Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE

User menu

  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Neuroscience
  • Log out
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Neuroscience

Advanced Search

Submit a Manuscript
  • HOME
  • CONTENT
    • Early Release
    • Featured
    • Current Issue
    • Issue Archive
    • Collections
    • Podcast
  • ALERTS
  • FOR AUTHORS
    • Information for Authors
    • Fees
    • Journal Clubs
    • eLetters
    • Submit
    • Special Collections
  • EDITORIAL BOARD
    • Editorial Board
    • ECR Advisory Board
    • Journal Staff
  • ABOUT
    • Overview
    • Advertise
    • For the Media
    • Rights and Permissions
    • Privacy Policy
    • Feedback
    • Accessibility
  • SUBSCRIBE
PreviousNext
Research Articles, Behavioral/Cognitive

EEG Correlates of Active Removal from Working Memory

Jiangang Shan and Bradley R. Postle
Journal of Neuroscience 9 July 2025, 45 (28) e2414242025; https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2414-24.2025
Jiangang Shan
1Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin–Madison, Madison, Wisconsin 53706
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Jiangang Shan
Bradley R. Postle
1Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin–Madison, Madison, Wisconsin 53706
2Department of Psychiatry, University of Wisconsin–Madison, Madison, Wisconsin 53719
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • Peer Review
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

The removal of no-longer-relevant information from visual working memory (WM) is important for the functioning of WM, given its severe capacity limitation. Previously, with an “ABC-retrocuing” WM task, we have shown that removing information can be accomplished in different ways: by simply withdrawing attention from the newly irrelevant memory item (IMI; i.e., via “passive removal”) or by “actively” removing the IMI from WM (Shan and Postle, 2022). Here, to investigate the neural mechanisms behind active removal, we recorded electroencephalogram (EEG) signals from human subjects (both sexes) performing the ABC-retrocuing task. Specifically, we tested the hijacked adaptation model, which posits that active removal is accomplished by a top-down-triggered down-modulation of the gain of perceptual circuits, such that sensory channels tuned to the to-be-removed information become less sensitive. Behaviorally, analyses revealed that, relative to passive removal, active removal produced a decline in the familiarity landscape centered on the IMI. Neurally, we focused on two epochs of the task, corresponding to the triggering, and to the consequence, of active removal. With regard to triggering, we observed a stronger anterior-to-posterior traveling wave for active versus passive removal. With regard to the consequence(s) of removal, the response to a task-irrelevant “ping” was reduced for active removal, as assessed with ERP, suggesting that active removal led to decreased excitability in perceptual circuits centered on the IMI.

  • active removal
  • EEG
  • target confusability competition (TCC) model
  • traveling wave
  • working memory

Significance Statement

The removal of no-longer-relevant information from working memory is critical for the flexible control of behavior. However, to our knowledge, the only explicit accounts of this operation describe the simple withdrawal of attention from that information (i.e., “passive removal”). Here, with measurements of behavior and electroencephalography (EEG), we provide evidence for a specific mechanism for the active removal of information from WM—hijacked adaptation—via the top-down triggering of an adaptation-like downregulation of gain of the perceptual circuits tuned to the to-be-removed information. These results may have implications for disorders of mental health, including rumination, intrusion of negative thoughts, and hallucination.

Introduction

A hallmark property of working memory (WM) is that it is rapidly updatable, in that new information can be added “on the fly” as required by moment-to-moment changes in the environment and/or in behavioral goals. And because of the severe capacity limitation of WM, this updating process is commonly assumed to also entail the removal of no-longer-relevant information. Despite this, however, empirical evidence indicates that removal is often not absolute. For example, the recall (e.g., of the orientation of a grating) of the current trial's item is often biased toward the sample from the previous trial (i.e., an attractive serial bias; Fischer and Whitney, 2014; Samaha et al., 2019). On the neural level, there is also considerable evidence for the incomplete removal of the no-longer-relevant information, with information from the previous trial persisting into the current trial (Bae and Luck, 2019; Barbosa et al., 2020; Zhang and Lewis-Peacock, 2024). All these findings suggest that, under many circumstances, updating may involve a default strategy of “passive removal” (i.e., the simple withdrawal of attention from no-longer-relevant information), such that some residual trace of this information remains in the cognitive system, and can interfere with subsequent behaviors.

There is, however, evidence that updating can involve the active removal of information from WM. In a behavioral study using an “ABC-retrocuing” WM task, whereas passive removal resulted in an attractive serial bias (replicating; Fischer and Whitney, 2014; Bliss et al., 2017; Samaha et al., 2019), active removal had the opposite effect: a repulsive serial bias. Similarly, in a dual WM + discrimination task, the condition encouraging passive removal produced attractive serial dependence, but the condition encouraging active removal produced repulsive serial dependence (Teng et al., 2022). These reversals of the sign of the serial dependence effect suggest that active removal may entail the transformation of the IMI into a “reversed” version of itself, one that also reverses its influence on subsequent WM processing. This idea gains support from a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study of WM with a retrocue (Lorenc et al., 2020), in which the multivariate representation of a no-longer-relevant item was reversed after it acquired this status. Computational modeling indicated that this result was best explained by a mechanism in which the gain of perceptual circuits tuned to this item was down-modulated (Lorenc et al., 2020), a mechanism reminiscent of the one underlying sensory adaptation (Jin et al., 2005).

The repulsive bias of serial dependence is believed to arise during the encoding of the current trial's item, due to sensory adaptation (Fritsche et al., 2017, 2020; Pascucci et al., 2019; Sheehan and Serences, 2022). Together, these findings have led us to speculate that the repulsive effects associated with active removal may implicate an adaptation-like mechanism. We refer to this mechanism as the “hijacked adaption” model of active removal from WM.

The current study was carried out to test behavioral and neural predictions of the “hijacked adaptation” model, whereby the active removal of information in WM is accomplished by a top-down modulation of the perceptual channels tuned to that information. To do this we recorded EEG activity from subjects performing an ABC-retrocuing WM task and focused on two epochs in the trial to assess the triggering of the active removal process and, later in the trial, the consequence(s) of its deployment.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

Twenty-seven subjects from the University of Wisconsin–Madison community participated in the study and were compensated monetarily. All subjects provided informed consent approved by the University of Wisconsin–Madison Health Sciences Institutional Review Board. One was removed from analyses for failing to follow task instructions; another was removed due to poor performance (mean absolute recall error >2 SD higher than the group average). Thus, data from 25 subjects (19 females, 6 males; mean age, 23.8; range, 19–30) were included in all analyses.

Stimuli

Subjects were seated in a dimly lit room at a viewing distance of 50 cm from the monitor. A chin rest was used to help keep the head stable during the task. Memory-sample stimuli were oriented gratings (radius, 3°; spatial frequency, 1circle/°; contrast, 0.5; random phase) presented at six possible locations (30, 90, 150, 210, 270, 330°) on an imaginary circle with radius of 7°. Stimuli were white (RGB = 255, 255, 255) appearing on a gray (128, 128, 128) background. Ping stimuli were white bullseyes presented at all six locations (radius, 3°; spatial frequency, 1circle/°; contrast, 1; random phase). The ping was intended to provide strong but orientation-neutral stimulation of circuits involved in visual perception. Retrocues were white circles with the same radius as the samples, and the probes were black response dials (unfilled black circles with a black line corresponding to the diameter of the circle) with the same radius and random starting orientation. A white fixation dot was presented at the center of the screen throughout each block of trials.

Experimental design

Subjects completed six blocks of ABC-retrocuing task (Fig. 1) in one session, three blocks of “no-overlap” trials, followed by three blocks of “overlap” trials. Each block had 120 trials and lasted for ∼27 min. Subjects were asked to take a self-paced pause every 40 trials (i.e., ∼9 min) and were required to keep their head stable during the pause. Each trial started with central fixation (750 ms), after which two sample gratings (items A and B) were presented at different locations (1,000 ms). Subjects were to remember these two samples across an initial delay (Delay 1; 1,500 ms), after which a retrocue appeared at the location occupied by one of the two gratings (750 ms), thereby indicating that this item might be tested at the end of the trial. (Subjects were explicitly informed that the uncued item would not be tested, thereby making it an IMI.) After a second delay (Delay 2.1; 2,000 ms), the ping was presented for 250 ms, followed by Delay 2.2 (1,000 ms), then another sample grating (item C, 500 ms), Delay 3 (1,000 ms), and finally a probe that appeared at the location that had been occupied by the retrocued item or by item C (with equal chance). The probe was displayed for 3,000 s, during which the subject was to recall the orientation of the probed item by adjusting the orientation of the response dial with a computer mouse, followed by feedback displaying the number of degrees of error (1,000 ms). The intertrial interval varied randomly from 500 to 700 ms.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

The experimental procedure.

The orientations of items A, B, and C were randomly drawn, with replacement, from a pool of 6 base orientations (20, 50, 80, 110, 140, 170°), with a random jitter of −3 to 3° added. The locations of A and B were randomly selected from two of the six possible locations. Importantly, for the first three blocks (the no-overlap condition), item C was randomly presented at one of the four locations that had not been occupied by A or B, a fact that was specified during pre-experiment instructions. For the final three blocks (the overlap condition item), C was always presented at the same location as had been the IMI. The logic of this manipulation was that, because the overlap condition featured higher cue conflict between the IMI and item C, subjects would be motivated to actively remove the IMI from WM as soon as it was so designated by the retrocue. For the no-overlap condition, in contrast, because item C was always presented at a new location, the potential interference from IMI would be weaker, and so subjects might just use the default strategy of passive removal of IMI. Note that there was no explicit instruction about using active or passive removal. The location-related difference between the overlap and the no-overlap condition was only explained to subjects after they had completed the first three blocks of the experiment, a procedural detail intended to reduce the likelihood that subjects would engage an active removal strategy on no-overlap trials.

Behavioral analyses

Mean absolute recall error was calculated for each subject and each condition. A two-tailed paired t test was conducted to test whether performance differed across conditions.

We applied a variant of the target confusability competition (TCC) model to track the fate of the IMI (Schurgin et al., 2020). With TCC models, the subject's report on a trial is assumed to be based on an aggregated familiarity landscape that reflects the influence of all of the items currently in WM (see Fig. 2A for an example illustration). TCC estimates the memory strength of an item with a single parameter, d’, which represents the magnitude of the signal corresponding to that item. Whereas the original formulation of the TCC model was restricted to the items currently in WM, and d’ was bounded to be non-negative, for the present study we extended this model to include an estimate of d’ for the putatively removed item (cf. Zhang and Lewis-Peacock, 2023), and so we needed to modify it in order to allow d’ to take on negative values. In particular, for the ABC-retrocuing task, although passive removal of the IMI was predicted to leave a small but positive d’ (corresponding to an incompletely removed memory trace), active removal of the IMI was predicted to produce a negative d’. This is because, according to the hijacked adaptation model, the active removal of the IMI is accomplished by down-modulating the gain of sensory channels tuned to its orientation. Thus, the subsequent encoding of item C should be influenced such that the final familiarity landscape would be reduced for orientations close to the IMI (Fig. 2B). A negative IMI d’ in the overlap condition would be consistent with this model.

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Illustrations of predictions of the hijacked adaptation model. A, Hypothetical behavioral data from a single trial, fit to TCC model, in which the familiarity signals for item #1 (solid green line) and for item #2 (dashed green line), are strong and weak, respectively. The solid black curve is the resultant familiarity landscape that is accessible to the cognitive system, generated by taking the sum of two green curves and adding some noise (note the “shoulder” corresponding to the weaker item #2). B, Hypothetical behavioral data from a trial in which item #2 has been actively removed, the down-modulation of its gain resulting in negative familiarity centered on its orientation (note the consequent reduction in the corresponding portion of the familiarity landscape). C, On trials in which neither item has been actively removed, the gain of all orientation-tuned sensory channels is comparable (bottom plot), and so a nonspecific visual “ping” has the effect of activating each by the same amount (top plot; each bar illustrates the level of activity of an orientation-tuned sensory channel). D, On trials in which an item (90°) has been actively removed, the gain of sensory channels centered on that item's orientation has been reduced. Consequently, the response of these sensory channels to a nonspecific visual “ping” is lower.

The TCC model incorporates the psychological similarity between stimulus items and posits that a graded familiarity signal is generated by each item in WM (Schurgin et al., 2020). For orientation, a 0° memorandum would boost familiarity for all possible orientations ranging from −90 to 90°, with the magnitude of this boost for any one value determined by its similarity to 0° (Fig. 2A). To apply the TCC model, we used the psychometric similarity function for orientation estimated in a previous study (Cai et al., 2022). To assess whether active removal of the IMI influenced the processing of item C, we focused on the 50% of trials in which subjects were probed to recall item C. The familiarity landscape for each trial was modeled to be the sum of the familiarity of the probed item (i.e., item C, estimated with probed d’) and the signal corresponding to the IMI (estimated with IMI d’). Both probed d’ and IMI d’ were allowed to take a negative value. To calculate d’ values, we combined the data from all subjects into a “super subject” and conducted all TCC analyses on this super subject. The TCC model was fitted to data from the overlap condition and the no-overlap condition separately, using Markov chain Monte Carlo (MCMC function in MemToolbox; Suchow et al., 2013). A total of 15,000 postconvergence samples were taken to calculate the 95% confidence interval (95% CI) of the parameter estimates. To investigate the difference between conditions, the differences in the estimated parameters from the 15,000 samples in each condition were calculated and the 95% CI was generated from them. p values were also calculated from each pool of samples by calculating the proportion of samples higher or lower than 0 and multiplying the result by 2 to make it two-tailed.

Although we do not have an explicit prediction about the results of fitting TCC on report-A trials (because item A was encoded before the IMI was removed, and, indeed, before it was known which item was the IMI), we also conducted this analysis for completeness.

EEG recording and preprocessing

Scalp EEG was recorded from 60 electrodes with an actiCHamp Plus system (Brain Products), at a sampling rate of 1,000 Hz. The position of electrodes was based on the extended 10–20 international system. The recording was referenced online to a frontal electrode (FCz). Preprocessing and analysis were conducted with EEGLAB (Delorme and Makeig, 2004), FieldTrip (Oostenveld et al., 2011), and custom MATLAB scripts. Data were first downsampled to 500 Hz and then a bandpass filter of 1–100 Hz was applied. Bad channels were detected and removed using the pop_rejchan function of EEGLAB. Then, the data were rereferenced to the average of all remaining electrodes. Continuous EEG data were segmented into 13.25 s epochs (from 1.25 s before sample onset to the offset of the feedback), and epochs containing artifacts were detected and rejected using the pop_autorej function of EEGLAB. After reducing the dimensionality of data to 32 with principal component analysis, and applying an independent component analysis, components containing eye movement artifacts, muscle artifacts, and line noise were detected and removed using ICLabel (Pion-Tonachini et al., 2019). Finally, bad channels were interpolated.

Logic of EEG analyses

As a mechanism of top-down control, one would expect hijacked adaptation to have two discrete sets of neural correlates: one associated with the triggering of the control signal, tightly time locked to the prioritization cue, and one associated with the consequences of the deployment of this control signal, which would be revealed in ping-evoked activity. Thus, we focused on activities time locked to these two events for our EEG analyses.

In activity time locked to the onset of the retrocue, we planned to look for evidence of the top-down control signal that is hypothesized to implement hijacked adaptation. We hypothesized that one potential manifestation of this signal might be traveling waves, the spatial propagation of neural oscillations across the cortex that have been shown to support communication between brain regions and that have been proposed to subserve cognitive processing (Muller et al., 2018; Xu et al., 2023; Luo and Ester, 2024). In particular, forward (posterior-to-anterior) and backward (anterior-to-posterior) traveling waves have been shown to play an important role in bottom-up and top-down processing of information, respectively (Alamia and VanRullen, 2019; Mohan et al., 2024). Thus, analyses time locked to the retrocue were predicted to reveal a stronger backward traveling wave on overlap versus on no-overlap trials.

Regarding the consequence of active removal, the downregulated gain of sensory channels tuned to the to-be-removed orientation was predicted to reduce the overall excitability of the perceptual circuit responsible for its processing (see Fig. 2D for an illustration). To assess this predicted effect, we delivered a strong orientation-nonspecific visual ping during the delay period that followed the retrocue, so as to trigger a robust evoked response and thereby detect the change in excitability of the critical perceptual circuit. Specifically, we predicted that the ping-evoked response would be weaker following active removal of the IMI (Fig. 2D), relative to passive removal (Fig. 2C), because only the former would be accompanied by a gain reduction in orientation-tuned sensory channels corresponding to the actively removed item. For traveling waves time locked to the ping, we expected to see decreased forward traveling waves on overlap versus on no-overlap trials, corresponding to the decreased bottom-up processing of information with hijacked adaptation.

ERP analyses

We studied the ERPs evoked by the onset of the retrocue and the ERPs evoked by the ping. For all ERP analyses, the EEG was baseline corrected using the 750 ms before the start of the trial. To test the difference of retrocue-evoked ERPs across two conditions, we applied the cluster-based permutation test in FieldTrip. First, a paired t test was conducted and all data points with a p value lower than 0.05 were identified. Temporally and spatially adjacent significant data points were taken into the same cluster, and the summed t value for each cluster was calculated. Finally, the summed t values were compared with the largest summed t values generated from 1,000 permutated data with randomly permuted labels of conditions, and significant clusters (cluster forming p value threshold = 0.05) were detected with a two-tailed alpha of 0.05.

For the ping-evoked response, we focused on posterior sensors (P7, P8, P5, P6, PO7, PO8, PO3, PO4, POz, O1, O2, Oz), because the hijacked adaptation model predicts a change in the sensory circuits responsible for encoding stimulus orientation. To analyze the statistical significance, temporal cluster-based permutation tests were conducted with a cluster forming p value threshold of 0.05, 1,000 random permutations and a two-tailed alpha of 0.05.

After the results were initially analyzed, we carried out an additional analysis to rule out the potential concern that a difference between ERPs may have been driven by a practice effect, because in our design all three no-overlap blocks always proceeded the three overlap blocks. For this additional analysis, we analyzed retrocue-evoked and ping-evoked ERPs in each subject and in each block. To quantify the effects for retrocue-locked ERPs, we took the averaged voltage from the time window in which a significant across-condition difference had been found (i.e., 208–456 ms after retrocue onset; see Results). These averaged voltages were tested with a repeated-measures ANOVA, with block number as the factor. Post hoc tests between all possible pairs of blocks were also conducted. For the ping-evoked ERPs, in the addition to the analysis on the averaged voltage in the time window (220–356 ms after ping onset; see Results), we took the peak-to-peak distance of each block (defined as the difference in the negative and positive peaks in the ERP; see Results for details) and used repeated-measures ANOVAs to test for a main effect of block number.

Traveling wave analyses

Forward (posterior-to-anterior) and backward (anterior-to-posterior) traveling waves were assessed in EEG epochs time locked to retrocue onset and time locked to ping onset. Traveling waves were estimated with a procedure based on 2D fast Fourier transform (2D-FFT). We used four anterior-posterior axes linking frontal and occipital electrodes (Fig. 5A; [PO7, P5, CP5, C5, FC5, F5, AF3], [O1, PO3, P3, CP3, C3, FC3, F3], [O2, PO4, P4, CP4, C4, FC4, F4], and [PO8, P6, CP6, C6, FC6, F6, AF4]). For each axis of electrodes, a 500 ms time window was used and the voltage from electrodes was taken to construct a 7 (channels)-by-250 (timepoints) image. Then, a 2D-FFT was applied to this image. The upper and lower quadrants of the resulting spectra quantify the magnitude of the backward (anterior-to-posterior) and forward (posterior-to-anterior) traveling waves, respectively (Alamia et al., 2023; Luo and Ester, 2024), with the x-axis corresponding to the temporal frequency of the traveling waves and the y-axis corresponding to their propagating speed. Then, for each temporal frequency, the maximum value in each quadrant was extracted. This resulted in a vector of traveling wave power for this time window. Then, the time window was slid by 50 ms and the procedure repeated. As the result, a time-by-frequency matrix of traveling wave power was generated for each axis of electrodes and each direction (forward vs backward).

The power matrix was corrected with a baseline matrix. The baseline matrix was constructed by randomly shuffling electrodes and generating time-by-frequency matrices with the procedure described above, repeating this process 50 times and then averaging the results. Lastly, traveling wave power was calculated as a decibel ratio between the observed and baseline power:W=10*log10observedpowerbaselinepower. Forward and backward waves were estimated separately from epochs time locked to retrocue onset and time locked to ping onset. (Note that these analyses allow for forward and backward waves to be present along the same axis during the same temporal epoch.) For the comparison between conditions, traveling wave power from the four axes of electrodes was averaged and cluster-based permutation testing (with 1,000 random permutations, cluster forming p value threshold of 0.05, and two-tailed alpha of 0.05) was used to detect significant clusters.

Alpha lateralization

One possible concern about our task design was that because in the overlap condition, item C would always appear at the same location as the IMI, subjects might allocate spatial attention to the IMI location in anticipation of item C appearing at this location. For this reason, any difference found in the retrocue-locked or ping-locked EEG activity between the overlap and no-overlap conditions could be attributable to a difference in attentional allocation, rather than to the hypothesized active removal process. To address this concern, we directly assessed whether subjects allocated spatial attention to the IMI location, following the retrocue, by assessing alpha lateralization (Thut et al., 2006; Pietrelli et al., 2022). Specifically, we tested whether alpha band power contralateral to the IMI location was greater relative to ipsilateral alpha on overlap trials. To carry out this analysis, we applied a complex Morlet wavelet decomposition (1–50 Hz, 0.35 Hz frequencies bins, 3–10 cycles) to the cleaned EEG data using FieldTrip. The resulting power values were averaged across the alpha band (8–14 Hz). The power in the time window (−750 to −300 ms relative to A&B onset) was used as the baseline to correct for alpha band power in the time window of interest (500 ms before retrocue onset to the time of probe onset) and to convert it to the decibel (dB) scale. The averaged power over left (P7, PO7, O1) and right (P8, PO8, O2) parieto-occipital electrodes was taken to assess alpha power contralateral and ipsilateral to the location of IMI. To test whether there was a significant difference between contra- and ipsilateral alpha power, we conducted a cluster-based permutation test (cluster forming p value threshold = 0.05, number of permutations = 100,000, two-tailed alpha = 0.05) on the time courses of contra- and ipsilateral alpha power.

Statistical analyses

Details about the statistical test used for each analysis appear in preceding subsections of the Materials and Methods. In brief, a two-tailed paired t test was used to test whether there was a difference in behavioral performance across conditions; for the TCC model analysis, the 95% CI of the parameter estimates was calculated based on the Markov chain Monte Carlo postconvergence samples and the statistical significance of difference between conditions was also obtained from these samples; for ERP analyses, traveling wave analyses, and the alpha lateralization analysis, cluster-based permutation tests were run to detect significant differences; repeated-measures ANOVA was used to assess the block-wise effect of ERPs.

Results

Behavioral results

Mean absolute recall error indicated compliance with task instructions (M ± SD = 12.540 ± 3.069°), with recall error in the overlap condition (11.960 ± 3.117°) significantly lower than in the no-overlap condition (13.120 ± 3.195°; t(24) = 3.8452, Cohen’s d = 0.769, p < 0.001). Breaking out performance as a function of which item was probed, in report-A trials (15.440 ± 4.075° overall), performance in the overlap condition (15.199 ± 4.448°) was similar to performance in the no-overlap condition (15.681 ± 4.049°; t(24) = 0.9703, Cohen’s d = 0.194, p = 0.3416); for report-C trials (9.640 ± 2.443°), performance in the overlap condition (8.721 ± 2.248°) was significantly better than in the no-overlap condition (10.559 ± 2.789°; t(24) = 6.7580, Cohen’s d = 1.352, p < 0.001).

Applying the TCC model to trials in which subjects were probed to recall item C indicated that this item evoked a strong familiarity signal in both conditions (Fig. 3; no-overlap probed d’ = 2.532, 95% CI = [2.462, 2.604], p < 0.0001; overlap probed d’ = 2.864, 95% CI = [2.785, 2.946], p < 0.0001). The probed d’ was significantly higher in the overlap condition than in the no-overlap condition (Difference CI = [0.226, 0.448], p < 0.0001). In the no-overlap condition, the IMI d’ was small but significantly higher than zero (Fig. 3; no-overlap IMI d’ = 0.252, 95% CI = [0.159, 0.341], p < 0.0001), suggesting that a residual trace of IMI persisted at the end of the trial. In the overlap condition, IMI d’ was numerically negative (overlap IMI d’ = −0.096, 95% CI = [−0.208, 0.016], p = 0.0888), and significantly lower than the no-overlap IMI d’ (difference CI = [−0.4925, −0.2046], p < 0.0001). For reference, in report-A trials, there was not a significant difference in either probed d’ (overlap probed d’ = 1.7934, no-overlap probed d’ = 1.7462, difference CI = [−0.0267, 0.1235], p = 0.2136) or IMI d’ (overlap IMI d’ = 0.0131, no-overlap IMI d’ = −0.0587, difference CI = [−0.0191, 0.1645], p = 0.1111) across conditions.

Figure 3.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3.

Histogram of parameter estimates from the 15,000 pos-convergence samples with TCC model, of trials on which item C was probed. Note the difference, for item C (“probed d’”) versus for the IMI (“IMI d’”), in values on the horizontal axis.

EEG results

Retrocue-evoked ERPs

In both conditions, scalp topographies showed a large negativity at central midline electrodes. Comparison of ERPs averaged over these eight central midline electrodes (Fz, F1, F2, FC1, FC2, C1, Cz, C2) revealed that the magnitude of the ERP was larger in the overlap condition, the two conditions diverging beginning during the initial negative-going deflection following retrocue onset and this difference persisting for roughly 250 ms (Fig. 4A; 208–456 ms after retrocue onset, p = 0.002).

Figure 4.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4.

A, ERPs from frontal midline electrodes (see the insert plot), time locked to retrocue onset. Shaded area indicates SEM across subjects. Horizontal black bar shows timepoints with significant difference across conditions. B, The averaged voltage of ERP (in the time window with the significant across-condition difference) for each of the six blocks. Error bars indicate SEM.

To rule out the possibility that this across-condition difference was driven by a practice effect that gradually emerged across blocks—rather than a genuine difference between the two conditions—we analyzed the ERP broken out for each of the six blocks. The averaged voltage in the time window in which the across-condition difference was significant (i.e., 208–456 ms after retrocue onset) was calculated and tested with a repeated-measure ANOVA, with block number as the factor, which revealed a significant main effect (Fig. 4B; F = 17.931, p < 0.001, eta2 = 0.428, with Greenhouse–Geisser correction). Consequently, post hoc tests between all possible pairs of blocks were conducted, and among all pairs of adjacent blocks, only the difference between the 1st and 2nd blocks and the difference between the 3rd and 4th blocks were significant (1st vs 2nd: t = 3.463, Cohen’s d = 0.420, p = 0.016; 2nd vs 3rd: t = 1.114, Cohen’s d = 0.097, p = 1; 3rd vs 4th: t = 3.247, Cohen’s d = 0.237, p = 0.021; 4th vs 5th: t = 0.808, Cohen’s d = 0.062, p = 1; 5th vs 6th: t = −0.850, Cohen’s d = −0.074, p = 1; all p values Holm corrected). We suggest that the most plausible interpretation of these results is for an initial practice effect (block 1-to-2) and a step-like difference between conditions (block 3-to-4).

Traveling waves time locked to the retrocue

The forward traveling wave analyses revealed evidence for tonically elevated forward wave at low frequencies (<10 Hz), and a tonically suppressed forward wave in the beta band (20–26 Hz), in each condition, and for each spanning the duration of Delay 1, the retrocue, and Delay 2.1. In both conditions, there was a brief increase in the magnitude of the low-frequency forward wave that was centered on the offset of the retrocue (i.e., at time 3.25 s). The two conditions only differed for a brief period starting from ∼800 ms after retrocue onset, when the tonically suppressed forward traveling in the beta band dipped lower in the no-overlap condition relative to the overlap condition (Fig. 5D, left; p = 0.01). Backward traveling wave analyses revealed similar patterns in both conditions of persistently elevated magnitudes in two frequency bands: one spanning high-alpha/low-beta and one, less strong, at higher frequencies in the beta band, spanning from ∼20 to 25 Hz (Fig. 5B, right, C, right). (Note that we cannot rule out the possibility that the higher-frequency of these two backward traveling waves may be a harmonic artifact of the lower-frequency one.) In both conditions these backward traveling waves were prominent prior to retrocue onset. The power of these backward waves was stronger in the overlap condition both during the epoch surrounding the retrocue (in alpha/low-beta from ∼700 ms before retrocue onset until ∼2 s after retrocue onset; Fig. 5D, right; p = 0.002) and in higher beta beginning shortly after the retrocue onset (∼200–750 ms after retrocue onset; Fig. 5D, right; p = 0.03).

Figure 5.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5.

Forward and backward traveling waves in EEG, assessed from the average over four axes of electrodes (A), and time locked to the retrocue onset (dashed line at 2.5 s), in no-overlap (B) and overlap conditions (C). D, The difference between conditions with significant clusters marked with black contours. The left column is forward waves and the right column shows backward waves.

Ping-evoked ERPs

For both conditions the ping evoked a large response in posterior electrodes, with a larger negative-going amplitude for the no-overlap condition beginning with the first negative-going deflection and persisting for roughly 135 ms (Fig. 6A; 220–356 ms after ping onset, p = 0.028). Because the large positive-going deflection that was also in this time window had a numerically higher voltage for the overlap than the no-overlap condition, we carried out an additional analysis to assess whether the difference between conditions during this 135 ms time window is best characterized as a larger amplitude ping-evoked response for no-overlap trials or, alternatively, a transient DC shift between the two conditions. To do this we calculated, for each subject, the peak-to-peak distance between the initial negative-going deflection and the subsequent large-amplitude positive-going deflection, reasoning that if the difference were due entirely to a DC shift, the peak-to-peak distances for the two conditions would be the same. For this analysis, the negative peak was defined as the lowest voltage in a 200 ms time window centered on the group-average negative peak and the positive peak was defined as the highest voltage in a 200 ms time window centered on the group-average positive peak. Statistical comparison of peak-to-peak distance between conditions indicated that the difference approached, but did not achieve, the threshold for significance (t(24) = −1.9789, p = 0.0594). Although this result was equivocal, it approached the level at which one would say that this difference was due, at least in part, to a larger-amplitude ping-evoked response.

Figure 6.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 6.

A, Ping-evoked ERPs from posterior electrodes (see the insert plot). Shaded area indicates SEM across subjects. Horizontal black bar shows timepoints with a significant difference across conditions. B, The averaged voltage of ERP (in the time window with the significant across-condition difference) for each of the six blocks. Error bars indicate SEM. C, The peak-to-peak distance of ERP for each of the six blocks. Error bars indicate SEM.

To assess the possibility of a practice effect driving the ping-evoked ERP difference across conditions, we conducted block-wise analyses on the two ways of measuring this effect. First, for the averaged voltage within the time window where the across-condition difference was found (i.e., 220–356 ms after ping onset), repeated-measure ANOVA revealed a main effect of block number (Fig. 6B; F = 24.823, p < 0.001, eta2 = 0.508, with Greenhouse–Geisser correction). Consequently, we conducted post hoc tests between all possible pairs of blocks. Among all pairs of adjacent blocks, significant differences were found between the 1st and 2nd block and between the 2nd and 3rd block (1st vs 2nd: t = −6.457, Cohen’s d = −0.386, p < 0.001; 2nd vs 3rd: t = −3.277, Cohen’s d = −0.164, p = 0.029; 3rd vs 4th: t = −1.238, Cohen’s d = −0.069, p = 0.976; 4th vs 5th: t = 0.411, Cohen’s d = 0.018, p = 0.976; 5th vs 6th: t = −1.332, Cohen’s d = −0.068, p = 0.976; all p values Holm corrected). This is consistent with the alternative possibility that the between-condition effect reported above may be the byproduct of a practice effect, whereby the amplitude of the ping-evoked response initially increased in a block-to-block manner before plateauing for the remainder of the experiment. Next we performed the same block-wise analysis on the peak-to-peak distance measure. Unlike for the average-voltage measure, repeated-measures ANOVA on the peak-to-peak data did not show a significant main effect of block number (Fig. 6C; F = 2.437, p = 0.067, eta2 = 0.092, with Greenhouse–Geisser correction). Thus, for this alternative measure of the ping-evoked response, there was no evidence that the ERP difference between conditions was driven by a practice effect.

Traveling waves time locked to the ping

Forward traveling waves, when time locked to the onset of a visual stimulus, are believed to correspond to bottom-up processing of the perceptual input (Alamia and VanRullen, 2019). Consequently, the hijacked adaptation model predicted that the forward traveling wave triggered by the ping would be reduced during blocks that encouraged active removal. Consistent with this prediction, the visual ping evoked a strong forward traveling wave in the theta band (∼4–8 Hz), starting from ∼250 ms after ping onset in both conditions (Fig. 7A, left, B, left). This traveling wave was numerically weaker in the overlap condition relative to the no-overlap condition (Fig. 7C, left).

Figure 7.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 7.

Forward and backward traveling waves in EEG time locked to the ping onset in the no-overlap (A) and overlap conditions (B). Row C shows the difference between conditions. The left column shows forward waves and the right column backward waves. Note that the scale is different for forward and backward traveling waves. The significant difference clusters approximately or after 6.5 s in time were likely caused by the onset of item C, which was at 6.5 s. Plotting conventions are the same as Figure 5.

A tonically elevated backward traveling wave spanning high-alpha/low-beta frequencies (∼10–15 Hz) and extending in time from Delay 2.1 until well into Delay 3, with a pause briefly following the ping, was prominent in both conditions (Fig. 7A,B). This was likely a continuation of the backward traveling wave observed in the analyses time locked to the retrocue (Fig. 5B,C). The power of this backward waves was stronger in the overlap condition during the epoch surrounding the ping (in alpha/low-beta starting from ∼650 ms before ping onset until ∼750 ms after ping onset; Fig. 7C, right; p = 0.002). A significant difference was also observed in high beta (∼23–28 Hz) during the time corresponding to the onset of item C, which was at 6.5 s (Fig. 7C, right; p = 0.016).

Alpha lateralization

A possible concern about our design is that because in the overlap condition item C was always presented at the same location as the IMI, subjects may have preferentially allocated attention to this location. By this alternative, the difference of the neural responses (both ERP and traveling waves) between the two conditions may be a consequence of a difference in attentional allocation rather than of an active removal process. To assess this alternative possibility, we analyzed lateralized alpha (8–14 Hz) power from the time of retrocue onset to the time of probe onset in the overlap condition. Here, we used alpha lateralization relative to the IMI location to assess whether subjects were differentially allocating attention to the IMI location, post retrocue, in the overlap condition. This analysis revealed two time windows with significantly different contra- versus ipsilateral alpha power. First, shortly after retrocue onset, alpha power contralateral to the location of the IMI was higher than ipsilateral power (200–838 ms after retrocue onset, p = 0.0055), a finding that rules out the alternative possibility that subjects may have preferentially allocated spatial attention to the location of IMI during this period of time. The second epoch with significant lateralization was found after the onset of item C (210–1,012 ms after item C onset, p = 0.0065): Here, as expected, alpha power contralateral to the IMI location (i.e., the location of item C) was weaker than ipsilateral alpha, indicating that subjects attended the location of item C when it was presented (presumably while encoding item C into WM). Importantly, no differences were found after the ping and before the onset of item C. To summarize, the alpha lateralization analysis showed that the differences in neural responses across conditions that are the effects of primary interest were unlikely to have been driven by the preferential allocation of spatial attention to the location of IMI in the overlap condition.

Discussion

Although it is widely assumed that updating the contents of working memory can include the operation of actively removing no-longer-relevant contents (Jonides et al., 1997; Postle et al., 2001), the mechanisms that might accomplish this operation have received far less attention than other aspects of working memory. We approached this question with the assumption that there may be (at least) two qualitatively different ways whereby information leaves working memory: passively, as a consequence of the withdrawal of attention (Chatham and Badre, 2013; Barbosa et al., 2020; Tsubomi et al., 2024) or actively, via the application of cognitive control. [For alternative accounts, see Kim et al. (2020) and Beukers et al. (2021).] We operationalized these two scenarios with a single task in which we manipulated the level of cue conflict between a no-longer-relevant item and a newly added item, reasoning that the low-conflict (“no-overlap”) condition would encourage passive removal, whereas the high-conflict (“overlap”) condition would encourage active removal. In previous work, using this, and similar, designs, we have generated indirect evidence that the no-longer-relevant item is, indeed, processed differently in the two conditions. More specifically, the fact that, in the high-conflict condition, this item exerts a repulsive serial bias is consistent with the idea that active removal might be accomplished via a mechanism of hijacked adaption (Shan and Postle, 2022; Teng et al., 2022). The hijacked adaption model posits that the removal of information from WM is accomplished by a top-down control signal that down-modulates the gain of perceptual circuits tuned to that information. In the present study we have extended the previous work by reporting novel and direct behavioral evidence for the “tuned suppression” of the target item in an active-removal condition, together with EEG effects consistent with both the implementation of hijacked adaptation (at the time of the retrocue), and with a subsequent consequence of its implementation (later in the trial, at the time of the ping; but see below for a qualification about this latter effect).

The hijacked adaptation model posits that the active removal of an item in working memory (the irrelevant memory item; IMI) is accomplished by the down-modulation of gain in perceptual circuits tuned to that item. This particular mechanism has been posited, rather than some other form of inhibition, to account for the residual effects of active removal on the processing of subsequent items. For example, whereas the IMI exerts an attractive bias on recall on the subsequent trial when it has been passively removed, the sign of this serial dependence effect is reversed after active removal (Shan and Postle, 2022; Teng et al., 2022, 2025). Here we provide novel behavioral evidence for this account with a modification of a TCC analysis that allowed us to estimate the effect of active removal of the IMI during that same trial. TCC models have been previously used to study the memory strength of the memorandum (Schurgin et al., 2020) and the intrusion effect of a distractor (Zhang and Lewis-Peacock, 2024). Here we further extended the TCC model in order to study the influence of the IMI, by including the IMI in the model and by allowing d’ to take on negative values. This allowed us to make the novel observation that active removal of the IMI leads to a drop in the familiarity landscape centered on the orientation of the IMI, with d’ for the IMI taking on a numerically negative value. In the no-overlap condition, in contrast, the lMI left a bump in the familiarity landscape, indicating it was not fully removed from WM, and the positive value of d’ of “no-overlap” IMI was significantly different from its value in the overlap condition. This pattern in the no-overlap condition was in line with our expectation for passive removal and consistent with many previous findings (Monsell, 1978; Fischer and Whitney, 2014; Bae and Luck, 2019; Samaha et al., 2019). Thus, these results represent novel behavioral evidence for active versus passive removal from WM.

An interesting possibility is that actively removing IMI may reduce working memory load, thereby releasing working memory resources. By this account, these freed-up resources could be allocated to the other two items, thereby benefiting recall performance for both. Inconsistent with this possibility, however, is the fact that both the mean absolute recall error of item A and the d’ for item A (as estimated with the TCC model) remained similar across the two conditions, suggesting that the benefit of actively removing no-longer-relevant information was primarily reflected in the encoding of the subsequent item, rather than in the precision of items already in WM.

At the neural level, our design incorporated a visual ping to detect the predicted difference in perceptual circuits after subjects conducted active versus passive removal. Because the hijacked adaptation model predicts decreased gain in IMI-tuned perceptual channels, the overall response to the ping was expected to be reduced on overlap versus no-overlap trials. The analysis of the ping-evoked ERP supported this prediction: The ERP evoked by the ping was significantly weaker in the overlap condition compared with the no-overlap condition, with the divergence in voltages beginning ∼250 ms after ping onset, suggesting a reduction of perceptual-circuit excitability following active removal. Interpretation of this observation must be qualified, however, by our inability to rule out the possibility that at least part of this effect may have been a byproduct of a practice effect. That is, our design necessitated blocking trials by condition, and one of two control analyses found evidence that the average voltage within a post-ping time window increased over blocks during the first half of the experiment (Fig. 6B). [The peak-to-peak distance measure, in contrast, did not similarly change during the first half of the trial (Fig. 6C).] Complementing the ERP effect, a traveling wave analysis also revealed a numerical reduction of the forward traveling wave, in the theta band, beginning at a time similar to the ERP divergence (i.e., ∼250 ms after ping onset). Please note, however, that this observation should be qualified by the fact that this relative decrease of the forward traveling wave in the overlap condition did not survive cluster-based permutation testing. Forward traveling waves have been proposed to index the feedforward processing of visual inputs (Alamia and VanRullen, 2019; Mohan et al., 2024), possibly as a manifestation of the synchronization of neural oscillations between brain regions (Fries, 2015). Together, these findings provide novel evidence that a mechanism for the active removal of information from WM, presumed to be initiated once the retrocue designated the IMI, had the effect of suppressing the visual processing of a task-irrelevant ping presented 2 s later in the trial.

We acknowledge that a more direct evaluation of the hypothesized mechanism of hijacked adaptation would be via measurements of the neural representations of the information being removed from WM, such as via multivariate reconstruction methods like inverted encoding modeling (IEM; cf. Lorenc et al., 2020; Yu et al., 2020). However, our attempts at reconstruction of stimulus orientation were unsuccessful, even for items actively maintained in WM (data not shown). This was likely due to the fact that our stimuli were presented at locations in the periphery and because there were always two items in WM. [For a direct illustration of the effects on stimulus representation of hijacked adaptation, see Teng et al. (2025).]

Active removal via hijacked adaptation is assumed to be accomplished via top-down signaling that commands the down-modulation of gain in posterior perceptual circuits. In the ABC-retrocuing task, this control signal is assumed to be triggered by the retrocue, which designates (by implication) that trial's IMI. As with the ping, we carried out two sets of analyses time locked to the onset of the retrocue: ERP and traveling wave. For ERPs, we found that subjects showed a stronger negative-going deflection of the ERP at frontal-midline electrodes for the overlap condition relative to the no-overlap condition. For traveling waves, we observed widespread and prominent backward waves that were stronger in the overlap than the no-overlap condition. These backward traveling waves were strongest in a frequency band spanning alpha and low beta, started prior to retrocue onset, and persisted until 2 s after the retrocue onset. Thus, the backward waves found in the current study could be reflecting a top-down signal that exerts cognitive control over posterior brain areas (cf. Alamia and VanRullen, 2019; Alamia et al., 2023; Luo and Ester, 2024). At a general level, this aligns well with literatures showing the important role of alpha and beta band synchronization in feedback signaling (Bastos et al., 2015; Fries, 2015; Das and Menon, 2021, 2022). Although we hesitate to speculate too much about the specific function(s) of the pattern of backward traveling waves found in the current study, it may be that the stronger alpha/low beta band backward traveling waves in the overlap condition correspond to a preparatory control process that starts before retrocue onset and persists until removal of the IMI is completed. In a set of WM tasks, the magnitude of alpha backward traveling waves has been found to be modulated by the load of WM such that higher WM load was associated with weaker alpha backward waves (Zeng et al., 2024). The authors proposed that alpha backward waves may reflect top-down inhibitory gain control (Zeng et al., 2024), which aligns well with our results in the current study. Oscillations in the beta band, on the other hand, are believed to be important for implementing domain-general inhibitory control (Wessel and Anderson, 2024), including the inhibition of WM content (Lundqvist et al., 2024). Although our analyses also detected a significant difference, across conditions, in forward waves in retrocue-locked activity, it is unclear how to interpret this, because this effect was driven by greater suppression of forward traveling waves in the no-overlap condition (i.e., lower amplitude in comparison with baseline; see Materials and Methods).

To summarize, by using EEG recordings and TCC modeling of behavioral data, we have shown that active removal of information from WM is associated with a top-down control process manifesting at frontocentral electrodes. It results in reduced excitability at posterior electrodes (a hypothesized correlate of down-modulation of gain in posterior perceptual circuits) and behavioral evidence for a stimulus-specific reduction in the familiarity landscape from which the recognition decision is read out. Together, they add to growing evidence for hijacked adaptation as a mechanism for the active removal of information from WM.

Footnotes

  • This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health Grant MH131678 (to B.R.P.).

  • The authors declare no competing financial interests.

  • Correspondence should be addressed to Bradley R. Postle at postle{at}wisc.edu.

SfN exclusive license.

References

  1. ↵
    1. Alamia A,
    2. Terral L,
    3. D'Ambra MR,
    4. VanRullen R
    (2023) Distinct roles of forward and backward alpha-band waves in spatial visual attention. Elife 12:e85035. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.85035 pmid:36876909
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Alamia A,
    2. VanRullen R
    (2019) Alpha oscillations and traveling waves: signatures of predictive coding? PLoS Biol 17:e3000487. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000487 pmid:31581198
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Bae G-Y,
    2. Luck SJ
    (2019) Reactivation of previous experiences in a working memory task. Psychol Sci 30:587–595. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797619830398 pmid:30817224
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Barbosa J,
    2. Stein H,
    3. Martinez RL,
    4. Galan-Gadea A,
    5. Li S,
    6. Dalmau J,
    7. Adam KCS,
    8. Valls-Solé J,
    9. Constantinidis C,
    10. Compte A
    (2020) Interplay between persistent activity and activity-silent dynamics in the prefrontal cortex underlies serial biases in working memory. Nat Neurosci 23:1016–1024. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-020-0644-4 pmid:32572236
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Bastos AM,
    2. Vezoli J,
    3. Bosman CA,
    4. Schoffelen J-M,
    5. Oostenveld R,
    6. Dowdall JR,
    7. De Weerd P,
    8. Kennedy H,
    9. Fries P
    (2015) Visual areas exert feedforward and feedback influences through distinct frequency channels. Neuron 85:390–401. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2014.12.018
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Beukers AO,
    2. Buschman TJ,
    3. Cohen JD,
    4. Norman KA
    (2021) Is activity silent working memory simply episodic memory? Trends Cogn Sci 25:284–293. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2021.01.003
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Bliss DP,
    2. Sun JJ,
    3. D’Esposito M
    (2017) Serial dependence is absent at the time of perception but increases in visual working memory. Sci Rep 7:14739. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-15199-7 pmid:29116132
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Cai Y,
    2. Fulvio JM,
    3. Samaha J,
    4. Postle BR
    (2022) Context binding in visual working memory is reflected in bilateral event-related potentials, but not in contralateral delay activity. eNeuro 9:ENEURO.0207-0222.2022. https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0207-22.20225199-7
    OpenUrl
  9. ↵
    1. Chatham CH,
    2. Badre D
    (2013) Working memory management and predicted utility. Front Behav Neurosci 7:83. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00083 pmid:23882196
    OpenUrlPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Das A,
    2. Menon V
    (2021) Asymmetric frequency-specific feedforward and feedback information flow between hippocampus and prefrontal cortex during verbal memory encoding and recall. J Neurosci 41:8427. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0802-21.2021 pmid:34433632
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  11. ↵
    1. Das A,
    2. Menon V
    (2022) Replicable patterns of causal information flow between hippocampus and prefrontal cortex during spatial navigation and spatial–verbal memory formation. Cereb Cortex 32:5343–5361. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhac018 pmid:35136979
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Delorme A,
    2. Makeig S
    (2004) EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J Neurosci Methods 134:9–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Fischer J,
    2. Whitney D
    (2014) Serial dependence in visual perception. Nat Neurosci 17:738–743. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3689 pmid:24686785
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Fries P
    (2015) Rhythms for cognition: communication through coherence. Neuron 88:220–235. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.034 pmid:26447583
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Fritsche M,
    2. Mostert P,
    3. de Lange FP
    (2017) Opposite effects of recent history on perception and decision. Curr Biol 27:590–595. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2017.01.006
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Fritsche M,
    2. Spaak E,
    3. de Lange FP
    (2020) A Bayesian and efficient observer model explains concurrent attractive and repulsive history biases in visual perception. Elife 9:e55389. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.55389 pmid:32479264
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Jin DZ,
    2. Dragoi V,
    3. Sur M,
    4. Seung HS
    (2005) Tilt aftereffect and adaptation-induced changes in orientation tuning in visual cortex. J Neurophysiol 94:4038–4050. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00571.2004
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Jonides J,
    2. Schumacher EH,
    3. Smith EE,
    4. Lauber EJ,
    5. Awh E,
    6. Minoshima S,
    7. Koeppe RA
    (1997) Verbal working memory load affects regional brain activation as measured by PET. J Cogn Neurosci 9:462–475. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1997.9.4.462
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Kim H,
    2. Smolker HR,
    3. Smith LL,
    4. Banich MT,
    5. Lewis-Peacock JA
    (2020) Changes to information in working memory depend on distinct removal operations. Nat Commun 11:6239. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-20085-4 pmid:33288756
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Lorenc ES,
    2. Vandenbroucke ARE,
    3. Nee DE,
    4. de Lange FP,
    5. D’Esposito M
    (2020) Dissociable neural mechanisms underlie currently-relevant, future-relevant, and discarded working memory representations. Sci Rep 10:11195. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-67634-x pmid:32641712
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Lundqvist M,
    2. Miller EK,
    3. Nordmark J,
    4. Liljefors J,
    5. Herman P
    (2024) Beta: bursts of cognition. Trends Cogn Sci 28:662–676. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2024.03.010
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Luo C,
    2. Ester EF
    (2024) Traveling waves link human visual and frontal cortex during working memory-guided behavior. bioRxiv:2024.2003.2012.584543.
  23. ↵
    1. Mohan UR,
    2. Zhang H,
    3. Ermentrout B,
    4. Jacobs J
    (2024) The direction of theta and alpha travelling waves modulates human memory processing. Nat Hum Behav 8:1124–1135. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-024-01838-3
    OpenUrlPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Monsell S
    (1978) Recency, immediate recognition memory, and reaction time. Cogn Psychol 10:465–501. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(78)90008-7
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  25. ↵
    1. Muller L,
    2. Chavane F,
    3. Reynolds J,
    4. Sejnowski TJ
    (2018) Cortical travelling waves: mechanisms and computational principles. Nat Rev Neurosci 19:255–268. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2018.20 pmid:29563572
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Oostenveld R,
    2. Fries P,
    3. Maris E,
    4. Schoffelen J-M
    (2011) FieldTrip: open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Comput Intell Neurosci 2011:156869. https://doi.org/10.1155/2011/156869 pmid:21253357
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Pascucci D,
    2. Mancuso G,
    3. Santandrea E,
    4. Della Libera C,
    5. Plomp G,
    6. Chelazzi L
    (2019) Laws of concatenated perception: vision goes for novelty, decisions for perseverance. PLoS Biol 17:e3000144. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000144 pmid:30835720
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    1. Pietrelli M,
    2. Samaha J,
    3. Postle BR
    (2022) Spectral distribution dynamics across different attentional priority states. J Neurosci 42:4026. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2318-21.2022 pmid:35387871
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  29. ↵
    1. Pion-Tonachini L,
    2. Kreutz-Delgado K,
    3. Makeig S
    (2019) ICLabel: an automated electroencephalographic independent component classifier, dataset, and website. Neuroimage 198:181–197. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.05.026 pmid:31103785
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Postle BR,
    2. Berger JS,
    3. Goldstein JH,
    4. Curtis CE,
    5. D’Esposito M
    (2001) Behavioral and neurophysiological correlates of episodic coding, proactive interference, and list length effects in a running span verbal working memory task. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 1:10–21. https://doi.org/10.3758/CABN.1.1.10
    OpenUrlPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Samaha J,
    2. Switzky M,
    3. Postle BR
    (2019) Confidence boosts serial dependence in orientation estimation. J Vis 19:25. https://doi.org/10.1167/19.4.25 pmid:31009526
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. Schurgin MW,
    2. Wixted JT,
    3. Brady TF
    (2020) Psychophysical scaling reveals a unified theory of visual memory strength. Nat Hum Behav 4:1156–1172. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-00938-0
    OpenUrlPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Shan J,
    2. Postle BR
    (2022) The influence of active removal from working memory on serial dependence. J Cogn 5:31. https://doi.org/10.5334/joc.222 pmid:36072093
    OpenUrlPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Sheehan TC,
    2. Serences JT
    (2022) Attractive serial dependence overcomes repulsive neuronal adaptation. PLoS Biol 20:e3001711. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3001711 pmid:36067148
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Suchow JW,
    2. Brady TF,
    3. Fougnie D,
    4. Alvarez GA
    (2013) Modeling visual working memory with the MemToolbox. J Vis 13:9. https://doi.org/10.1167/13.10.9 pmid:23962734
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  36. ↵
    1. Teng C,
    2. Fulvio JM,
    3. Jiang J,
    4. Postle BR
    (2022) Flexible top-down control in the interaction between working memory and perception. J Vis 22:3. https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.22.11.3 pmid:36205937
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Teng C,
    2. Fulvio JM,
    3. Pietrelli M,
    4. Jiang J,
    5. Postle BR
    (2025) Temporal dynamics and representational consequences of the control of processing conflict between visual working memory and visual perception. J Cogn Neurosci 1–20. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_02310 pmid:39918918
    OpenUrlPubMed
  38. ↵
    1. Thut G,
    2. Nietzel A,
    3. Brandt SA,
    4. Pascual-Leone A
    (2006) Alpha-band electroencephalographic activity over occipital cortex indexes visuospatial attention bias and predicts visual target detection. J Neurosci 26:9494–9502. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0875-06.2006 pmid:16971533
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  39. ↵
    1. Tsubomi H,
    2. Fukuda K,
    3. Kikumoto A,
    4. Mayr U,
    5. Vogel EK
    (2024) Task termination triggers spontaneous removal of information from visual working memory. Psychol Sci 35:995–1009. https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976241246709
    OpenUrlPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Wessel JR,
    2. Anderson MC
    (2024) Neural mechanisms of domain-general inhibitory control. Trends Cogn Sci 28:124–143. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2023.09.008
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  41. ↵
    1. Xu Y,
    2. Long X,
    3. Feng J,
    4. Gong P
    (2023) Interacting spiral wave patterns underlie complex brain dynamics and are related to cognitive processing. Nat Hum Behav 7:1196–1215. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-023-01626-5
    OpenUrlPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. Yu Q,
    2. Teng C,
    3. Postle BR
    (2020) Different states of priority recruit different neural representations in visual working memory. PLoS Biol 18:e3000769. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3000769 pmid:32598358
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Zeng Y,
    2. Sauseng P,
    3. Alamia A
    (2024) Alpha traveling waves during working memory: disentangling bottom-up gating and top-down gain control. J Neurosci 44:e0532242024. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0532-24.2024 pmid:39505407
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  44. ↵
    1. Zhang Z,
    2. Lewis-Peacock J
    (2023) Prioritization sharpens working memories but does not protect them from distraction. J Exp Psychol Gen 152:1158–1174. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001309 pmid:36395057
    OpenUrlPubMed
  45. ↵
    1. Zhang Z,
    2. Lewis-Peacock JA
    (2024) Integration of history information drives serial dependence and stabilizes working memory representations. J Neurosci 44:e2399232024. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2399-23.2024 pmid:38897722
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
Back to top

In this issue

The Journal of Neuroscience: 45 (28)
Journal of Neuroscience
Vol. 45, Issue 28
9 Jul 2025
  • Table of Contents
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Masthead (PDF)
Email

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Neuroscience article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
EEG Correlates of Active Removal from Working Memory
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Neuroscience
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Neuroscience.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
View Full Page PDF
Citation Tools
EEG Correlates of Active Removal from Working Memory
Jiangang Shan, Bradley R. Postle
Journal of Neuroscience 9 July 2025, 45 (28) e2414242025; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2414-24.2025

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Respond to this article
Request Permissions
Share
EEG Correlates of Active Removal from Working Memory
Jiangang Shan, Bradley R. Postle
Journal of Neuroscience 9 July 2025, 45 (28) e2414242025; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2414-24.2025
Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Significance Statement
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • Peer Review
  • PDF

Keywords

  • active removal
  • EEG
  • target confusability competition (TCC) model
  • traveling wave
  • working memory

Responses to this article

Respond to this article

Jump to comment:

No eLetters have been published for this article.

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

Research Articles

  • CaMKIIβ-mediated phosphorylation enhances protein stability of spastin to promote neurite outgrowth
  • Vocal error monitoring in the primate auditory cortex
Show more Research Articles

Behavioral/Cognitive

  • Neural Distinction between Visual Word and Object Recognition: An fMRI Study Using Pictographs
  • Attentional Precursors of Errors Predict Error-Related Brain Activity
  • Directed Neural Network Dynamics in Sensorimotor Integration: Divergent Roles of Frontal Theta Band Activity Depending on Age
Show more Behavioral/Cognitive
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Follow SFN on BlueSky
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Facebook
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on Twitter
  • Follow Society for Neuroscience on LinkedIn
  • Visit Society for Neuroscience on Youtube
  • Follow our RSS feeds

Content

  • Early Release
  • Current Issue
  • Issue Archive
  • Collections

Information

  • For Authors
  • For Advertisers
  • For the Media
  • For Subscribers

About

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Privacy Notice
  • Contact
  • Accessibility
(JNeurosci logo)
(SfN logo)

Copyright © 2025 by the Society for Neuroscience.
JNeurosci Online ISSN: 1529-2401

The ideas and opinions expressed in JNeurosci do not necessarily reflect those of SfN or the JNeurosci Editorial Board. Publication of an advertisement or other product mention in JNeurosci should not be construed as an endorsement of the manufacturer’s claims. SfN does not assume any responsibility for any injury and/or damage to persons or property arising from or related to any use of any material contained in JNeurosci.