Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Research Articles, Behavioral/Cognitive

Dissociable Causal Roles of Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex and Primary Motor Cortex over the Course of Motor Skill Development

Quynh N. Nguyen, Katherine J. Michon, Michael Vesia and Taraz G. Lee
Journal of Neuroscience 14 May 2025, 45 (20) e2015232025; https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2015-23.2025
Quynh N. Nguyen
1Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan 48109
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Katherine J. Michon
1Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan 48109
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Michael Vesia
2School of Kinesiology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan 48109
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Taraz G. Lee
1Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan 48109
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Taraz G. Lee
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Established models of motor skill learning posit that early stages of learning are dominated by an attentionally demanding, effortful mode of control supported by associative corticostriatal circuits involving the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC). As skill develops, automatic and “effortless” performance coincides with a transition to a reliance on sensorimotor circuits that include primary motor cortex (M1). However, the dynamics of how control evolves during the transition from novice to expert are currently unclear. This lack of clarity is due, in part, to the fact that most motor learning studies comprise a limited number of training sessions and rely on correlative techniques such as neuroimaging. Here, we train human participants (both sexes) on a discrete motor sequencing task over the course of 6 weeks, followed by an assessment of the causal roles of DLPFC and M1 at varying levels of expertise. We use repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation to transiently disrupt activity in these regions immediately prior to performance in separate sessions. Our results confirm the dissociable importance of DLPFC and M1 as training progresses. DLPFC stimulation leads to larger behavioral deficits for novice skills than more highly trained skills, while M1 stimulation leads to relatively larger deficits as training progresses. However, our results also reveal that prefrontal disruption causes performance deficits at all levels of training. These findings challenge existing models and indicate an evolving rather than a strictly diminishing role for DLPFC throughout learning.

  • dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
  • motor cortex
  • motor learning
  • motor skills
  • sequence learning
  • transcranial magnetic stimulation

Significance Statement

Motor skills involve the sequential chaining of individual actions. For example, playing the piano involves learning to rapidly transition to from one finger press to another. Human neuroimaging studies have shown that primary motor cortex (M1) and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) support novice motor sequencing skills, but activity in both regions declines over training. This has been interpreted as increased efficiency in M1 and yet a reduction in the involvement of DLPFC as expertise develops. We causally test this assumption by using noninvasive brain stimulation to transiently disrupt cortical activity following extended skill training. Although we confirm dissociable contributions of DLPFC and M1 as training progresses, we show that both regions are necessary for performance regardless of skill level.

Introduction

Most models of motor skill learning suggest that with practice, movements transition from being attentionally demanding and effortful to more automatic (Fitts and Posner, 1967; Newell, 1991; Willingham, 1998; Abrahamse et al., 2013). The neurophysiological changes that accompany motor skill development are often studied by examining motor sequencing tasks that involve merging motor primitives together to create a smooth, multicomponent action. These tasks are intended to mimic complex actions, such as a tennis serve, that require binding serial actions together. During the early stages of learning, performing motor skills is thought to require cognitive resources from an associative corticostriatal circuit that includes the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC; Hikosaka et al., 2002; Doyon and Benali, 2005; Verwey et al., 2019). This associative circuit is thought to be released from control as a “slow-learning” sensorimotor corticostriatal circuit, including primary motor cortex (M1), dominates performance at later stages of learning (Hikosaka et al., 2002; Doyon and Benali, 2005).

However, recent work has questioned the roles of DLPFC and M1 in skilled motor performance. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies suggest that M1 is responsible for executing individual discrete movements rather than containing skill-specific representations (Yokoi et al., 2018; Yokoi and Diedrichsen, 2019; Berlot et al., 2020). Further, M1 may not be necessary for the expression of an expert skill at all, as its ablation in rodents does not alter performance following extensive training (Kawai et al., 2015). Several neuroimaging and noninvasive brain stimulation studies suggest that increased activity in the DLPFC may be detrimental to the long-term development of skill and performance following initial training (Galea et al., 2010; Cohen and Robertson, 2011; Bassett et al., 2015). A fast pace of improvement over the course of training may rely on rapid reductions in the contribution of DLPFC (Bassett et al., 2015) and using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to disrupt DLPFC activity just following an initial training session has been shown to result in improved performance in subsequent sessions (Galea et al., 2010; Cohen and Robertson, 2011).

Neural plasticity likely involves multiple, temporally overlapping processes, complicating our understanding of how the brain supports motor skill learning. For example, learning leads to a shift in neuronal recruitment whereby new neuronal populations become engaged (Doyon and Benali, 2005; Dayan and Cohen, 2011; Wymbs and Grafton, 2015). Other brain regions that were integral to performance early on show reductions in activity as measured by fMRI because they are no longer needed (Toni et al., 1998; Ungerleider, 2002; Wymbs and Grafton, 2015). However, reduced activity can also reflect increases in efficiency whereby neurons maintain the same function while receiving less presynaptic input (Picard et al., 2013). These diverging possibilities have made inferences from studies using fMRI difficult, as there is evidence for both increased and decreased activity levels in various regions of interest (Doyon and Benali, 2005; Dayan and Cohen, 2011). For example, M1 and DLPFC often exhibit similar decreases in activity over the course of training (Steele and Penhune, 2010; Dayan and Cohen, 2011). However, this pattern is often interpreted in these studies as reflecting increased efficiency in M1 and a reduced role in supporting performance for DLPFC. More recent neuroimaging studies examining motor sequencing skills have used multivariate analysis techniques to examine distributed patterns of activity within a single brain area to resolve the ambiguity inherent in interpreting univariate increases and decreases in BOLD activity. For example, several cortical motor areas have been shown to carry more distinct sequence-specific patterns of activity over the course of training even as univariate levels of activity decline (Wiestler and Diedrichsen, 2013; Yokoi and Diedrichsen, 2019; Berlot et al., 2020). Although these studies have provided some clarity by identifying cortical areas that continue to code for various aspects of motor skill as proficiency develops, neuroimaging is an inherently correlational technique and cannot definitively speak to the causal necessity of a brain region for a particular task condition.

TMS can provide such strong causal evidence for the role of brain regions in cognitive processes of interest by measuring the effect of their disruption on behavior (Pitcher et al., 2021). Most studies examining motor skill learning using TMS have examined short timescales, such as single-session training, which cannot provide insight into the long-term dynamics of neurophysiological changes necessary for skilled motor performance.

To address the shortcomings of previous studies, the present study used TMS to investigate the causal role of DLPFC and M1 in the expression of learned motor behavior in humans by studying motor sequencing skills trained over a 6 week period. Our longitudinal within-participant design allowed us to assess the impact of cortical disruption on three different levels of skill in a single session. If M1 is increasingly involved in supporting skilled performance as the skill develops, we would expect disrupting M1 activity would have the most detrimental impact on highly trained skills and a relatively minimal impact on skills that have been minimally trained. If, however, M1 is primarily important for producing each discrete movement in isolation, we would expect a static cost to M1 disruption across all skill levels. If DLPFC is only important during the initial acquisition of motor sequencing skills and is not needed once performance nears ceiling, we would expect large deficits in the performance of novice skills following its disruption with essentially no impact on the most highly trained skills. DLPFC disruption may even lead to improved performance.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Twenty-one right-handed participants (six females; mean age, 21.7 years) were recruited locally from the University of Michigan and surrounding community. All participants gave informed consent to participate and were compensated for their time. All participants additionally underwent a TMS Adult Safety Screening to assess the potential risk of adverse reactions to TMS. All experimental protocols were approved by the Institutional Review Boards of the University of Michigan Medical School (IRBMED).

Discrete sequence production task

Participants performed the discrete sequence production (DSP) task using their left (nondominant) hand (Fig. 1). Each trial began with a color cue (1.5 s) signifying the identity of the sequence to be performed. Then a row of four gray rectangles corresponding to the four fingers of the left hand were presented at the center of the display (1 s during training; variable duration 3–8 s during scanning). After this delay period, one rectangle would turn white, signaling the first element of the cued sequence. As soon as participants pressed the corresponding button with the correct finger, another box would turn white according to the next element in the motor sequence and so on, until eight elements of the sequence were pressed, or until the time deadline of 3 s (8 s for training sessions). Incorrect key press at any element within a sequence would cause the corresponding rectangle to turn red and would also conclude the trial. Participants were instructed to complete each trial as quickly as possible while still being correct. For each participant, we randomly selected six different sequences from a common pool of 11 distinct sequences that were free of key repetitions and trills (e.g., 1–2–1). We randomly paired each of the selected sequences with a distinct color cue (blue, red, green, magenta, orange, purple) for each participant. This procedure ensures that the effects observed reflect differences in the level of expertise attained and do not depend on particular finger transitions or the particular sequence identity cues used.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Discrete sequence production (DSP) task. On each trial, the colored cue signaled the identity of the upcoming 8-item motor sequence. Participants pressed the key corresponding to the illuminated position on the screen. As soon as a key was pressed, the next element of the sequence was immediately illuminated. The timing denoted here pertains to the baseline session and the test sessions (see Materials and Methods).

Design overview

Following an initial screening session to determine individualized TMS parameters and assess stimulation tolerability (see TMS protocol below), each participant completed an initial fMRI scan, during which they were introduced to the DSP task described above. Then, each participant completed 24 training sessions over a 6 week period (∼4 d/week) where they practiced six different motor sequences. These sequences involved different eight-element key presses and had different practice frequencies: two sequences were practiced at three trials/session (minimal training; 72 trials total), another two at 15 trials/session (intermediate training; 360 trials total), and the last two at 45 trials/session (extensive training; 1080 trials total) using a within-subjects, counterbalanced design. Each training session comprised three blocks of 42 trials each. This training regime was designed such that each participant would obtain skills at varying levels of proficiency (minimally trained, moderately trained, and extensively trained) that could be assessed concurrently in a single session following training. Although the exact number of trials chosen for each level of training was somewhat arbitrary, prior work with motor sequencing tasks has shown clear neural differences following a similar training regime (Bassett et al., 2015; Wymbs and Grafton, 2015). The first seven participants completed training sessions on an in-lab computer. Due to the COVID-19 pandemic, training for the remaining 14 participants was completed on their personal computers at home to minimize in-person contact. At the end of the training period, each participant simultaneously possessed a relatively high level of skill in some sequences and was a relative novice in other sequences. In the initial fMRI session, participants were not exposed to the sequences that would be minimally trained and were asked to perform the four extensive/moderate sequences for a total of 48 trials each. Participants then returned to undergo three separate sessions where inhibitory TMS (see below) was applied to either DLPFC, M1, or a control site just prior to DSP performance (8 blocks of 30 trials; 40 trials of each sequence total; ∼5 min per block) in the fMRI scanner to assess the impact of cortical disruption on performance (see Fig. 2A for a schematic of the overall timeline of the experiment). These sessions were spaced at least 48 h apart to prevent carry-over effects (mean of 6.9 d between sessions; standard deviation of 3.6 d), and the site of stimulation was counterbalanced across participants. Neuroimaging data from these sessions are not reported here.

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Experimental timeline. A, Participants first attended a baseline session during which they gained initial exposure to the DSP task and some experience with the sequences that would be extensively and moderately trained. Structural MRI scans were also obtained during the baseline session. Participants then practiced the DSP task for 6 weeks (4 sessions/week) with varying training frequencies for each sequence (2 Extensive: 45 trials/session; 2 Moderate: 15 trials/session; 2 Minimal: 3 trials/session). At the end of training participants simultaneously had a relatively high level of proficiency on certain sequences but were relative novices on others. Participants then returned to three follow-up sessions where they received inhibitory TMS over one of three cortical targets (DLPFC, M1, or a control site) just prior to an assessment of performance on all the trained sequences. The order of stimulation was counterbalanced across participants. B, Right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) target projected onto a standard surface mesh. C, Right primary motor cortex (M1) target projected onto a standard surface mesh. Right M1 was identified anatomically for each individual. The cortical control site for stimulation (data not shown) was medial postcentral gyrus (putative hip/leg area of somatosensory cortex) and was likewise identified anatomically for each individual.

TMS target definition

The right DLPFC stimulation site was defined using coordinates from a meta-analysis of executive action control (Cieslik et al., 2013). Specifically, we used a region along the right posterior inferior frontal sulcus (MNI coordinate: 37, 33, 32) that is activated by tasks involving attention to action and action execution (Fig. 2B). Additionally, this area shows strong task-based and resting-state functional connectivity with dorsal premotor cortex and presupplementary motor area (Cieslik et al., 2013). We created TMS targets by first normalizing each participant's structural MRI scan to the MNI152 2009 template (Fonov et al., 2011). The resulting transformation matrix was used to reverse normalize a 5 mm radius spherical region of interest (ROI) into each participant's native space. The M1 stimulation site was defined as the hand knob of right M1 using each participant's anatomical MRI scan (Fig. 2C). Specifically, this site was localized anatomically for each participant based on previously reported anatomical landmarks (Yousry et al., 1997). We confirmed that a single suprathreshold TMS pulse at this location elicited a twitch in the left hand of each participant to ensure that this location was the hand area of M1 in all participants. To control for nonspecific effects of brain stimulation (cutaneous sensation, auditory stimulation, induction of current in the cortex), we selected the midline of the bilateral postcentral gyrus in each participant as a control site. This area putatively corresponds to the leg/trunk area of somatosensory cortex and was not activated by the DSP task in the baseline fMRI session. A frameless neuronavigation system (Brainsight 2, Rogue Research) aligned the previously acquired structural scan to each participant's skull in real time to allow for precise targeting.

TMS protocol

Repetitive TMS (rTMS) was delivered through a MagPro X100 magnetic stimulator and a 70 mm figure-8 coil (MC-B70, MagVenture). In the initial screening session, motor evoked potentials (MEPs) elicited using biphasic posterior-anterior stimulation and coil oriented 45° to coronal plane were recorded from left first dorsal interosseous (FDI) using surface electromyography (2-channel built in EMG device, Rogue Research). Active motor threshold (AMT) was determined as the percentage of stimulator output that elicits an MEP of ≥ 200 μV peak-to-peak on five out of 10 trials while the FDI muscle is contracted at 20% of maximum. At the end of this screening session, participants were given several pulses at 80% AMT over regions of the scalp roughly corresponding to DLPFC, M1, and the control site to ensure that the stimulation intensity would be tolerable in future sessions. No participants reported discomfort nor were any participants screened out in this process. In the experimental sessions following training, we delivered continuous theta-burst stimulation (cTBS) over a single region (DLPFC, M1, or the control site) with standard parameters and within consensus safety recommendations (Oberman et al., 2011; Rossi et al., 2020): three pulses of stimulation at 50 Hz at 80% AMT, repeated every 200 ms, for a total of 600 pulses in 40 s. The cTBS protocol has been found to have a disruptive effect on the site of stimulation for ∼60 min following stimulation (Huang et al., 2005), allowing an assessment of DSP task performance during the period of disruption. The mean time between the end of stimulation and the start of the first block of DSP performance was 6.83 min (SD = 1.42 min).

Statistical analyses

For each trial, we computed the initiation time (IT), interkey intervals (IKIs), movement time (MT), and trial-level accuracy. IT is defined as the time from when the first rectangle turns white (denoting visual presentation of the first sequence element) to the time of the actual correct key press. IKI is defined as the time from any key press to the next immediate key press. MT is defined as the time from the first key press to the last key press (i.e., the total duration of all eight finger movements). IT, IKI, and MT were taken from accurate trials only. A trial is considered accurate only if all sequence elements are correctly executed within the time limit. Only trials with at least one key press were included in calculating accuracy rate as we assume trials with no response are due to attentional lapses rather than motor execution errors (136 trials total across all participants and all TMS sessions, ∼0.9% of all trials). Because of the main effect of training depth observed at baseline, the same absolute decrease in MT or accuracy would mean more for the extensively trained sequences that are executed very quickly than for the minimally trained sequences that are executed much more slowly. To properly weigh the impact of TMS disruption across different skill levels, we calculated the percentage change in performance relative to the control condition rather than the absolute change in MT or accuracy for each level of skill (Anderson et al., 2020).

We then assessed the effect of skill level, indexed by training depth, and of certain brain regions, indexed by TMS site, on each of the above metrics using a linear mixed-effects model. We modeled training depth and TMS site as fixed effects and participant identity as the grouping factor. We also included day order as a nuisance covariate to remove the confounding effect of extra practice on sessions involving TMS on behavioral performance. Note that Figures 4⇓–6 all plot the dependent variables with this effect of day regressed out. The full model was formulated as follows: DVijk=μ+βskilskillj+βsitesitek+βskil×siteskilljsitek+βdayday+γi+εijk, for participant i performing a sequence previously trained to skill level j immediately after receiving TMS to site k. DV can be any of the behavioral metrics defined above. We used trialwise DV values for all analyses, except for control-day accuracy due to nonconstant residuals in the model fit. We remedied this heteroscedasticity by averaging the accuracy rate per sequence per subject and then taking the logit transformation of the average. We implemented all models in Python using the statsmodels package (Seabold and Perktold, 2010).

It is possible that the three training intensities we have arbitrarily chosen (Minimal, Moderate, Extensive) do not precisely reflect the actual skill levels attained by different participants as they underwent training. A talented participant may become more skilled at typing minimally trained sequences than an inept participant is at typing extensively trained sequences. Even within the same training intensity, different participants may demonstrate different levels of mastery. This means that comparing the effects of brain stimulation across training intensities may not have to do with the amount of training per se but might instead reflect the impact of stimulation at a particular level of competency. To control for the confounding effect of competency on TMS disruption, we conducted two additional analyses to confirm that the effects of stimulation reported were driven by the extent of training experience.

In the first analysis, we correlated the degree of TMS-induced impairment with the level of competency following training across participants using Spearman's rank coefficient. This was done separately for each training intensity (Minimal, Moderate, Extensive) and each TMS site (DLPFC, M1). We used average movement time and average accuracy during control stimulation to index the competency of a participant. We used the average percentage change in movement time and in accuracy during DLPFC or M1 TMS condition to index the degree of TMS-induced impairment, after controlling for the effect of day. Statistical significance was assessed after Bonferroni’s correction to account for the number of correlational analyses (3 training intensities × 2 TMS sites = 6 analyses).

In the second analysis, we statistically controlled for the level of competency by including it as a covariate in the linear mixed-effects model. To operationalize the level of competency demonstrated by a given participant at each training intensity, we ranked participants' performance (keys per second, see next section). Values below the 0.25 percentile were considered rank 1, between 0.25 and 0.75 percentile rank 2, and above the 0.75 percentile rank 3. This allows us to see how any participant performs at any training intensity relative to the whole group. We can then complement the nominal training intensities (Minimal, Moderate, Extensive) with the actual levels of competency (1, 2, 3). The new linear mixed-effects model was formulated as follows:DVijkl=μ+βskilskillj+βsitesitek+βrankrankl+βskil×siteskilljsitek+βrank×siteranklsitek+βdayday+γi+εijkl.

This analysis should allow us to control for an individual's level of competency in performing a particular sequence when examining the impact of stimulation on performance at each level of training.

Speed–accuracy composite skill score

To control for the well-known tradeoff in speed and accuracy, we calculated composite scores combining both measures to encapsulate the total skill level of our participants for each sequence.

To assess performance for the control stimulation condition, we operationalized “skill score” as the number of correct keys pressed per second (KPS). KPS is computed by dividing the total number of key presses after the first key, by the time from the first correct key press to the last key press, regardless of its accuracy. To avoid division by zero, we only included trials with at least two key presses in the calculation of KPS.

To examine the impact of DLPFC and M1 stimulation on performance, we took an additional step to incorporate each individual's baseline level of performance after control stimulation into the skill score. For each participant and each sequence, we computed the average KPS and average partial accuracy in the control session only. Partial accuracy was operationalized as the number of correct key presses after the first key divided by either the number of attempted key presses after the first key, if the trial terminated within 3 s, or 7 otherwise. We then subtracted this baseline from the corresponding KPS or partial accuracy value of each trial of the same sequence in the other TMS sessions of the same participant. This gave us a %ΔKPS and %Δpacc value for each trial, both of which scale with the relative change in performance in the same direction (positive, gain; negative, loss). The average of this bivariate measure was taken to index the combined dynamic of speed–accuracy.

Chunking analysis

In motor sequencing tasks, it is well-known that longer sequences are “chunked” into smaller subsequences that can each be performed quickly as training progresses. These chunks are thought to reflect a shift from stimulus–response to response–response processing. A “chunk point” is defined as the start of a chunk. This is the key press that has a significantly longer IKI than both the previous and the next key press. We made three assumptions in identifying chunk points: (1) the first key press is always a chunk point, (2) the second key press cannot be a chunk point, and (3) neither can the last key press. Chunk points were identified for each sequence and each participant using only the correct key presses from all the trials in a session. From these chunk points, we computed the chunk number and average chunk length of each sequence. Chunk number and average chunk length were calculated using data from all sessions.

We then examined the effect of TMS on two other chunking metrics: chunk initiation and chunk execution. This was done by applying the chunk points defined on the control session to the correct trials of the active TMS sessions. Chunk initiation was calculated as the trialwise mean IKI associated with the chunk points. Chunk execution was calculated as the trialwise mean IKI associated with the remaining key presses. Similar to the previous analyses, we calculated the percentage change in these metrics relative to the control condition, rather than their absolute change, for each level of skill. We excluded the first key press from all chunking analyses.

Results

The evolution of performance during training

Our main hypotheses centered around the roles DLPFC and M1 play in supporting minimally trained, moderately trained, and extensively trained motor skills. Participants practiced six separate motor sequences trained to three different depths in 24 separate sessions over a 6 week period so that they would all concurrently have two different sequencing skills at each level of proficiency.

To confirm that participants successfully learned all sequences during the training period, we compared the raw MT required to execute each sequence and the accuracy rate for each sequence at the end of Week 6 to that at the beginning of Week 1. Over the course of training, participants markedly sped up sequence execution as indexed by the total MT required to press all eight keys accurately (Week 6 vs 1: β = −1,183.0, z = −52.8, p < 0.001 overall; p < 0.001 for every skill level). The rate of improvement was higher for more deeply trained sequences compared with the minimally trained ones (Week × Skill level Extensive vs Minimal: β = 291.5, z = 4.6, p < 0.001; Moderate vs Minimal: β = 278.1, z = 4.2, p < 0.001). Participants similarly improved their accuracy rate over the course of training for the moderately and extensively trained sequences (accuracy Week 6 vs 1 Extensive: β = 0.042, z = 3.5, p < 0.001; Moderate: β = 0.068, z = 3.3, p = 0.001). We also observed a quadratic skill level × time interaction suggesting that participants’ accuracy improved to a greater extent on moderately and extensively trained sequences compared with minimally trained sequences (β = 0.052, z = 2.0, p = 0.043).

To evaluate potential ceiling effects toward the end of training, we compared MT at the end of Week 6 with that at the end of Week 5. Highly trained sequences continued to improve even during this last week of training (Week 6 vs 5 overall: β = −59.5, z = −2.7, p = 0.008; Extensive: β = −112.0, z = −8.7, p < 0.001; Moderate: β = −127.8, z = −5.6, p < 0.001; Minimal: β = 61.4, z = 1.0, p = 0.323). Note that there were many fewer trials for the minimally trained sequences, which may have limited our power to detect an effect.

We also sought to confirm that participants had three levels of skill proficiency on different sequences concurrently. To this end, we compared MT of the three levels of training at the end of Week 6 in a pairwise manner. In line with our expectations, extensively trained sequences were executed the fastest, followed by moderately trained sequences, and then minimally trained sequences, which were executed the most slowly (Extensive vs Moderate: β = −324.3, z = −17.5, p < 0.001; Extensive vs Minimal: β = −1,078.6, z = −23.9, p < 0.001; Moderate vs Minimal: β = −754.4, z = −16.0, p < 0.001). The quadratic contrast across skill level was significant (β = 175.6, z = 7.8, p < 0.001), indicating that the improvement in MT was larger going from minimal to moderate levels of training relative to the improvement observed moderate and extensive training. We observed similar results when examining accuracy rate. Participants were more accurate in completing extensively trained sequences relative to moderately trained sequences (β = 0.041, z = 2.5, p = 0.013) and more accurate on both extensive and moderate sequences when compared with minimally trained sequences (Extensive vs Minimal: β = 0.314, z = 9.4, p < 0.001; Moderate vs Minimal: β = 0.273, z = 7.7, p < 0.001). The quadratic contrast across skill level was again significant, indicating that the improvement in accuracy rate was larger going from minimal to moderate levels of training relative to the further improvement seen going from moderate to extensive training (β = −0.095, z = −5.2, p < 0.001).

Individuals learn at different rates, where some improve more rapidly than others during the same training sessions. For example, the extensively trained sequences in some participants may not always be executed within the same range of time as in other participants—although they are always executed faster than moderate and minimally trained sequences for every participant. To account for such interparticipant variation while maintaining the intraparticipant skill gradient, we standardized the MT separately for each participant by subtracting the participant mean from all values and dividing the differences by the participant standard deviation. This generated a list of z-scored movement times for each participant. Repeating the above analyses on the new metric yielded the same results as observed in the raw MT when comparing improvement over the course of training and the levels of skill attained at the end of training (Week 6 vs 1: β = −1.714, z = −54.0, p < 0.001 overall; p < 0.001 for every skill level; Week × Skill level Extensive vs Minimal: β = 0.287, z = 3.2, p = 0.001; Moderate vs Minimal: β = 0.277, z = 3.0, p = 0.003; Week 6 vs 5: β = −0.085, z = −2.7, p = 0.007 overall; p < 0.001 for Extensive and Moderate levels, p = 0.194 for Minimal level; end-of-training Extensive vs Moderate: β = −0.475, z = −18.2, p < 0.001; Extensive vs Minimal: β = −1.586, z = −24.8, p < 0.001; Moderate vs Minimal: β = −1.111, z = −16.7, p < 0.001; Linear contrast across skill levels: β = −1.122, z = −24.8, p < 0.001; Quadratic contrast: β = 0.260, z = 8.1, p < 0.001; Fig. 3A).

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

DSP performance over the course of six weeks of training. A, Movement time (MT) decreased universally and uniformly across all sequences. Curves denote double-exponential curves fit to the group-average MT for each sequence separately (colored lines) and combined across sequences (dotted line). Note the overlapping learning trajectories. B, After training was complete, participants exhibited the desired gradient of skill level where minimally trained sequences were executed the most slowly, moderately trained sequences somewhat more quickly, and extensively trained sequences the most quickly (data from control stimulation session). C, The difference in execution speed is not due to a speed-accuracy tradeoff across skill level as minimally trained sequences were the least accurately performed and moderately and extensively trained sequences showed similar levels of accuracy. D, The effects of training were also reflected in the initiation time for each sequence such that more highly trained sequences were initiated more quickly than less highly trained sequences following presentation of the visual cue for the first element in each sequence. E, Another way to look at the effect of training on the speed of performance is to examine the duration of the average interkey interval (this is essentially a rescaling of movement time and the effects of training are identical). Note the data in B–E reflect the raw dependent measures for visualization rather than the standardized measures used in the analyses. Error bars represent SEM.

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

Performance changes following M1 stimulation. M1 disruption led to deficits in both (A) the speed of execution and (B) the accuracy of execution relative to performance after control stimulation. However, this performance cost was graded by skill level such that the minimally trained skills were impacted the least relative to the moderately and extensively trained skills. ΔMT, % change in movement time relative to control stimulation. Δ accuracy, % change in accuracy relative to control stimulation.

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

Performance changes following DLPFC stimulation. DLPFC disruption led to deficits in both (A) the speed of execution and (B) the accuracy of execution relative to performance after control stimulation. However, this performance cost was graded by skill level such that the minimally trained skills were impacted the most relative to the moderately and extensively trained skills. ΔMT, % change in movement time relative to control stimulation. Δ accuracy, % change in accuracy relative to control stimulation.

Figure 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 6.

Dissociable impact of M1 and DLPFC stimulation on skilled motor performance as a function of the extent of training. Although disruption of both M1 and DLPFC led to deficits in skilled performance, we observed a significant interaction between the site of stimulation and the level of training. The cost of disruption was reduced as a function of training following DLPFC stimulation but increased as a function of training following M1 stimulation.

Effects of training (i.e., performance following control stimulation)

Our active control stimulation condition serves as a comparison for the effects of DLPFC and M1 stimulation as it controls for nonspecific effects of stimulation (e.g., auditory clicks, sensation of stimulation at the scalp, current induced in the cortex, etc.). As such, we first verified that the basic pattern of results from the last training session still held for all participants in this session. We confirmed the effect of training depth on standardized MT, where the more deeply trained sequences were executed more quickly than the more shallowly trained sequences during this session (Linear contrast across skill levels: β = −0.868, z = −35.2, p < 0.001; Quadratic contrast: β = 0.108, z = 4.5, p < 0.001; Extensive vs Moderate: β = −0.5, z = −14.5, p < 0.001; Extensive vs Minimal: β = −1.2, z = −35.2, p < 0.001; Moderate vs Minimal: β = −0.7, z = −21.4, p < 0.001). We also observed a significant effect of training depth on accuracy, where the more deeply trained sequences were executed more accurately than the minimally trained sequences (Linear contrast across skill levels: β = 0.494, z = 3.2, p < 0.001; Quadratic contrast: β = −0.354, z = −2.3, p = 0.022; Extensive vs Minimal: β = 0.698, z = 3.2, p < 0.001; Moderate vs Minimal: β = 0.783, z = 3.6, p < 0.001). We did not observe a significant difference in accuracy between extensively trained sequences and moderately trained sequences (Extensive vs Moderate: β = −0.084, z = −0.4, p = 0.698). Accuracy on extensively and moderately trained sequences potentially converged due to ceiling effects (Extensive accuracy rate = 84.7%, moderate accuracy rate = 85.9%, minimal accuracy rate = 72.1%). These results indicate that extensive training did in fact improve performance and does not reflect a simple shift along a single speed–accuracy function as sequences could be performed more quickly without an increase in errors (Fig. 3B,C).

Given the coupled improvement in both movement time and accuracy as a function of training depth, we sought to unify these quantities into a single metric to encapsulate performance at each level of skill (see Materials and Methods). For this purpose, we calculated a “skill score” that combines the number of correct keys pressed as a function of time to assess the combined effect of movement time and accuracy: Faster movement time coupled with higher accuracy should strongly increase skill score and vice versa. We again found a main effect of training depth on skill score where more keys were pressed correctly within a shorter amount of time for deeply trained sequences compared with shallowly trained ones (Linear contrast across skill levels: β = 0.786, z = 27.6, p < 0.001; Quadratic contrast: β = −0.059, z = −2.1, p = 0.040; Extensive vs Moderate: β = 0.48, z = 12.0, p < 0.001; Extensive vs Minimal: β = 1.11, z = 27.6, p < 0.001; Moderate vs Minimal: β = 0.63, z = 15.5, p < 0.001).

One possible mechanism by which training can improve performance is via an improvement in motor planning (rather than in simply execution itself). Recall that each sequence is cued in advance and there is a variable delay between cue presentation and the presentation of the first item in the sequence. Thus, one index of motor planning is the initial response time (initiation time) to the presentation of the first item in each sequence as this first key press could be prepared and deployed immediately as the associated key position was illuminated. This hypothesis found some support as initiation time was significantly faster for the extensively trained sequences compared with the moderately and minimally trained sequences (Extensive vs Moderate: β = −21.1, z = −4.5, p < 0.001; Extensive vs Minimal: β = −22.3, z = −4.6, p < 0.001; Fig. 3D). Though numerically in the same direction, we did not observe a significant effect when comparing moderately to minimally trained sequences (β = −1.2, z = −0.3, p = 0.801).

Training can also improve other aspects of performance, such as the speed of individual key presses or the formation of motor chunks. Key-to-key transitions benefited from training as we observed a main effect of training depth for all key positions (p < 0.001 for all pairwise comparisons at all transitions, except for Extensive vs Moderate at the 2-to−3 transition, p = 0.013; Fig. 3E). With faster transitions, several individual key presses can be executed in quick succession. These discrete subsets of key presses are called motor chunks. We found a main quadratic effect of training on chunk number and average chunk length, where higher skill levels have fewer and longer chunks (Chunk number: Linear contrast: β = −0.056, z = −1.1, p = 0.251; Quadratic contrast: β = 0.150, z = 3.2, p = 0.001; Chunk length: Linear contrast: β = 0.058, z = 0.9, p = 0.360; Quadratic contrast: β = −0.199, z = −3.3, p = 0.001). These results suggested that training had beneficial impacts on all aspects of the motor skill, from planning through execution.

Effects of M1 disruption

BOLD activity in M1 has been reported to decrease over the course of training during the performance of motor sequencing skills (Steele and Penhune, 2010; Dayan and Cohen, 2011). Some previous researchers have made the conjecture that this decrease reflects an increase in metabolic efficiency of the region as learning progresses (Steele and Penhune, 2010; Kornysheva and Diedrichsen, 2015; Wymbs and Grafton, 2015). However, other recent neuroimaging studies have struggled to find any evidence for distinct skill-specific patterns of activity in M1 as skill develops (Yokoi et al., 2018; Yokoi and Diedrichsen, 2019). The evolving causal role for M1 in supporting motor skill as a function of motor skill proficiency has thus remained unclear. Our design allows us to make causal inferences regarding the role of M1 in motor skills as we disrupt M1 activity with inhibitory TMS and examine its impact on behavioral performance at each level of skill.

M1 disruption produced deficits in the speed of execution for all skill levels relative to the control stimulation condition (%ΔMT vs vertex Minimal: β = 0.047, z = 2.2, p = 0.029; Moderate: β = 0.097, z = 4.6, p < 0.001; Extensive β = 0.092, z = 4.3, p < 0.001). We next examined whether the disruptive effects of stimulation varied as a function of skill level. We found strong evidence for an effect of skill level on the impact of M1 disruption. M1 stimulation produced larger impairments in performance for the more extensively trained sequences (Linear contrast across skill levels: β = 0.032, z = 7.3, p < 0.001; Quadratic contrast: β = −0.023, z = −5.4, p < 0.001; Fig. 4A). Extensively and moderately trained sequences were executed more slowly than minimally trained sequences after controlling for their baseline differences (%ΔMT M1 vs vertex Extensive vs Minimal: β = 0.045, z= 7.3, p < 0.001; Moderate vs Minimal: β = 0.050, z = 8.2, p < 0.001). This effect was independent of IT (%ΔIT M1vs vertex Minimal: β = −0.035, z = −1.6, p = 0.109; Moderate: β = −0.031, z = −1.4, p = 0.151; Extensive β = −0.014, z = −0.7, p = 0.515; Linear contrast across skill levels: β = 0.015, z = 1.7, p = 0.088; Quadratic contrast: β = 0.005, z = 0.6, p = 0.547; Extensive vs Minimal: β = 0.021, z = 1.7, p = 0.088; Moderate vs Minimal: β = 0.004, z = 0.3, p = 0.727).

This effect of M1 TMS on movement time cannot be attributed to a speed–accuracy tradeoff. Accuracy rate was either unaffected (%Δacc M1vs vertex Minimal: β = −0.050, z = −1.6, p = 0.104) or significantly worse following M1 stimulation (Extensive: β = −0.090, z = −2.9, p = 0.004; Moderate: β = −0.115, z = 3.7, p < 0.001). We found some evidence that accuracy rate was more impaired for the higher levels of training, confirming that the deleterious impact of M1 stimulation is greater at higher levels of skill (%Δacc M1vs vertex Linear contrast across skill level: β = −0.028, z = −1.8, p = 0.074; Quadratic contrast: β = 0.037, z = 2.4, p = 0.018; Extensive vs Moderate: β = 0.025, z = 1.2, p = 0.250; Extensive vs Minimal: β = −0.040, z = −1.8, p = 0.074; Moderate vs Minimal: β = −0.065, z = −2.9, p = 0.003; Fig. 4B).

Our combined speed–accuracy skill score similarly reflects that the causal importance of M1 in supporting skilled performance grows as a function of the level of training. M1 stimulation negatively impacted both extensively trained and moderately trained skills to a greater extent than minimally trained skills (%Δskill score M1vs vertex Linear contrast across skill level: β = −0.016, z = −4.2, p < 0.001; Extensive vs Minimal: β = −0.022, z = −4.2, p < 0.001; Moderate vs Minimal: β = −0.028, z = −5.4, p < 0.001; Extensive vs Moderate: β = 0.006, z = 1.2, p = 0.243; Fig. 6, orange curve).

The results above suggest that disruption of M1 activity leads to greater costs in motor performance as an individual gains proficiency through training due to our within-subject design. However, as all participants become more skilled at a sequence as training progresses (e.g., they complete each sequence in a shorter amount of time), it is possible that it is the skill level attained rather than the extent of training that determines the impact of M1 and DLPFC disruption. For example, it is possible that the performance of someone who has trained relatively little, but is performing quite quickly and accurately would show a large cost following M1 stimulation. To disambiguate whether the effects of stimulation are due to the amount of training received or the overall level of skill attained, we performed two separate, but conceptually related analyses. First, for sequences at each level of training (Minimal, Moderate, Extensive), we examined whether there was a significant correlation between skill level attained (i.e., performance after control stimulation) and the cost of disruption across individuals for M1 and DLPFC stimulation separately. This analysis essentially investigates whether the best and worst participants within each level of training respond similarly to TMS. We did not find any significant evidence suggesting that an individual’s speed or accuracy was related to the cost of M1 stimulation irrespective of the amount of training received (all pcorrected > 0.05, rank correlation range: −0.30–0.04 for movement time, −0.44 – −0.01 for accuracy). In our second analysis addressing this issue, we included the rank of each individual in terms of their level of proficiency for each training level as a covariate in our linear mixed-effects models (see Materials and Methods for full details). Although the fastest participants at one level of training tended to be among the faster participants at the other levels of training (rank correlation = 0.39, p < 0.001), there was considerable variability in rank across the three levels of training (Cohen's Kappa = 0.41; moderate agreement between ranked level and training level; Cohen, 1968). Including this covariate did not substantially alter our results as all of the main effects and interactions of interest remained significant (%ΔMT M1vs vertex Extensive vs Minimal: β = 0.041, z = 5.2, p < 0.001; Moderate vs Minimal: β = 0.048, z = 7.3, p < 0.001; %Δacc M1vs vertex Extensive vs Minimal: β = −0.020, z = −0.7, p = 0.479; Moderate vs Minimal: β = −0.055, z = −2.3, p = 0.020; %Δskill score M1vs vertex Extensive vs Minimal: β = −0.017, z = −2.5, p = 0.011; Moderate vs Minimal: β = −0.026, z = −4.6, p < 0.001). In addition, our covariate (ranked level of performance) did not account for a significant amount of variability in our data (%ΔMT M1vs vertex ranked level: β = 0.006, z = 0.9, p = 0.385; %Δacc M1vs vertex ranked level: β = −0.026, z = −1.2, p = 0.235; %Δskill score M1vs vertex ranked level: β = −0.006, z = −1.1, p = 0.273).

It is possible that the observed impairment in motor execution resulted from disruption in the chunking structure of the learned motor sequences. As a follow-up analysis, we examined the change in chunking structures following stimulation. We did not observe any effect of M1 stimulation on the number of chunks for any skill level (all p > 0.05). We further examined whether the effect of stimulation is specific to chunk initiation or chunk execution. We found that chunk initiation was impacted only for higher skill levels (%ΔIKI M1vs vertex Minimal: β = 0.021, z = 1.0, p = 0.307; Moderate: β = 0.058, z = 2.9, p = 0.004; Extensive β = 0.054, z = 2.7, p = 0.008), whereas chunk execution were universally impacted (Minimal: β = 0.053, z = 2.6, p = 0.009; Moderate: β = 0.098, z = 4.9, p < 0.001; Extensive β = 0.088, z = 4.4, p < 0.001). Across all skill levels, chunk execution was disrupted to a greater extent than chunk initiation when M1 was stimulated (%ΔIKI execution vs initiation Minimal: β = 0.032, z = 3.3, p = 0.001; Moderate: β = 0.041, z = 5.1, p < 0.001; Extensive β = 0.034, z = 4.1, p < 0.001). These results suggested that both chunk initiation and chunk execution play a part in the overall impairment in motor execution.

We next examined whether the disruptive effects of stimulation varied as a function of skill level. Compared with control site, when M1 was disrupted, we found that higher levels of training exhibited both longer chunk initiation (%ΔIKI M1vs vertex Linear contrast across skill levels: β = 0.023, z = 3.5, p < 0.001; Quadratic contrast: β = −0.017, z = −2.7, p = 0.007; Extensive vs Minimal: β = 0.033, z = 3.5, p < 0.001; Moderate vs Minimal: β = 0.037, z = 4.0, p < 0.001) and chunk execution (Linear contrast across skill levels: β = 0.025, z = 4.1, p < 0.001; Quadratic contrast: β = −0.023, z = −4.0, p < 0.001; Extensive vs Minimal: β = 0.035, z = 4.1, p < 0.001; Moderate vs Minimal: β = 0.046, z = 5.3, p < 0.001). This pattern mirrored how the role of M1 varied as a function of skill level previously reported for movement time.

Effects of DLPFC disruption

Similar to M1, BOLD activity in DLPFC has been reported to decrease over the course of motor skill training. Unlike M1, however, this finding has usually been interpreted as a reduced role for the region in supporting expert motor skills. If true, we would expect that DLPFC stimulation would have a large impact on performance for minimally trained skills and a negligible impact on the most extensively trained skills.

We found that disruption of DLPFC produced behavioral performance deficits for all levels of training. The speed of execution was impaired relative to the control stimulation condition for all skill levels (%ΔMT DLPFC vs vertex Minimal: β = 0.093, z = 4.4, p < 0.001; Moderate: β = 0.103, z = 4.9, p < 0.001; Extensive: β = 0.091, z = 4.3, p < 0.001). Accuracy also suffered at all skill levels when this site was stimulated (%Δacc DLPFC vs vertex Minimal: β = −0.157, z = −5.0, p < 0.001; Moderate: β = −0.096, z = −3.1, p = 0.002; Extensive: β = 0.106, z = −3.4, p < 0.001). Initiation time was unaffected (%ΔIT DLPFC vs vertex Minimal: β = −0.035, z = −1.6, p = 0.117; Moderate: β = −0.030, z = −1.4, p = 0.158; Extensive β = −0.022, z = −1.0, p = 0.315).

We next queried whether this disruptive effect of DLPFC stimulation varied as a function of skill level. Stimulation had a larger negative impact on normalized MT for moderately trained sequences relative to the extensively trained sequences (%ΔMT DLPFC vs vertex Extensive vs Moderate: β = −0.012, z = −2.0, p = 0.046; Fig. 5A). We did not observe significant differences in the impact of stimulation when comparing minimally trained sequences to either of the other two levels of skill (%ΔMT DLPFC vs vertex Extensive vs Minimal: β = −0.002, z = −0.3, p = 0.744; Moderate vs Minimal: β = 0.010, z = 1.5, p = 0.123). Initiation time again did not show any effect of stimulation as a function of training (%ΔIT DLPFC vs vertex Linear contrast across skill levels: β = 0.009, z = 1.0, p = 0.314; Quadratic contrast: β = 0.002, z = 0.2, p = 0.845; Extensive vs Minimal: β = 0.013, z = 1.0, p = 0.314; Moderate vs Minimal: β = 0.004, z = 0.3, p = 0.731). However, when examining the disruptive effect of DLPFC stimulation on accuracy rate, we observed a significant linear trend across skill level (%Δacc DLPFC vs vertex β = 0.036, z = 2.2, p = 0.028; Fig. 5B). The impact of DLPFC stimulation was larger for minimally trained sequences than in the other, more highly trained sequences (Extensive vs Minimal: β = 0.050, z = 2.2, p = 0.028; Moderate vs Minimal: β = 0.060, z = 2.6, p = 0.008). We did not observe a difference in the impact of stimulation on accuracy between extensively trained and moderately trained sequences (Extensive vs Moderate: β = −0.010, z = −0.04, p = 0.664).

The effects of DLPFC stimulation on execution speed and accuracy rate produced a mixed pattern of results across skill level. However, this is the precise situation in which our composite speed–accuracy skill score can be illuminating. We observed a significant linear trend across skill level (%Δskill score DLPFC vs vertex Linear contrast across skill levels: β = 0.014, z = 3.6, p < 0.001), suggesting that the impact of DLPFC stimulation diminished as a function of training. Specifically, the deleterious effect of DLPFC stimulation on skill score was significantly larger for minimally trained skills when compared with the more extensively trained skills (Extensive vs Minimal: β = 0.019, z = 3.6, p < 0.001). The same was true for moderately trained skills relative to extensively trained skills (Extensive vs Moderate: β = 0.014, z = 2.6, p = 0.008; Fig. 6, blue curve). We did not find a significant difference in the impact of stimulation on skill score between moderately trained and minimally trained sequences (Moderate vs Minimal: β = 0.005, z = 0.9, p = 0.352). This pattern of results all point to the fact that DLPFC disruption had a graded impact on performance whereby the effect of stimulation was more muted for the most extensively trained skills. However, we found that DLPFC stimulation led to deficits in performance as measured by this composite skill score for all skills regardless of the level of training (Δskill score DLPFC vs vertex Minimal: β = −0.048, z = −3.8, p < 0.001; Moderate: β = −0.043, z = −3.4, p = 0.001; Extensive: β = −0.028, z = −2.3, p = 0.022).

We again conducted two separate follow-up analyses to disambiguate the effect of attained level of skill from the amount of training received. We did find evidence to suggest that TMS disruption varied as a function of attained skill within each level of training (all pcorrected > 0.05, rank correlation range: 0.06–0.24 for movement time, −0.04–0.09 for accuracy). Furthermore, including the covariate of ranked level of performance in the linear mixed-effects models did not substantially alter our main findings (%ΔMT DLPFC vs vertex Extensive vs Moderate: β = −0.015, z = −2.2, p = 0.027; Extensive vs Minimal: β = −0.007, z = −0.8, p = 0.408; Moderate vs Minimal: β = 0.008, z = 1.2, p = 0.235; %Δacc DLPFC vs vertex Extensive vs Minimal: β = 0.084, z = 2.9, p = 0.003; Moderate vs Minimal: β = 0.076, z = 3.1, p = 0.002; Δskill score DLPFC vs vertex Extensive vs Minimal: β = 0.016, z = 2.4, p = 0.018; Extensive vs Moderate: β = 0.013, z = 2.2, p = 0.030; Moderate vs Minimal: β = 0.004, z = 0.6, p = 0.525). This covariate did not consistently account for a substantial amount of variability in our data (%ΔMT DLPFC vs vertex ranked level: β = 0.006, z = 1.0, p = 0.342; %Δacc DLPFC vs vertex ranked level: β = −0.044, z = −2.0, p = 0.049; Δskill score DLPFC vs vertex ranked level: β = 0.004, z = 0.7, p = 0.511).

We also tested for the effect of DLPFC stimulation on motor chunking. We found that stimulation to DLPFC did not have an impact on the number of chunks for any skill level (all p > 0.05). Upon closer inspection of possible changes to the chunking structure, DLPFC stimulation was found to impact both chunk initiation (%ΔIKI DLPFC vs vertex Minimal: β = 0.042, z = 2.0, p = 0.041; Moderate: β = 0.030, z = 1.5, p = 0.132; Extensive β = 0.047, z = 2.3, p = 0.020) and chunk execution (%ΔIKI DLPFC vs vertex Minimal: β = 0.099, z = 4.8, p < 0.001; Moderate: β = 0.111, z = 5.6, p < 0.001; Extensive β = 0.096, z = 4.8, p < 0.001). Similar to M1 stimulation results, chunk execution was disrupted to a greater extent than chunk initiation regardless of skill level when DLPFC was stimulated (%ΔIKI execution vs vertex Minimal: β = 0.057, z = 5.7, p < 0.001; Moderate: β = 0.081, z = 9.9, p < 0.001; Extensive β = 0.049, z = 5.8, p < 0.001). These results suggested that both chunk initiation and chunk execution were implicated in the role of DLPFC in motor execution. However, the effect of stimulation did not consistently vary as a function of skill level for between chunk initiation (%ΔIKI DLPFC vs vertex Linear contrast across skill levels: β = 0.003, z = 0.5, p = 0.635; Quadratic contrast: β = 0.012, z = 1.8, p = 0.067) and chunk execution (Linear contrast across skill levels: β = −0.002, z = −0.3, p = 0.739; Quadratic contrast: β = −0.011, z = −2.0, p = 0.050).

The effects of DLPFC and M1 disruption on performance are dissociable

The above results establish that both DLPFC and M1 stimulation had a deleterious impact on skilled motor performance and that disruption of both sites showed a graded impact as a function of skill level. Are the effects of stimulation between the two sites dissociable as a function of skill level? We found strong evidence for just such a dissociation.

TMS site significantly modulated the effect of skill level on normalized MT (site × skill level interaction for %ΔMT Linear contrast: β = −0.033, z = −5.3, p < 0.001). We observed a similar significant interaction when examining the impact of stimulation on accuracy rate (site x skill level interaction for %Δacc Linear contrast: β = 0.064, z = 2.8, p = 0.005; Quadratic contrast: β = −0.066, z = −2.9, p = 0.004), suggesting that the dissociable effect of stimulation to DLPFC and M1 could not simply be attributed to a speed–accuracy tradeoff.

We then directly compared the effect of stimulation on our composite skill score across the two stimulation sites of interest. Overall, DLPFC stimulation had a larger negative impact on skill score than M1 stimulation did (Δskill score DLPFC vs M1 β = −0.008, z = −2.7, p = 0.007). However, we found a significant interaction between the effect of TMS site and training depth on the composite skill score (site × skill level interaction for Δskill score β = 0.029, z = 5.5, p < 0.001; Fig. 6). This interaction was driven by the fact that there was a much larger harmful impact of DLPFC stimulation on minimally trained skills relative to M1 stimulation (Δskill score DLPFC vs M1 Minimal: β = −0.033, z = −6.2, p < 0.001; Moderate: β = −0.000, z = −0.02, p = 0.985; Extensive: β = 0.008, z = 1.5, p = 0.132). The causal effect of DLPFC and M1 disruption on motor performance depends on the extent of training that an individual has with the executed sequence.

Discussion

We trained human participants on six separate motor sequences at three levels of training intensity (minimal, moderate, extensive) over a 6 week training period. Although BOLD activity in both DLPFC and M1 has been reported to decrease as motor skill develops, this has been assumed to reflect a reduced role for DLPFC in supporting performance but increased efficiency for M1. Here, we causally test this assumption by applying an inhibitory rTMS protocol over each site just prior to an assessment of performance at each skill level. Although cortical disruption at both sites produced deficits in performance for all motor skills relative to a control stimulation condition, we found dissociable effects on skilled performance as a function of skill level. Specifically, DLPFC stimulation led to the greatest cost for minimally trained skills and relatively smaller costs to performance for the moderately and extensively trained skills. M1 stimulation led to the opposite pattern of results whereby performance on the more extensively trained skills was impacted the most with relatively small costs for the minimally trained skills. Our control analyses confirmed that this pattern of results was not due to the raw level of performance across individuals, but rather was driven by the extent of training. These results confirm the assumptions of many models of motor skill acquisition but also point to a maintained role for DLPFC in supporting performance even for the most highly trained skills.

Over the 6 week training period, participants improved both the speed and accuracy of performance across all motor sequences as expected. We validated our within-participant experimental design where, at the end of the training, each individual demonstrated a performance gradient across the extensively, moderately, and minimally trained sequences. This design had the advantage of allowing us to make inferences about the neural mechanisms supporting each skill level within a single session without temporal confounds (e.g., general task familiarity) that might arise when using a single sequence examined at different time points during skill development. Although several studies have queried the role of both DLPFC and M1 in motor sequencing skills with noninvasive brain stimulation (Wilkinson et al., 2010; Greeley and Seidler, 2019; Greeley et al., 2020), to our knowledge, the present study is the first to examine the impact of stimulation following extensive training on a novel motor skill. Most prior work has either examined the effects of cortical disruption on skill acquisition in a single session (Pascual-Leone et al., 1996; Robertson et al., 2001; Wilkinson et al., 2010) or has interrogated how stimulation early on in training affects consolidation and/or retrieval after long delays (Reis et al., 2009; Galea et al., 2010; Cohen and Robertson, 2011; Greeley et al., 2020). Our results provide the first causal evidence in humans for the evolving roles of both M1 and DLPFC over the course of long-term skill development.

Our finding that DLPFC plays a causal role in motor sequencing performance on novice skills mirrors the results from several studies; however, most of these studies were concerned with initial skill acquisition rather than the expression of a well-learned skill. Several groups have found that repetitive TMS over DLPFC impairs performance and disrupts sequence learning in a single session (Robertson et al., 2001, 2001; Dayan et al., 2018). It is important to note that Robertson et al. (2001) found this was only true for spatially cued sequences but not when movements needed to be learned based on symbolic cues presented in a single location. Patients with lesions to prefrontal cortex also display sequence learning deficits on motor sequencing tasks (Beldarrain et al., 2002). More recently, Greeley et al., (2020) found that applying transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) over DLPFC similarly has detrimental impacts on sequence learning regardless of the polarity of stimulation. Participants in our study had initial exposure to the minimally trained sequences and showed improvement in performance over the course of training prior to the first stimulation session. Similar to the studies cited above, we likewise found that disruption of DLPFC led to impaired performance for these minimally trained skills. Several studies have suggested that DLPFC activity may actually be detrimental to the development and execution of trained motor skills (Galea et al., 2010; Cohen and Robertson, 2011; Ambrus et al., 2020). Noninvasive brain stimulation that disrupts DLPFC activity following a bout of training can lead to benefits in consolidation and improved performance on subsequent sessions (Galea et al., 2010; Cohen and Robertson, 2011). There is also neuroimaging evidence that individuals who improve the most during long-term motor sequence training are those who display a release of prefrontal cortex functional connectivity with sensorimotor areas (Bassett et al., 2015). Our results, however, suggest that DLPFC activity is causally necessary for successful performance, even for extensively trained skills. Given the purported role of DLPFC in supporting working memory operations in motor skills, one might expect the cost to performance for the extensively trained skills to be related to impairments in the maintenance of the sequence cue over the brief delay just before executing the sequence. However, we found no difference in the time it takes to initiate sequences between the stimulation conditions, suggesting that the cost to performance following DLPFC stimulation comes during skill execution.

The role of M1 in supporting skilled motor performance has come into question in recent years. Noninvasive brain stimulation studies suggest that disrupting M1 activity impairs initial skill acquisition and novice performance, but our findings indicate a relatively minor impact on minimally trained skills. However, recent work with rodents has suggested that while M1 may be necessary for the acquisition of a simple motor skill, it plays a minimal role in the execution of a skill once it has been trained extensively (Kawai et al., 2015). Along a similar vein, several neuroimaging studies have reported that M1 may not be involved in supporting motor sequencing skills beyond the simple execution of each individual motor element in a sequence (Kornysheva and Diedrichsen, 2015; Yokoi et al., 2018; Berlot et al., 2020). These studies employ representational similarity analyses (RSAs) and other multivariate analyses to attempt to identify brain areas that contain representations of trained motor sequences. These representations have been very difficult to read out from primary motor cortex and seem to reside in more anterior premotor areas in frontal cortex (e.g., SMA, pre-SMA, premotor cortex). This finding has been puzzling and has left it unclear whether M1 is only important for individual low-level motor commands, whether there is a limit to the inferences that can be drawn from multivariate fMRI analyses or perhaps whether the skill that is learned in motor sequencing tasks is more of a “cognitive” rather than a motor skill (Wong and Krakauer, 2019). If M1 does not contribute to the skill itself and only in the execution of each individual element, we would have expected that rTMS over M1 would lead to a relatively static cost to performance for all levels of skill. However, given that the cost of cortical disruption grows larger as motor skill proficiency develops, our results indicate that M1 plays an integral role in supporting extensively trained motor sequencing skills in line with prior work (Karni et al., 1998; Penhune and Doyon, 2002; Floyer-Lea, 2004).

Several models of skill development have suggested that the initial learning of a skill relies on more cognitive “higher-level” processing that becomes less necessary over time (Fitts and Posner, 1967; Willingham, 1998; Hikosaka et al., 2002; Doyon and Benali, 2005; Verwey et al., 2015). For example, the Cognitive Framework for Sequential Motor Behavior (C-SMB) that was developed with a particular eye toward the DSP task used here posits that motor sequencing skills require coordination between a central processor presumed to be supported by DLPFC and a motor processor involving M1 (Verwey et al., 2015, 2019). Motor skills are initially based on a central-symbolic mode that relies on cognitively demanding spatial and verbal representations supported by the central processor. After extensive practice, motor elements are chunked together into motor sequence representations that can be executed by a motor processor that needs minimal input from the central processor following action selection. Similarly, Hikosaka and colleagues proposed that early motor sequence learning requires transformation between spatial and motor coordinates that requires a frontoparietal associative network (2002). However, later stages of learning and skill expression are dominated by slow-learning sensorimotor circuits, as performance can rely on a motor sequencing mechanism without the need for a spatial-motor transformation. Our results are largely in line with these models as the detrimental impact of DLPFC stimulation diminishes as motor skill develops while the detrimental impact of M1 stimulation becomes more pronounced. The fact that DLPFC stimulation still carries performance costs even for the most highly trained skills is perhaps most consistent with the C-SMB, which suggests that chunked motor representations must still be selected and loaded by a central processor prior to execution. However, an alternate view would be that DLPFC stimulation affects well-trained performance only because the DSP task we employed has a high demand on visuospatial processing. As noted above, there is some evidence to suggest that DLPFC activity is more involved in motor sequencing tasks when movements are spatially cued relative to symbolically cued movements (Koch and Hoffmann, 2000; Robertson et al., 2001; Bo et al., 2011). In either case, the work presented here is consistent with recent suggestions from both rodent and neuroimaging studies that even well-trained movements may require flexibility in certain contexts, which necessitates a role for higher-level cortical circuits including DLPFC (Mizes et al., 2023; Yewbrey et al., 2023). Future work is necessary to disentangle whether DLPFC’s maintained role in supporting well-trained motor skills is specific to tasks with a visuospatial component or is especially important for tasks where flexible adaptation of well-learned skills is essential.

Prior studies examining motor sequence learning have shown that people often perform these sequences by grouping subsets of key presses into discrete chunks. This work has identified that the areas of the brain that seem to be important in chunking elements into discrete groups are the putamen, presupplementary motor area (pre-SMA), M1, and DLPFC (Kennerley et al., 2004; Wymbs and Grafton, 2015; Greeley et al., 2020). As training progresses and motor skill develops, people are able to “chunk” together different elements of the sequence such that more highly trained sequences have fewer chunks (and thus longer average chunk length). Here, we found that chunk number decreased while average chunk length increased as a function of training. However, we did not observe that either DLPFC or M1 stimulation had a significant effect on how sequences were chunked nor did we find that stimulation had a specific effect on chunk initiation or chunk execution. Instead, we found that M1 stimulation produced the same pattern of disruption in chunking as in total movement time, while DLPFC stimulation did not lead to a clear pattern across chunk initiation and chunk execution. However, the design of our experiment was not optimized to examine motor sequence chunking. Many studies examining chunking have individuals train on the same sequence for an extended period of time (Kennerley et al., 2004; Bo and Seidler, 2009; Acuna et al., 2014). In our study, the identity of each sequence was pseudorandomized across participants and across training level. This fact made it somewhat difficult to examine chunking across individuals and may have prevented us from observing any effects of stimulation. In addition, previous work has shown that the time it takes to initiate a motor sequence (e.g., the start of a chunk) is heavily influenced by an individual’s level of motivation (Wong et al., 2015). For example, the benefit of explicit knowledge on the speed of motor performance is driven at least in part by a change in motivational state. It is possible that the training effects on chunking behavior observed here may also be partly attributable to motivational differences across the training conditions. Further work is needed to establish the causal roles of M1 and DLPFC on motor sequence chunking as skill develops.

We observed clear effects of M1 and DLPFC stimulation separately on both the accuracy and the speed of performing motor sequencing skills. Given the well-known speed–accuracy tradeoff, we further opted to use a combined speed–accuracy metric to further characterize performance. This was especially useful to examine performance following DLPFC stimulation as we did not observe significant effects that converge across both dependent measures (MT and accuracy). Unfortunately, it is also well-known that there is no theoretically ideal way to combine a person’s accuracy rate with their reaction times short of empirically obtaining their speed–accuracy tradeoff function (SATF) with time deadlines or other experimental manipulations (Wickelgren, 1977; Lee et al., 2024). This fact has been especially appreciated by groups examining skilled motor behavior and obtaining empirically derived SATFs has been fruitful in characterizing an individual's level of skill across different experimental conditions (Reis et al., 2009; Shmuelof et al., 2012). Nevertheless, empirically derived SATFs require a large number of trials and was not feasible for the current study. Here, we combined speed and accuracy into a single metric by taking their average after first putting them on the same scale via Z-scoring. This approach is very similar to the “Balanced Integration Score (BIS)” for combining accuracy rate and response time. BIS is more well behaved in preserving performance differences across conditions than other metrics that have been proposed in the cognitive psychology literature (Liesefeld and Janczyk, 2019).

Our inferences about the effect of DLPFC and M1 stimulation on skilled motor performance rely in part on comparison with performance following stimulation of a control site (midline postcentral gyrus). We opted for an active control stimulation site rather than sham stimulation (i.e., no actual pulses delivered to cortex) as the former allows for stronger inferences to be made regarding the specificity of the effects of brain stimulation (Polanía et al., 2018). For example, an active control site allows researchers to control for the sensory stimulation at the scalp and the total amount of induced current in the brain, while sham stimulation does not. Although we did not observe any task-related activity at our control site for any of our participants during their baseline fMRI scan, we cannot completely rule out that stimulation on this day had an impact on performance. However, this fact does not preclude drawing inferences regarding the relative effects of DLPFC and M1 stimulation as disrupting either site carries performance costs that are dissociable from one another.

In summary, the present study provides causal evidence of the dissociable roles of DLPFC and M1 over the course of long-term motor skill development but shows that both regions are instrumental in supporting highly trained skills. Our findings support models of skill learning that posit separable associative and sensorimotor systems that display different time courses of involvement over the course of learning, but also suggest that both systems are required for performance at all levels of skill.

Footnotes

  • We thank James A. Brissenden and Joseph Deluisi for their helpful comments during the drafting of this manuscript.

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Taraz G. Lee at tarazlee{at}umich.edu.

SfN exclusive license.

References

  1. ↵
    1. Abrahamse EL,
    2. Ruitenberg MFL,
    3. de Kleine E,
    4. Verwey WB
    (2013) Control of automated behavior: insights from the discrete sequence production task. Front Hum Neurosci 7:82. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00082 pmid:23515430
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Acuna DE,
    2. Wymbs NF,
    3. Reynolds CA,
    4. Picard N,
    5. Turner RS,
    6. Strick PL,
    7. Grafton ST,
    8. Kording KP
    (2014) Multifaceted aspects of chunking enable robust algorithms. J Neurophysiol 112:1849–1856. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00028.2014 pmid:25080566
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Ambrus GG,
    2. Vékony T,
    3. Janacsek K,
    4. Trimborn ABC,
    5. Kovács G,
    6. Nemeth D
    (2020) When less is more: enhanced statistical learning of non-adjacent dependencies after disruption of bilateral DLPFC. J Mem Lang 114:104144. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2020.104144
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  4. ↵
    1. Anderson SP,
    2. Adkins TJ,
    3. Gary BS,
    4. Lee TG
    (2020) Rewards interact with explicit knowledge to enhance skilled motor performance. J Neurophysiol 123:2476–2490.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Bassett DS,
    2. Yang M,
    3. Wymbs NF,
    4. Grafton ST
    (2015) Learning-Induced autonomy of sensorimotor systems. Nat Neurosci 18:744–751. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3993 pmid:25849989
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Beldarrain MG,
    2. Gafman J,
    3. Ruiz De Velasco I,
    4. Pascual-Leone A,
    5. Garcia-Monco J
    (2002) Prefrontal lesions impair the implicit and explicit learning of sequences on visuomotor tasks. Exp Brain Res 142:529–538. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-001-0935-2
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Berlot E,
    2. Popp NJ,
    3. Diedrichsen J
    (2020) A critical re-evaluation of fMRI signatures of motor sequence learning. Elife 9:e55241. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.55241 pmid:32401193
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Bo J,
    2. Seidler RD
    (2009) Visuospatial working memory capacity predicts the organization of acquired explicit motor sequences. J Neurophysiol 101:3116–3125. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00006.2009 pmid:19357338
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Bo J,
    2. Peltier SJ,
    3. Noll DC,
    4. Seidler RD
    (2011) Symbolic representations in motor sequence learning. Neuroimage 54:417–426. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.08.019 pmid:20727412
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Cieslik EC,
    2. Zilles K,
    3. Caspers S,
    4. Roski C,
    5. Kellermann TS,
    6. Jakobs O,
    7. Langner R,
    8. Laird AR,
    9. Fox PT,
    10. Eickhoff SB
    (2013) Is there one DLPFC in cognitive action control? Evidence for heterogeneity from co-activation-based parcellation. Cereb Cortex 23:2677–2689. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhs256 pmid:22918987
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Cohen J
    (1968) Weighted kappa: nominal scale agreement provision for scaled disagreement or partial credit. Psychol Bull 70:213–220. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0026256
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Cohen DA,
    2. Robertson EM
    (2011) Preventing interference between different memory tasks. Nat Neurosci 14:953–955. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2840 pmid:21706019
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Dayan E,
    2. Cohen LG
    (2011) Neuroplasticity subserving motor skill learning. Neuron 72:443–454. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2011.10.008 pmid:22078504
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Dayan E,
    2. Herszage J,
    3. Laor-Maayany R,
    4. Sharon H,
    5. Censor N
    (2018) Neuromodulation of reinforced skill learning reveals the causal function of prefrontal cortex. Hum Brain Mapp 39:4724–4732. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24317 pmid:30043536
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Doyon J,
    2. Benali H
    (2005) Reorganization and plasticity in the adult brain during learning of motor skills. Curr Opin Neurobiol 15:161–167. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2005.03.004
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Fitts PM,
    2. Posner MI
    (1967) Human performance. Belmont, CA: Brooks/Cole.
  17. ↵
    1. Floyer-Lea A
    (2004) Changing brain networks for visuomotor control with increased movement automaticity. J Neurophysiol 92:2405–2412. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.01092.2003
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Fonov V,
    2. Evans AC,
    3. Botteron K,
    4. Almli CR,
    5. McKinstry RC,
    6. Collins DL
    (2011) Unbiased average age-appropriate atlases for pediatric studies. Neuroimage 54:313–327. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.033 pmid:20656036
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Galea JM,
    2. Albert NB,
    3. Ditye T,
    4. Miall RC
    (2010) Disruption of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex facilitates the consolidation of procedural skills. J Cogn Neurosci 22:1158–1164. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21259 pmid:19413472
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Greeley B,
    2. Barnhoorn JS,
    3. Verwey WB,
    4. Seidler RD
    (2020) Multi-session transcranial direct current stimulation over primary motor cortex facilitates sequence learning, chunking, and one year retention. Front Hum Neurosci 14:75. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2020.00075 pmid:32226370
    OpenUrlPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Greeley B,
    2. Seidler RD
    (2019) Differential effects of left and right prefrontal cortex anodal transcranial direct current stimulation during probabilistic sequence learning. J Neurophysiol 121:1906–1916. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00795.2018
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Hikosaka O,
    2. Nakamura K,
    3. Sakai K,
    4. Nakahara H
    (2002) Central mechanisms of motor skill learning. Curr Opin Neurobiol 12:217–222. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0959-4388(02)00307-0
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Huang Y-Z,
    2. Edwards MJ,
    3. Rounis E,
    4. Bhatia KP,
    5. Rothwell JC
    (2005) Theta burst stimulation of the human motor cortex. Neuron 45:201–206. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2004.12.033
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Karni A,
    2. Meyer G,
    3. Rey-Hipolito C,
    4. Jezzard P,
    5. Adams MM,
    6. Turner R,
    7. Ungerleider LG
    (1998) The acquisition of skilled motor performance: fast and slow experience-driven changes in primary motor cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 95:861–868. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.95.3.861 pmid:9448252
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  25. ↵
    1. Kawai R,
    2. Markman T,
    3. Poddar R,
    4. Ko R,
    5. Fantana AL,
    6. Dhawale AK,
    7. Kampff AR,
    8. Ölveczky BP
    (2015) Motor cortex is required for learning but not for executing a motor skill. Neuron 86:800–812. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.03.024 pmid:25892304
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Kennerley SW,
    2. Sakai K,
    3. Rushworth MFS
    (2004) Organization of action sequences and the role of the pre-SMA. J Neurophysiol 91:978–993. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00651.2003
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Koch I,
    2. Hoffmann J
    (2000) Patterns, chunks, and hierarchies in serial reaction-time tasks. Psychol Res Psychol Forsch 63:22–35. https://doi.org/10.1007/PL00008165
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  28. ↵
    1. Kornysheva K,
    2. Diedrichsen J
    (2015) Motor skill learning between selection and execution. Trends Cogn Sci (Regul Ed) 19:227–233. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.02.003 pmid:25746123
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    1. Lee TG,
    2. Sellers J,
    3. Jonides J,
    4. Zhang H
    (2024) The forced-response method: a new chronometric approach to measure conflict processing. Behav Res Methods 57:15. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-024-02516-y
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Liesefeld HR,
    2. Janczyk M
    (2019) Combining speed and accuracy to control for speed-accuracy trade-offs? Behav Res Methods 51:40–60. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-018-1076-x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Mizes KGC,
    2. Lindsey J,
    3. Escola GS,
    4. Ölveczky BP
    (2023) Dissociating the contributions of sensorimotor striatum to automatic and visually guided motor sequences. Nat Neurosci 26:1791–1804. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-023-01431-3 pmid:37667040
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. Newell KM
    (1991) Motor skill acquisition. Annu Rev Psychol 42:213–237. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ps.42.020191.001241
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Oberman L,
    2. Edwards D,
    3. Eldaief M,
    4. Pascual-Leone A
    (2011) Safety of theta burst transcranial magnetic stimulation: a systematic review of the literature. J Clin Neurophysiol 28:67–74. https://doi.org/10.1097/WNP.0b013e318205135f pmid:21221011
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Pascual-Leone A,
    2. Wassermann EM,
    3. Grafman J,
    4. Hallett EM
    (1996) The role of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in implicit procedural learning. Exp Brain Res 107:479–485. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00230427
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Penhune VB,
    2. Doyon J
    (2002) Dynamic cortical and subcortical networks in learning and delayed recall of timed motor sequences. J Neurosci 22:1397–1406. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-04-01397.2002 pmid:11850466
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  36. ↵
    1. Picard N,
    2. Matsuzaka Y,
    3. Strick PL
    (2013) Extended practice of a motor skill is associated with reduced metabolic activity in M1. Nat Neurosci 16:1340–1347.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Pitcher D,
    2. Parkin B,
    3. Walsh V
    (2021) Transcranial magnetic stimulation and the understanding of behavior. Annu Rev Psychol 72:97–121. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-081120-013144
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    1. Polanía R,
    2. Nitsche MA,
    3. Ruff CC
    (2018) Studying and modifying brain function with non-invasive brain stimulation. Nat Neurosci 21:174–187. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-017-0054-4
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Reis J,
    2. Schambra HM,
    3. Cohen LG,
    4. Buch ER,
    5. Fritsch B,
    6. Zarahn E,
    7. Celnik PA,
    8. Krakauer JW
    (2009) Noninvasive cortical stimulation enhances motor skill acquisition over multiple days through an effect on consolidation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106:1590–1595. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0805413106 pmid:19164589
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  40. ↵
    1. Robertson EM,
    2. Tormos JM,
    3. Maeda F,
    4. Pascual-Leone A
    (2001) The role of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during sequence learning is specific for spatial information. Cereb Cortex 11:628–635. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/11.7.628
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  41. ↵
    1. Rossi S, et al.
    (2020) Safety and recommendations for TMS use in healthy subjects and patient populations, with updates on training, ethical and regulatory issues: expert guidelines. Clin Neurophysiol 132:269–306. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2020.10.003 pmid:33243615
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. Seabold S,
    2. Perktold J
    (2010) Statsmodels: econometric and statistical modeling with Python. In: Proceedings of the 9th Python in Science Conference, 2010, Austin, TX, pp 92–96.
  43. ↵
    1. Shmuelof L,
    2. Krakauer JW,
    3. Mazzoni P
    (2012) How is a motor skill learned? Change and invariance at the levels of task success and trajectory control. J Neurophysiol 108:578–594. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00856.2011 pmid:22514286
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    1. Steele CJ,
    2. Penhune VB
    (2010) Specific increases within global decreases: a functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of five days of motor sequence learning. J Neurosci 30:8332–8341. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5569-09.2010 pmid:20554884
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  45. ↵
    1. Toni I,
    2. Krams M,
    3. Turner R,
    4. Passingham RE
    (1998) The time course of changes during motor sequence learning: a whole-brain fMRI study. Neuroimage 8:50–61. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.1998.0349
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    1. Ungerleider L
    (2002) Imaging brain plasticity during motor skill learning. Neurobiol Learn Mem 78:553–564. https://doi.org/10.1006/nlme.2002.4091
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    1. Verwey WB,
    2. Shea CH,
    3. Wright DL
    (2015) A cognitive framework for explaining serial processing and sequence execution strategies. Psychon Bull Rev 22:54–77. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0773-4
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    1. Verwey WB,
    2. Jouen A-L,
    3. Dominey PF,
    4. Ventre-Dominey J
    (2019) Explaining the neural activity distribution associated with discrete movement sequences: evidence for parallel functional systems. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 19:138–153. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-018-00651-6 pmid:30406305
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    1. Wickelgren WA
    (1977) Speed-accuracy tradeoff and information processing dynamics. Acta Psychol (Amst) 41:67–85. https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(77)90012-9
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  50. ↵
    1. Wiestler T,
    2. Diedrichsen J
    (2013) Skill learning strengthens cortical representations of motor sequences. Elife 2013:1–20. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.00801 pmid:23853714
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    1. Wilkinson L,
    2. Teo JT,
    3. Obeso I,
    4. Rothwell JC,
    5. Jahanshahi M
    (2010) The contribution of primary motor cortex is essential for probabilistic implicit sequence learning: evidence from theta burst magnetic stimulation. J Cogn Neurosci 22:427–436. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21208
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    1. Willingham DB
    (1998) A neuropsychological theory of motor skill learning. Psychol Rev 105:558–584. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.105.3.558
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    1. Wong AL,
    2. Krakauer JW
    (2019) Why are sequence representations in primary motor cortex so elusive ? Neuron 103:956–958. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2019.09.011
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    1. Wong AL,
    2. Lindquist MA,
    3. Haith AM,
    4. Krakauer JW
    (2015) Explicit knowledge enhances motor vigor and performance: motivation versus practice in sequence tasks. J Neurophysiol 114:219–232. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00218.2015 pmid:25904709
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    1. Wymbs NF,
    2. Grafton ST
    (2015) The human motor system supports sequence-specific representations over multiple training-dependent timescales. Cereb Cortex 25:4213–4225. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhu144 pmid:24969473
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  56. ↵
    1. Yewbrey R,
    2. Mantziara M,
    3. Kornysheva K
    (2023) Cortical patterns shift from sequence feature separation during planning to integration during motor execution. J Neurosci 43:1742–1756. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1628-22.2023 pmid:36725321
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  57. ↵
    1. Yokoi A,
    2. Diedrichsen J
    (2019) Neural organization of hierarchical motor sequence representations in the human neocortex. Neuron 103:1178–1190.e7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2019.06.017
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  58. ↵
    1. Yokoi A,
    2. Arbuckle SA,
    3. Diedrichsen J
    (2018) The role of human primary motor cortex in the production of skilled finger sequences. J Neurosci 38:1430–1442. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.2798-17.2017 pmid:29305534
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  59. ↵
    1. Yousry TA,
    2. Schmid UD,
    3. Alkadhi H,
    4. Schmidt D,
    5. Peraud A,
    6. Buettner A,
    7. Winkler P
    (1997) Localization of the motor hand area to a knob on the precentral gyrus. A new landmark. Brain 120:141–157. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/120.1.141
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 45 (20)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 45, Issue 20
14 May 2025
  • Table of Contents
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Masthead (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Dissociable Causal Roles of Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex and Primary Motor Cortex over the Course of Motor Skill Development
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
Dissociable Causal Roles of Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex and Primary Motor Cortex over the Course of Motor Skill Development
Quynh N. Nguyen, Katherine J. Michon, Michael Vesia, Taraz G. Lee
Journal of Neuroscience 14 May 2025, 45 (20) e2015232025; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2015-23.2025

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
Dissociable Causal Roles of Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex and Primary Motor Cortex over the Course of Motor Skill Development
Quynh N. Nguyen, Katherine J. Michon, Michael Vesia, Taraz G. Lee
Journal of Neuroscience 14 May 2025, 45 (20) e2015232025; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2015-23.2025
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Significance Statement
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
  • motor cortex
  • motor learning
  • motor skills
  • sequence learning
  • transcranial magnetic stimulation

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Articles

  • Change of spiny neuron structure in the basal ganglia song circuit and its regulation by miR-9 during song development
  • Increased neuronal expression of the early endosomal adaptor APPL1 replicates Alzheimer’s Disease-related endosomal and synaptic dysfunction with cholinergic neurodegeneration.
  • Presynaptic mu opioid receptors suppress the functional connectivity of ventral tegmental area dopaminergic neurons with aversion-related brain regions
Show more Research Articles

Behavioral/Cognitive

  • Change of spiny neuron structure in the basal ganglia song circuit and its regulation by miR-9 during song development
  • Increased neuronal expression of the early endosomal adaptor APPL1 replicates Alzheimer’s Disease-related endosomal and synaptic dysfunction with cholinergic neurodegeneration.
  • Presynaptic mu opioid receptors suppress the functional connectivity of ventral tegmental area dopaminergic neurons with aversion-related brain regions
Show more Behavioral/Cognitive
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Accessibility
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.