epilepsy. Hum Brain Mapp 30:1535–1547. doi:10.1002/hbm.20619
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Hartigan JA,
    2. Wong MA
    (1979) Algorithm AS 136: a K-means clustering algorithm. Appl Stat 28:100. doi:10.2307/2346830
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  37. ↵
    1. Haynie DL,
    2. Silver E,
    3. Teasdale B
    (2006) Neighborhood characteristics, peer networks, and adolescent violence. J Quant Criminol 22:147–169. doi:10.1007/s10940-006-9006-y
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  38. ↵
    1. Hofer S,
    2. Frahm J
    (2006) Topography of the human corpus callosum revisited–comprehensive fiber tractography using diffusion tensor magnetic resonance imaging. NeuroImage 32:989–994. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.05.044
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Hoffman E,
    2. Clark D,
    3. Orendain N,
    4. Hudziak J,
    5. Squeglia L,
    6. Dowling G
    (2019) Stress exposures, neurodevelopment and health measures in the ABCD study. Neurobiol Stress 10:100157. doi:10.1016/j.ynstr.2019.100157
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  40. ↵
    1. Holland D,
    2. Dale AM
    (2011) Nonlinear registration of longitudinal images and measurement of change in regions of interest. Med Image Anal 15:489–497. doi:10.1016/j.media.2011.02.005
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. Huang H,
    2. Gundapuneedi T,
    3. Rao U
    (2012) White matter disruptions in adolescents exposed to childhood maltreatment and vulnerability to psychopathology. Neuropsychopharmacology 37:2693–2701. doi:10.1038/npp.2012.133
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. Hughes K,
    2. Ford K,
    3. Kadel R,
    4. Sharp CA,
    5. Bellis MA
    (2020) Health and financial burden of adverse childhood experiences in England and Wales: a combined primary data study of five surveys. BMJ Open 10:e036374. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2019-036374
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  43. ↵
    1. Ingoldsby EM,
    2. Shaw DS,
    3. Winslow E,
    4. Schonberg M,
    5. Gilliom M,
    6. Criss MM
    (2006) Neighborhood disadvantage, parent–child conflict, neighborhood peer relationships, and early antisocial behavior problem trajectories. J Abnorm Child Psychol 34:293–309. doi:10.1007/s10802-006-9026-y
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    1. Jain AK
    (2010) Data clustering: 50 years beyond K-means. Pattern Recognit Lett 31:651–666. doi:10.1016/j.patrec.2009.09.011
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  45. ↵
    1. Johansen-berg H,
    2. Scholz J,
    3. Stagg C
    (2010) Relevance of structural brain connectivity to learning and recovery from stroke. Front Syst Neurosci 4:146. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2010.00146
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    1. Karcher NR,
    2. Barch DM
    (2021) The ABCD study: understanding the development of risk for mental and physical health outcomes. Neuropsychopharmacology 46:131–142. doi:10.1038/s41386-020-0736-6
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  47. ↵
    1. Kaufman J,
    2. Kobak K,
    3. Birmaher B,
    4. de Lacy N
    (2021) KSADS-COMP perspectives on child psychiatric diagnostic assessment and treatment planning. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 60:540–542. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2020.08.470
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  48. ↵
    1. Kobak KA,
    2. Kratochvil C,
    3. Stanger C,
    4. Kaufman J
    (2013) Computerized screening for comorbidity in adolescents with substance or psychiatric disorders. clinicaltrials.gov. Available at: https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT01866956 (Accessed Feb. 16, 2023).
  49. ↵
    1. Kodinariya T,
    2. Makwana P
    (2013) Review on determining of cluster in K-means clustering. Int J Adv Res Comput Sci Manag Stud 1:90–95. doi:ISSN: 2321-7782
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  50. ↵
    1. Kolb B
    (2009) Brain and behavioural plasticity in the developing brain: neuroscience and public policy. Paediatr Child Health 14:651–652. doi:10.1093/pch/14.10.651
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    1. Lai C-H,
    2. Wu Y-T
    (2014) Alterations in white matter micro-integrity of the superior longitudinal fasciculus and anterior thalamic radiation of young adult patients with depression. Psychol Med 44:2825–2832. doi:10.1017/S0033291714000440
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  52. ↵
    1. Lebel C,
    2. Beaulieu C
    (2011) Longitudinal development of human brain wiring continues from childhood into adulthood. J Neurosci 31:10937–10947. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5302-10.2011
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  53. ↵
    1. Lebel C,
    2. Deoni S
    (2018) The development of brain white matter microstructure. NeuroImage 182:207–218. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.12.097
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    1. Liao H,
    2. Yan C,
    3. Ma Y,
    4. Wang J
    (2021) Impact of adverse childhood experiences on older adult poverty: mediating role of depression. Front Public Health 9:749640. doi:10.3389/fpubh.2021.749640
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  55. ↵
    1. Lim L,
    2. Hart H,
    3. Howells H,
    4. Mehta MA,
    5. Simmons A,
    6. Mirza K,
    7. Rubia K
    (2019) Altered white matter connectivity in young people exposed to childhood abuse: a tract-based spatial statistics (TBSS) and tractography study. J Psychiatry Neurosci 44:E11–E20. doi:10.1503/jpn.170241
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  56. ↵
    1. Lim L,
    2. Howells H,
    3. Radua J,
    4. Rubia K
    (2020) Aberrant structural connectivity in childhood maltreatment: a meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 116:406–414. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.07.004
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  57. ↵
    1. Little TD,
    2. Jorgensen TD,
    3. Lang KM,
    4. Moore EWG
    (2014) On the joys of missing data. J Pediatr Psychol 39:151–162. doi:10.1093/jpepsy/jst048
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  58. ↵
    1. Lloyd S
    (1982) Least squares quantization in PCM. IEEE Trans Inf Theory 28:129–137. doi:10.1109/TIT.1982.1056489
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  59. ↵
    1. Lu S,
    2. Wei Z,
    3. Gao W,
    4. Wu W,
    5. Liao M,
    6. Zhang Y,
    7. Li W,
    8. Li Z,
    9. Li L
    (2013) White matter integrity alterations in young healthy adults reporting childhood trauma: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 47:1183–1190. doi:10.1177/0004867413508454
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  60. ↵
    1. Luders E,
    2. Narr KL,
    3. Bilder RM,
    4. Thompson PM,
    5. Szeszko PR,
    6. Hamilton L,
    7. Toga AW
    (2007) Positive correlations between corpus callosum thickness and intelligence. NeuroImage 37:1457–1464. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.06.028
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  61. ↵
    1. Maguire-Jack K,
    2. Font S,
    3. Dillard R,
    4. Dvalishvili D,
    5. Barnhart S
    (2021) Neighborhood poverty and adverse childhood experiences over the first 15 years of life. Int J Child Maltreatment Res Policy Pract 4:93–114. doi:10.1007/s42448-021-00072-y
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  62. ↵
    1. McBride Murry V,
    2. Berkel C,
    3. Gaylord-Harden NK,
    4. Copeland-Linder N,
    5. Nation M
    (2011) Neighborhood poverty and adolescent development. J Res Adolesc 21:114–128. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00718.x
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  63. ↵
    1. McCarthy-Jones S,
    2. Oestreich LKL,
    3. Lyall AE,
    4. Kikinis Z,
    5. Newell DT,
    6. Savadjiev P,
    7. Shenton ME,
    8. Kubicki M,
    9. Pasternak O,
    10. Whitford TJ
    (2018) Childhood adversity associated with white matter alteration in the corpus callosum, corona radiata, and uncinate fasciculus of psychiatrically healthy adults. Brain Imaging Behav 12:449–458. doi:10.1007/s11682-017-9703-1
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  64. ↵
    1. McCrory E,
    2. De Brito SA,
    3. Viding E
    (2010) Research review: the neurobiology and genetics of maltreatment and adversity. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 51:1079–1095. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02271.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  65. ↵
    1. McLaughlin NCR
    , et al. (2007) Diffusion tensor imaging of the corpus callosum: a cross-sectional study across the lifespan. Int J Dev Neurosci 25:215–221. doi:10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2007.03.008
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  66. ↵
    1. McNeish D
    (2020) Relaxing the proportionality assumption in latent basis models for nonlinear growth. Struct Equ Model Multidiscip J 27:817–824. doi:10.1080/10705511.2019.1696201
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  67. ↵
    1. Mills KL,
    2. Goddings A-L,
    3. Clasen LS,
    4. Giedd JN,
    5. Blakemore S-J
    (2014) The developmental mismatch in structural brain maturation during adolescence. Dev Neurosci 36:147–160. doi:10.1159/000362328
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  68. ↵
    1. Moos RH,
    2. Moos BS
    (1994) Family environment scale manual: development, applications, research, 3rd ed. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
  69. ↵
    1. Morris TP,
    2. White IR,
    3. Royston P
    (2014) Tuning multiple imputation by predictive mean matching and local residual draws. BMC Med Res Methodol 14:75. doi:10.1186/1471-2288-14-75
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  70. ↵
    1. Mujahid MS,
    2. Diez Roux AV,
    3. Morenoff JD,
    4. Raghunathan T
    (2007) Assessing the measurement properties of neighborhood scales: from psychometrics to ecometrics. Am J Epidemiol 165:858–867. doi:10.1093/aje/kwm040
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  71. ↵
    1. Ornelas IJ,
    2. Perreira KM,
    3. Ayala GX
    (2007) Parental influences on adolescent physical activity: a longitudinal study. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 4:3. doi:10.1186/1479-5868-4-3
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  72. ↵
    1. Palmer CE,
    2. Pecheva D,
    3. Iversen JR,
    4. Hagler DJ,
    5. Sugrue L,
    6. Nedelec P,
    7. Fan CC,
    8. Thompson WK,
    9. Jernigan TL,
    10. Dale AM
    (2021) Microstructural development from 9 to 14 years: evidence from the ABCD study. Dev Cogn Neurosci 53:101044. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2021.101044
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  73. ↵
    1. Parsons S,
    2. McCormick EM
    (2022) “Don’t peek at your data” applies to longitudinal studies too: two time points poorly capture trajectories of change. Available at: https://psyarxiv.com/96ph3/ (Accessed Feb. 16, 2023).
  74. ↵
    1. Paul R
    , et al. (2008) The relationship between early life stress and microstructural integrity of the corpus callosum in a non-clinical population. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat 4:193–201. doi:10.2147/NDT.S1549
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  75. ↵
    1. Paus T
    (2010) Growth of white matter in the adolescent brain: myelin or axon? Brain Cogn 72:26–35. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2009.06.002
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  76. ↵
    1. Paus T,
    2. Keshavan M,
    3. Giedd JN
    (2008) Why do many psychiatric disorders emerge during adolescence? Nat Rev Neurosci 9:947–957. doi:10.1038/nrn2513
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  77. ↵
    Anon (2016) PhenX Toolkit: protocols. Available at: https://www.phenxtoolkit.org/protocols/view/540501 (Accessed Feb. 17, 2023).
  78. ↵
    1. Pollmann A,
    2. Fritz J,
    3. Barker ED,
    4. Fuhrmann D
    (2022) Networks of adversity in childhood and adolescence and their relationship to adult mental health. Available at: https://psyarxiv.com/e5xmn/ (Accessed July 1, 2022).
  79. ↵
    R Core Team (2021) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Found Stat Comput Vienna Austria. Available at: https://www.r-project.org/ (Accessed June 20, 2021).
  80. ↵
    1. Reiss F,
    2. Meyrose A-K,
    3. Otto C,
    4. Lampert T,
    5. Klasen F,
    6. Ravens-Sieberer U
    (2019) Socioeconomic status, stressful life situations and mental health problems in children and adolescents: results of the German BELLA cohort-study. PLoS One 14:e0213700. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0213700
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  81. ↵
    1. Rousseeuw PJ
    (1987) Silhouettes: a graphical aid to the interpretation and validation of cluster analysis. J Comput Appl Math 20:53–65. doi:10.1016/0377-0427(87)90125-7
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  82. ↵
    RStudio Team (2021) RStudio: integrated development for R. RStudio. PBC Boston MA. Available at: http://www.rstudio.com/ (Accessed June 20, 2021).
  83. ↵
    1. Saptura D,
    2. Saputra D,
    3. Oswari L
    (2020) Effect of distance metrics in determining K-value in K-means clustering using elbow and silhouette method. In: Proceedings of the Sriwijaya International Conference on Information Technology and Its Applications (SICONIAN 2019). pp 341–346. Atlantis Press. doi:10.2991/aisr.k.200424.051
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  84. ↵
    1. Saragosa-Harris NM
    , et al. (2022) A practical guide for researchers and reviewers using the ABCD study and other large longitudinal datasets. Dev Cogn Neurosci 55:101115. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2022.101115
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  85. ↵
    1. Sawyer SM,
    2. Azzopardi PS,
    3. Wickremarathne D,
    4. Patton GC
    (2018) The age of adolescence. Lancet Child Adolesc Health 2:223–228. doi:10.1016/S2352-4642(18)30022-1
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  86. ↵
    1. Senova S,
    2. Fomenko A,
    3. Gondard E,
    4. Lozano AM
    (2020) Anatomy and function of the fornix in the context of its potential as a therapeutic target. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 91:547–559. doi:10.1136/jnnp-2019-322375
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  87. ↵
    1. Sheikh HI,
    2. Joanisse MF,
    3. Mackrell SM,
    4. Kryski KR,
    5. Smith HJ,
    6. Singh SM,
    7. Hayden EP
    (2014) Links between white matter microstructure and cortisol reactivity to stress in early childhood: evidence for moderation by parenting. NeuroImage Clin 6:77–85. doi:10.1016/j.nicl.2014.08.013
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  88. ↵
    1. Short AK,
    2. Baram TZ
    (2019) Early-life adversity and neurological disease: age-old questions and novel answers. Nat Rev Neurol 15:657–669. doi:10.1038/s41582-019-0246-5
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  89. ↵
    1. Soares J,
    2. Marques P,
    3. Alves V,
    4. Sousa N
    (2013) A hitchhiker’s guide to diffusion tensor imaging. Front Neurosci 7:31. doi:10.3389/fnins.2013.00031
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  90. ↵
    1. Sporns O
    (2013) Structure and function of complex brain networks. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 15:247–262. doi:10.31887/DCNS.2013.15.3/osporns
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  91. ↵
    1. Tendolkar I,
    2. Mårtensson J,
    3. Kühn S,
    4. Klumpers F,
    5. Fernández G
    (2018) Physical neglect during childhood alters white matter connectivity in healthy young males. Hum Brain Mapp 39:1283–1290. doi:10.1002/hbm.23916
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  92. ↵
    1. Thomas AG,
    2. Koumellis P,
    3. Dineen RA
    (2011) The fornix in health and disease: an imaging review. RadioGraphics 31:1107–1121. doi:10.1148/rg.314105729
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  93. ↵
    1. Thomason ME,
    2. Thompson PM
    (2011) Diffusion imaging, white matter, and psychopathology. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 7:63–85. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032210-104507
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  94. ↵
    1. Tian T,
    2. Li J,
    3. Zhang G,
    4. Wang J,
    5. Liu D,
    6. Wan C,
    7. Fang J,
    8. Wu D,
    9. Zhou Y,
    10. Zhu W
    (2020) Effects of childhood trauma experience and COMT Val158Met polymorphism on brain connectivity in a multimodal MRI study. Brain Behav 10:e01858. doi:10.1002/brb3.1858
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  95. ↵
    1. Ursache A,
    2. Noble KG
    , The Pediatric Imaging, Neurocognition and Genetics Study (2016) Socioeconomic status, white matter, and executive function in children. Brain Behav 6:e00531. doi:10.1002/brb3.531
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  96. ↵
    1. Van Buuren S,
    2. Groothuis-Oudshoorn K
    (2011) Mice: multivariate imputation by chained equations in R. J Stat Softw 45:1–67. doi:https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v045.i03
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  97. ↵
    1. Vettore MV,
    2. Ahmad SFH,
    3. Machuca C,
    4. Fontanini H
    (2019) Socio-economic status, social support, social network, dental status, and oral health reported outcomes in adolescents. Eur J Oral Sci 127:139–146. doi:10.1111/eos.12605
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  98. ↵
    1. Zucker RA,
    2. Gonzalez R,
    3. Feldstein Ewing SW,
    4. Paulus MP,
    5. Arroyo J,
    6. Fuligni A,
    7. Morris AS,
    8. Sanchez M,
    9. Wills T
    (2018) Assessment of culture and environment in the adolescent brain and cognitive development study: rationale, description of measures, and early data. Dev Cogn Neurosci 32:107–120. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2018.03.004
    OpenUrlCrossRef
View Abstract
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 44 (8)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 44, Issue 8
21 Feb 2024
  • Table of Contents
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Masthead (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Making Connections: Neurodevelopmental Changes in Brain Connectivity After Adverse Experiences in Early Adolescence
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Making Connections: Neurodevelopmental Changes in Brain Connectivity After Adverse Experiences in Early Adolescence
Ayla Pollmann, Remo Sasso, Kathryn Bates, Delia Fuhrmann
Journal of Neuroscience 21 February 2024, 44 (8) e0991232023; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0991-23.2023

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Making Connections: Neurodevelopmental Changes in Brain Connectivity After Adverse Experiences in Early Adolescence
Ayla Pollmann, Remo Sasso, Kathryn Bates, Delia Fuhrmann
Journal of Neuroscience 21 February 2024, 44 (8) e0991232023; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0991-23.2023
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Significance Statement
    • Introduction
    • Method
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • adverse childhood experiences
  • adverse adolescent experiences
  • brain connectivity
  • family conflict
  • fractional anisotropy
  • K-means clustering
  • latent basis growth models
  • neighborhood safety
  • socioeconomic status
  • traumatic experiences

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Articles

  • Strategies to decipher neuron identity from extracellular recordings in behaving non-human primates
  • Cell type-dependent short-term plasticity and dopaminergic modulation of sensory synapses onto mouse superficial dorsal horn neurons
  • Interference of mid-level speech and noise statistics underlies human speech recognition sensitivity in natural environmental noise.
Show more Research Articles

Development/Plasticity/Repair

  • Developmental Changes in Brain Cellular Membrane and Energy Metabolism: A Multi-Occasion 31P Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy Study
  • Microglia Support Both the Singular Form of LTP Expressed by the Lateral Perforant Path and Episodic Memory
  • The Epigenetic Reader PHF23 Is Required for Embryonic Neurogenesis
Show more Development/Plasticity/Repair
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Accessibility
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.