Abstract
Attention is key to perception and human behavior, and evidence shows that it periodically samples sensory information (<20 Hz). However, this view has been recently challenged due to methodological concerns and gaps in our understanding of the function and mechanism of rhythmic attention. Here we used an intensive ∼22 h psychophysical protocol combined with reverse correlation analyses to infer the neural representation underlying these rhythms. Participants (male/female) performed a task in which covert spatial (sustained and exploratory) attention was manipulated and then probed at various delays. Our results show that sustained and exploratory attention periodically modulate perception via different neural computations. While sustained attention suppresses distracting stimulus features at the alpha (∼12 Hz) frequency, exploratory attention increases the gain around task-relevant stimulus feature at the theta (∼6 Hz) frequency. These findings reveal that both modes of rhythmic attention differentially shape sensory tuning, expanding the current understanding of the rhythmic sampling theory of attention.
Significance Statement
For the past decade, low-frequency rhythms have been observed in attentional performance. Here, we go beyond description and assess the underlying neural computations in the sensory system. We used an intensive psychophysical protocol combined with reverse correlation analysis to infer the system's sensitivity to and selectivity for stimulus feature (orientation) across time and for two attention modes, i.e., sustained and exploratory attention. Our results reveal that sustained and exploratory attention modes differentially shape the sensory tuning to stimulus features: respectively altering either noise suppression or signal enhancement rhythmically, leading to alternating periods of performance enhancement and decrement.
Introduction
Attention selectively facilitates sensory information processing, efficiently allocating resources based on task demands (Lennie, 2003; Carrasco, 2011). When voluntarily deployed, attention's influence on perception can be sustained at will, benefiting performance for long periods of time. Although this classic understanding of sustained attention is well established, recent evidence suggests that attention periodically samples information, across both time and spatial locations (VanRullen, 2016; Fiebelkorn and Kastner, 2019; Kienitz et al., 2022).
Periodicity in behavioral performance has been assessed with paradigms including a sensory stimulus presented to reset participants’ attention at a specified time and spatial location (button press, saccade, arm movement are also used; Sauseng et al., 2007; Harris, 2023), followed by a probe stimulus assessing changes in the spatiotemporal dynamics of the attention focus at various delays after the attentional reset. The temporal probing needs to be dense enough and across a long enough window for appropriate frequency exploration (Kienitz et al., 2022). Behavioral sampling studies have shown rhythmic fluctuations in performance at low frequency (<20 Hz) due to attention (also observed for, e.g., expectation and serial dependence; Ho et al., 2022; Keitel et al., 2022), suggesting that sensory information processing alternates between periods of greater (when in the attention focus) and lesser (when outside) facilitation (Landau and Fries, 2012; Fiebelkorn et al., 2013; Dugué and VanRullen, 2014; Song et al., 2014; Dugué et al., 2015b; Landau et al., 2015; Ho et al., 2017; Senoussi et al., 2019; Michel et al., 2022). Specifically, when attention is cued to a target location (valid cue)—sustained at one location preceding the probe—performance in the probe task fluctuates in alpha (∼12 Hz). When attention is cued to a distractor stimulus location (invalid)—attention must shift and explore an uncued location—performance in the probe task fluctuates in theta (∼6 Hz; Dugué and VanRullen, 2017; Kienitz et al., 2022).
The view that attention modulates performance rhythmically has, however, been recently challenged (Ruzzoli et al., 2019; Keitel et al., 2022; van der Werf et al., 2022) notably due to methodological considerations [low trial number (van der Werf et al., 2022), low sampling frequency resolution (Kienitz et al., 2022), and issues regarding analyses (Brookshire, 2022); but see Fiebelkorn, 2022a,b; Re et al., 2022; Harris and Beale, 2024]. Additionally, while distinct frequency profiles of sustained and exploratory attention are consistent with distinct mechanisms and functional roles, the specific neural computations underlying variations in behavioral performance are still poorly understood. Specifically, it is unclear whether periodic changes in behavioral performance due to either attention mode are a result of fluctuations in signal enhancement or in noise suppression, two distinct neural computations by which attention shapes performance and sensory representations (Dosher and Lu, 2000; Lu and Dosher, 2000).
We used an intensive protocol for measuring periodic fluctuations of attentional performance in individual participants and assessed how sensory tuning fluctuates with either exploratory or sustained attention. Following a precue manipulating voluntary attention, a first discrimination task ensures exploratory and sustained attention were successfully manipulated. At various delays after the first task, a second detection task (vertical Gabor within noise) interrogates the dynamics of attentional stimulus sampling (Dugué et al., 2015b, 2017; Senoussi et al., 2019; Michel et al., 2022). Critically, reverse correlation analysis (Ahumada, 2002; Eckstein et al., 2002; Paltoglou and Neri, 2012; Wyart et al., 2012; Li et al., 2016; Tu et al., 2023; Xue et al., 2024) assessed potential fluctuations in sensory representations during the probe period. By regressing the stimulus energy across trials with behavioral responses in the detection task, we extracted orientation tuning curves of participants’ decision weights and evaluated signal enhancement and noise suppression over time for each attention mode. The results replicate previous findings of distinct rhythmic fluctuations of attentional performance for different attention modes (alpha for sustained and theta exploratory) and crucially reveal distinct sensory tuning mechanisms for either attention mode.
Materials and Methods
Participants
Nineteen participants were recruited for this experiment (11 female, age M ± SD = 23.26 ± 3.80 years; range 18–31). Two participants were excluded from the analyses because they did not complete the full experiment, two because they were not able to perform the task and four because they did not exhibit a significant attentional effect (see below, Analyses and statistics). All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and reported no history of psychiatric or neurological disorders, gave their written informed consent, and were compensated for their participation. All procedures were approved by the French ethics committee Ouest IV - Nantes (IRB #20.04.16.71057) and followed the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki).
Apparatus and stimuli
Participants sat in a dark room at 57 cm from a calibrated and linearized CRT monitor (800*600 pixels; width, 37.8 cm; height, 28.4 cm; refresh rate, 120 Hz). Their heads were positioned on a chin rest to maintain the distance between the monitor and the eyes. Stimuli were generated using Psychtoolbox 3 (3.0.12; Brainard, 1997; Pelli, 1997; Kleiner et al., 2007) in Matlab R2014b (The MathWorks Inc., 2014). The background was gray (127.5, 127.5, 127.5 RGB). A black central fixation annulus with inner eccentricity of 0.2° (degree of visual angle) and thickness of 0.16° was presented throughout the experiment. The precue and response cue were white rectangles (255, 255, 255 RGB, 0.5° long, 0.16° large) adjacent to the fixation (0.65° eccentricity from the center) and pointing down at a 45° angle toward the bottom left or bottom right. Stimuli were two conventional Landolt-C optotypes (2° high, 2° wide, 5.5° from the center at 45° angle toward the left or the right), and randomly generated circular noise patches at the same locations (2° diameter) with or without embedded Gabors (three cycles per degree; circular window). Specifically, the noise had random orientation content, drawn from a uniform distribution across all orientations −90 to +90° (polar angle) and a spatial frequency of three cycles per degree (cpd). On half of the trials, a Gabor (3 cpd sinusoidal grating in a Gaussian envelope subtending 2°; oriented vertically) was embedded in the noise patch, while maintaining the same total contrast as the original noise patch (20% contrast).
Eye-tracking
The dominant eye of each participant was monitored using an infrared video camera (EyeLink 1000 plus, SR Research). Participants were instructed to maintain fixation. Each trial started contingent on fixating, and they had to maintain fixation until after the probe presentation. If a fixation break occurred, i.e., if participants’ gaze deviated by >1.5° or if they blinked, the trial was stopped and removed from the analysis. Supernumerary trials were added at the end of each block to compensate for rejected trials (M ± SD = 2,257.73 ± 1,320.76 fixation breaks total on average across participants).
Procedure
Participants performed a total of 22 sessions. First, in a 1 h training session they were familiarized with the trial sequence at slower delays. They then performed two staircase blocks (see below) in an additional 1 h session. They finally performed 20 1-h-long sessions (two sessions per day with a break of 30 min between session; the entire experiment took on average 2.5 months) of the main task (totaling a number of 10,080 trials per participant).
Main task
Each session was composed of four blocks of 126 trials each as well as supernumerary trials in case of fixation break (see above, Eye-tracking). Central fixation remained on the screen during the entire block. After a 1,000 ms stable fixation, a precue appeared for 60 ms followed by a 400 ms interstimulus interval (ISI). The two Landolt-Cs were displayed for 60 ms (one in each lower quadrant) along with a response cue indicating which of the two was the target. Participants were instructed to discriminate the position of the C gap (randomly presented on the left or the right) of the target Landolt-C (their response was collected at the end of the trial sequence). When the response cue corresponded to the precued location (2/3 of the trials), the trial was valid; when the response cue corresponded to the un-precued location (1/3 of the trials), the trial was invalid. Then a second, variable ISI (randomly chosen among 14 different possibilities equally distributed between 40 and 495 ms) preceded the presentation of two new stimuli: two patches of noise or noise + Gabor for 33 ms, at the same location as the previous Landolt-Cs, followed by a 60 ms delay. Participants were instructed to report the presence versus absence of the Gabor inside each patch of noise. The presence or absence of a Gabor in either patch within a trial was independent and random (50%), such that one, the other, both, or neither having a Gabor embedded were equally likely. These patches presented at multiple delays after the previous Landolt-Cs were used to probe the state of attention across time (Dubois et al., 2009; Dugué et al., 2015b; Senoussi et al., 2019), i.e., attention deployed in the Landolt-C task modulates probe report performance over the course of its sustained orienting (valid condition) or reorienting (invalid, endogenous reorienting, as its orienting, is not effective right from the onset of the response cue; Jonides, 1981; Muller and Rabbitt, 1989; Nakayama and Mackeben, 1989; Cheal and Lyon, 1991; Liu et al., 2007; Carrasco, 2011). At the end of the trial, participants were first asked to report the position of the gap in the target Landolt-C (press C key for a gap on the left with the left hand, and press B key for a gap on the right with the right hand; maximum response time of 1,500 ms) and then to report the presence or the absence of the Gabor inside each patch (for left patch, press C key for Gabor present and D key for Gabor absent with left hand; for right patch, press B key for Gabor present and H key for Gabor absent with right hand; maximum response time 2,000 ms).
Staircases
Two independent staircases were implemented via a Best PEST procedure (Pentland, 1980) using the Palamedes toolbox (Prins and Kingdom, 2018) in Matlab, to adapt first the size of the Landolt-C gap in the discrimination task and second the SNR [signal (Gabor)-to-noise ratio] for the detection task. In the first staircase (208 trials), only the discrimination task sequence was presented and the size of the Landolt-Cs gap (1.4 ± 0.4° on average across participants) was adjusted. This procedure (Best PEST) aimed at finding the gap size that maximized the likelihood of a cumulative normal distribution. The gap size selected is that which corresponded with 75% accuracy of the cumulative normal model. Here the trial sequence consisted of a neutral precue (both the left and right precue) for 60 ms, a 400 ms ISI and the Landolt-Cs together with the response cue for 60 ms. Participants had 1.5 s to report the location of the gap. In the second staircase (200 trials), the procedure was the same but only the detection task sequence was presented. The Best PEST procedure aimed at finding the Gabor-to-noise ratio that maximized the likelihood of the Weibull distribution. The Gabor-to-noise ratio selected was the one that matched 62% accuracy of the Weibull model. In this staircase, a trial was defined as correct when the response to both patches (left and right) was correct. One trial consisted of neutral precue for 60 ms, a 400 ms ISI, and the two probe patches (50% noise only and 50% noise + Gabor, at each location) with a response cue for 33 ms. Participants had 2 s to report the presence or the absence of the Gabor in each of the two patches. Note that it is the Gabor-like properties of the noise and its orientation, not the Gabor per se, that is crucial for the analysis. Specifically, participants are attempting to detect the presence of a signal composed of a specific orientation and frequency, which will happen to be strongly represented in the random noise on certain trials. During analysis, we relate the noise properties on each trial with the propensity to report the presence versus absence of the Gabor, such that both correct and incorrect responses provide information on sensory tuning (see below, Reverse correlation analysis). Therefore, we aimed for a lower percent correct in the detection task, such that the Gabor was difficult to detect and thus participants’ responses are more likely to be influenced by the orientation content of the noise on any given trial.
Analyses and statistics
Except when otherwise specified, all analyses were performed using Matlab 2021b (The MathWorks Inc., 2021) and the Circular Statistics Toolbox 2012a (Berens, 2009).
Attentional manipulation
To ensure that attention was successfully manipulated (Carrasco, 2011; Dugué and VanRullen, 2014; Dugué et al., 2016, 2019; Senoussi et al., 2019), we assessed performance in the discrimination task as per d-prime (Hanning et al., 2019; Eq. 1):
Attentional manipulation. A, Trial sequence. Trials began with a central fixation period of 1,000 ms. An endogenous precue appeared for 60 ms. After 400 ms, Landolt-Cs were displayed for 60 ms together with a response cue indicating which Landolt-C was the target (2/3 of valid trials, i.e., target at precued location). Each Landolt-C could be oriented to the right or to the left randomly. After a variable delay from 40 to 495 ms (35 ms step; only the fixation circle remained on the screen), two patches were presented for 33 ms at the same location as the Landolt-C. Each patch could either show noise only (1/2 of trials) or noise + Gabor (1/2). After a third ISI of 60 ms, participants were instructed to report: (1) the orientation of the target Landolt-C (right/left; discrimination task) and (2) the presence or absence of a Gabor in each of the two patches. B, Behavioral results of the discrimination task. Individual (gray) and averaged (black) d-primes and median reaction times (RT) in the valid and invalid conditions. Error bars: standard error of the mean. C, Behavioral result of the Gabor detection in noise patches. Individual (gray) and averaged (black) d-primes. Error bars: standard error of the mean.
The validity effect (comparing performance between valid and invalid trials) was first assessed for each individual participant. D-prime was computed for each session and nonparametric statistics were performed: Wilcoxon signed rank test for paired-sample statistical comparison of valid and invalid d-prime. Out of the 15 participants, four participants did not show significant difference between valid and invalid trials, suggesting that the attentional manipulation was not successful. The following analyses were thus conducted on the remaining 11 participants (all correct and incorrect trials were included). We report the following effect sizes (Eq. 2):
Behavioral rhythms
For the detection task, d-primes were examined for each cueing (valid and invalid) and ISI (delay between the Landolt-C and the probe patches) condition (note that d-primes were normalized for visualization purposes only; Fig. 2; all analyses were performed on non-normalized d-prime). Responses to both patches were combined in order to increase the number of trials for reverse correlation analysis (and because no difference was found between the two patches locations; analysis not shown). In total, there were 720 trials per ISI and participant. Fast Fourier transform decompositions (FFT) were further performed on averaged d-primes to assess possible periodicity. Amplitude spectra for valid and invalid trials were plotted from 2.04 to 14.29 Hz with steps of 2.04 Hz (eight different frequencies). Phase angles were extracted from FFTs performed on each participant’s data for 12.2 Hz in valid and 6.1 Hz in invalid. We selected high- and low-performance trials by taking local maxima (above d-prime average) and minima (below d-prime average) for each of the d-prime time courses (ISI valid high: 145, 285, 390 and 460 ms; low: 75, 180, 320, 425 and 495 ms; ISI invalid high: 110, 320 and 425 ms; low: 40, 145, 355 and 460 ms). Two local maxima were removed, one in each condition, because they were below the averaged d-primes. All analyses described below were done separately for the high- and low-performance trials.
Behavioral rhythms. A, Individual (light traces) and averaged (dark traces) d-prime time courses (n = 11) for valid (blue) and invalid (orange) conditions separately. Shaded area: 95% confidence interval. B, Amplitude spectra obtained from FFT of individual (light traces) d-prime time courses and from FFT of averaged d-prime time courses across participants (dark traces). Dotted line: p = 0.001 (permutation test for averaged d-prime time course). Polar histogram of individual phases (light bins) and weighted phase averages (arrows) at 12.2 Hz in the valid condition and 6.1 Hz in the invalid condition. Bar plots: number of participants at each frequency with the highest peak in individual amplitude spectra.
Note that valid and invalid conditions do not have the same number of trials. Also, the ISI selection detailed above shows differences between validity condition and high/low performance. To assess the potential impact of such differences in trial number, all previous analyses for both discrimination and detection tasks were also performed on subsampled trials (100 repetitions, random selection), thus equalizing trial numbers across all conditions. The results were the same as without subsampling (analysis not shown). We thus present the results from nonsubsampled data.
All previous analyses were similarly performed on the decision criterion to assess whether the observed periodicity is exclusively on sensitivity or can also be observed on decision bias. Since in our task criterion is not independent of d-prime, we calculated criterion-prime (Eq. 3) as follows:
Reverse correlation analysis
To address behavioral feature tuning, we first transformed the noise from luminance intensity into the energy of different orientation components. We converted the noise image (either noise only or noise + Gabor) of each trial from pixel space (luminance intensity of each pixel) to a 1D space defined by the noise energy of components responding to different orientations (varied across all of orientation space, −90° to 90°, in steps of 7.5°, for 25 points on a linear scale). To compute the energy of each component, we took the noise image
S of each trial and convolved it with two grating filters (
We assessed the statistical differences between high- and low-performance trials’ tuning curves using linear mixed model with R (4.2.2; (R Core Team, 2022)), and specifically, differences in the standard deviation, amplitude, and baseline of the tuning curves. Linear mixed models were fitted by maximizing the restricted log likelihood and using a normal distribution function. Performance was used as predictor with two levels (high and low) and participants as random effect. Nonparametric, paired-sample Wilcoxon signed rank tests were further performed to compare, for each orientation valid, high- and low-performance decision weights (FDR correction for multiple comparisons was applied).
Similar to the d-prime analyses, we assessed the potential impact of differences in trial number using a subsampling procedure (100 repetitions, random selection). The results were the same as without subsampling (analysis not shown). We thus present the results from nonsubsampled data.
Data and code availability
All data and analysis code are available on the OSF repository (https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/CXKBU).
Results
Successful attentional manipulation
We used a well-established psychophysics protocol (Fig. 1A) tailored to identify rhythms in attentional tasks (for review Kienitz et al., 2022). Covert, voluntary attention was manipulated using a precue (2/3 validity). Participants were instructed to first perform a discrimination task—indicating the side of the gap, left or right, of a target Landolt-C—allowing us to confirm that attention was successfully manipulated and deployed to the precued location. Only participants showing a significantly higher d-prime for valid (sustained mode of attention; target at the precued location) than invalid (exploratory attention; target at the unpre-cued location) trials were included in the following analyses (see Materials and Methods, Analyses and statistics). The included participants are represented in Figure 1B (to avoid circularity, inferential statistics were not performed on d-primes across participants—participants were selected based on a higher d-prime in valid compared with invalid). Note that reaction times (RTs) were similar for valid and invalid trials (W = 17, z = −1.42, p = 0.15, effect size r = −0.30; note that RT values are short because they are measured from the onset of the Response 1 window), indicating no speed–accuracy trade-off (Dugué et al., 2018; Senoussi et al., 2019; Duyar et al., 2023).
Attentional performance fluctuates periodically at 12.2 Hz for sustained and 6.1 Hz for exploratory attention
In the same trial, after the discrimination task and a variable delay, participants were presented with a second target, on which they were asked to perform a detection task reporting the presence or absence of a vertical Gabor embedded in band-passed noise with random orientation content (Gabor present in 50% of trials; same protocol as in Li et al., 2016). Averaged d-primes for this task (Fig. 1C) revealed a significant difference between valid and invalid conditions (Wilcoxon signed rank test; W = 66, z = 2.93, p = 0.0033, effect size r = 0.63). Importantly, this second task was used to probe attention at each of the two stimulus locations and at various times during attentional orienting (valid trials; sustained attention) and reorienting (invalid; exploratory attention), assessing potential rhythms in attentional sampling. We then computed d-primes for the detection task independently for the valid and invalid conditions and for each ISI between the discrimination stimulus offset and detection probes onset. Across ISIs, d-prime exhibited multiple peaks and troughs, and individual data makes clear this was highly consistent across participants (Fig. 2A; note that d-prime is normalized in the figure, but analyses were performed on non-normalized d-primes).
We then performed fast Fourier transforms (FFT) of the averaged d-primes for the valid and invalid conditions separately. Peak frequencies were identified: 12.2 Hz in valid and 6.1 Hz in invalid (permutations tests: 100,000 surrogates, p = 0.001 after correction for multiple comparisons; note that we purposefully selected a conservative significance criterion to alleviate concerns regarding statistical power; Brookshire, 2022; Fig. 2B). Secondary effects were observed in the valid condition at lower frequencies, indicating some variabilities across participants (see Discussion). We concentrated the next analyses on the 12.2 Hz effect, which was the most prominent effect and was consistent between the results of the FFT on the averaged d-primes and FFT on individual d-primes. In the invalid condition, an additional significant peak frequency was observed at 2.04 Hz reflecting the inverted U-shape trend [no detrending was performed here (Fiebelkorn et al., 2013; Michel et al., 2022); we did not analyze it further].
We further showed that the 12.2 Hz (for the valid condition) and 6.1 Hz (for the invalid) individual periodic modulations were in phase across participants (Rayleigh tests for circular data showed significant nonuniform distributions for both valid: Z = 6.14, p = 0.001; and invalid: Z = 4.54, p = 0.0075). Finally, in a supplementary analysis we examined whether there was periodicity in decision bias (criterion), as was found in sensitivity (d-prime). Because d-prime differed across ISIs, we needed to take that into account by controlling for d-prime in our measurement of decision bias. Specifically, we calculated criterion-prime [criterion/d-prime (Macmillan and Creelman, 2005); measure of decision bias unaffected by fluctuations of d-prime], in both valid and invalid conditions. We found no periodicity in criterion-prime. Our results are thus not attributable to changes in decision bias (analysis not shown).
Together, our results replicate previously observed behavioral rhythms (Dugué and VanRullen, 2014; Dugué et al., 2016, 2019; Senoussi et al., 2019) and specifically show a highly consistent effect in sensitivity across participants (see also Li et al., 2016).
Rhythmic sensory representations
For valid and invalid conditions separately, we split the ISI into low- and high-performance ISIs. ISIs corresponding to d-prime local maxima (and which were above the mean d-prime) were categorized as high-performance and ISIs corresponding to d-prime local minima (and which were below the mean) were categorized as low-performance. We then combined all high-performance trials (coming from four different ISIs in valid: 145, 285, 390 and 460 ms; three ISIs in invalid: 110, 320 and 425 ms) and low-performance trials (five ISIs in valid: 75, 180, 320, 425 and 495 ms; four ISIs in invalid: 40, 145, 355 and 460 ms) for the next analysis. This trial separation was then used to assess modulations in sensory representations using reverse correlation analysis.
Fluctuations in sensitivity (d-prime) suggest a modification of sensory representations over time, such that high performance should be associated with more optimal sensory tuning compared with low performance. Here we assess which characteristics of sensory representations varied. Sensory tuning was modeled as a Gaussian distribution across orientations (Fig. 3), thus tuning could vary via differences in the following: (1) tuning sharpness—for high-performance trials, high decision weights arise only for orientations more similar to the target orientation; (2) gain or amplitude—for high-performance trials, decision weights are multiplicatively higher across orientations; and (3) suppression or baseline shift—for high-performance trials, irrelevant orientations, i.e., those far from the target orientation, are suppressed more successfully.
Sensory tuning hypotheses. Gaussian model of participant's sensory tuning. Sensory representations are different during phases of high (black traces) versus phases of low (gray traces) performance. Modifications in the (A) tuning sharpness, (B) tuning amplitude (gain), and (C) tuning baseline.
Reverse correlation of behavioral performance (see Materials and Methods) allowed testing of these three hypotheses, separately for each attention condition—sustained (valid) and exploratory (invalid). This provided a sensitivity kernel for each orientation, i.e., decision weight, representing the level of correlation between the proportion of trials reporting “Target Present” and the energy of that particular orientation in the noise of the stimulus (Fig. 4; note that decision weights were averaged for positive and negative orientations). A high decision weight indicates a high correlation, suggesting that more energy for the orientation value in question within the noise positively influenced the likelihood of “Target Present” responses. The results show, first, that the highest decision weights were observed for orientation corresponding to the target grating orientation (0°; peak of the Gaussian). For each orientation, a paired-sample Wilcoxon signed rank test on the difference between high- and low-performance trials’ decision weights was then computed and FDR corrected for 13 comparisons (p-threshold FDR corrected = 0.0038; positive and negative orientation results were averaged). We observed significant differences for the values far from the target orientation mainly in the valid condition and for near-target orientation values in the invalid condition.
Rhythmic sensory representations: Decision weights from reverse correlation for valid and invalid trials. Dark traces: high-performance trials. Light traces: low-performance trials. Circles: average weight across participants of the correlation between energy at each orientation (averaged across positive and negative orientations) and participant's response using a GLM. Error bars: 95% confidence interval. Solid lines: Gaussian fit. * is the significant difference between high- and low-performance (p-threshold FDR corrected = 0.0038).
We then fit a single Gaussian curve—the tuning curve—to the 25 decisions weights (1 per orientation; see Materials and Methods). To identify the changes in tuning that accompany periodicity in d-prime, we used linear mixed model to compare, across participants, each of the three following free parameters in the tuning fits: standard deviation (SD), amplitude and baseline, between high-performance and low-performance trials, separately for the valid and invalid conditions. There was no significant difference between high- and low-performance trials in both valid and invalid conditions for the SD (valid: t(10) = 1.36, p = 0.205, estimate = 15.09 ± 11.13, standard error; invalid: t(10) = −0.197, p = 0.848, estimate = −2.73 ± 13.84). Amplitude was significantly higher in high- compared with low-performance trials in both valid and invalid conditions (valid: t(10) = −6.57, p = 0.0001, estimate = −0.12 ± 0.02; invalid: t(10) = −3.94, p = 0.003, estimate = −0.11 ± 0.03). Baseline was significantly lower in high- compared with low-performance trials for the valid condition only (valid: t(10) = 7.62, p = 0, estimate = 0.08 ± 0.01; invalid: t(10) = −0.407, p = 0.692, estimate = −0.008 ± 0.02). These results indicate that, in the valid condition, when attention was sustained at one location, the performance fluctuation at alpha frequency was mediated by suppression of distracting features (baseline; hypothesis C) with a corresponding enhancement (gain) that kept sensitivity at target orientations constant across time (Fig. 3), whereas, in the invalid condition, when attention had to explore the space, the fluctuation at theta frequency was mediated exclusively by a rhythmic change in enhancement (gain) of relevant features (Fig. 3, hypothesis B).
Discussion
The rhythmic sampling theory of attention has become an important topic of research yet remains debated due to methodological considerations and a lack of clear understanding regarding sensory mechanisms underlying performance fluctuations. We addressed this gap using a paradigm with a large amount of data collected for each participant and applied reverse correlation analysis relating stimulus sensory information with behavioral performance. We assessed fluctuations in sensory representations for sustained (valid) and exploratory (invalid) attention. Attention was manipulated using spatial cueing. Reaction times were short (measured after the long target-to-response delay) and below 600 ms—typical time required for working memory processes to occur (Phillips, 1974). We favored a large number of trials per participant than a large sample following recent recommendations (Smith and Little, 2018) but a replication in independent samples would be informative.
Performance sensitivity (d-prime) showed a main periodic fluctuation at the alpha frequency (12.2 Hz) for sustained attention and theta (6.1 Hz) for exploratory attention, replicating previous reports (Dugué et al., 2017, 2019; Senoussi et al., 2019; Michel et al., 2022; for review Dugué and VanRullen, 2017; Kienitz et al., 2022). Such effect, obtained with a protocol involving far more individual data than previous studies, represents a strong confirmation of rhythmicity of voluntary attention. Interestingly, for valid trials, we found a large decrease in d-prime ∼300 ms. This has been observed in previous publications on behavioral rhythms (Dugué et al., 2015a; Dugué and VanRullen, 2017). We speculate that if behavioral sampling emerges from iterative feedforward–feedback loops between different cortical regions (presumably between visual and frontal regions here; Fiebelkorn and Kastner, 2019), 300 ms may be the average time for the optimal number of iterations to successfully performed the task. Further investigation is warranted. Note also that there were secondary effects at lower frequencies in the valid condition. They likely reflect harmonics, similar to the 12.2 Hz nonsignificant peak in the invalid condition. Contrary to previous reports, we did not find phase opposition between probes at the response-cued and response-uncued locations. Phase opposition between the two sampled location and no opposition have both been reported in the literature. For instance, although Landau and Fries (2012) have reported antiphase oscillation between validity conditions, Michel et al. (2022) found phase consistency across conditions and participants. One possibility, as previously proposed, is that the time ratio of location sampling/attention shift is unequal (Gaillard and Ben Hamed, 2022), i.e., the phase at one location would not be in antiphase with the one at the other location but only phase lagged. Our design using fixed ISIs may not provide a high enough frequency resolution to detect such phase differences.
Importantly, reverse correlation analysis was used to infer the neural representation underlying behavioral rhythms and approximate electrophysiological tuning properties. The results reveal differences in feature tuning across time between the two attention modes. Sustained attention displayed rhythmic suppression of distracting features—a change in baseline that decreased the influence of the most irrelevant orientation energy and a corresponding change in gain that kept the influence of target orientation energy constant across time. Exploratory attention exclusively showed enhancement of relevant features, i.e., a pure gain of the tuning function at peaks relative to troughs.
Previous behavioral and electrophysiological evidence supports rhythmicity in perception and attention. Behavioral studies revealed rhythmic fluctuations of performance (reaction time, d-prime, accuracy) in various sensory modalities [vision (Landau and Fries, 2012; Fiebelkorn et al., 2013; Dugué et al., 2015a, 2017; Senoussi et al., 2019), audition (VanRullen et al., 2014; Ho et al., 2019), somatosensation (Baumgarten et al., 2015)] and types of response (finger, hand, arm, eye movements; Chota et al., 2018; Kienitz et al., 2018; Benedetto and Morrone, 2019). It was suggested that when attention is sustained at one location, information is sampled at the alpha frequency, whereas, when exploring, attention samples information at the theta frequency (for review Dugué and VanRullen, 2017; Kienitz et al., 2022)—consistent with the results of the current study. This specific frequency effect was also reported in EEG studies (Dugué et al., 2015a; Harris et al., 2018; Merholz et al., 2022). As this research matured, the need to characterize specific neural computations underlying periodic behavioral fluctuations has grown. Authors have speculated that such variations were due to fluctuations in decision bias. Changes in EEG alpha activity could be explained by changes in decision criterion (Samaha et al., 2020). Here, using behavior alone, we demonstrated that fluctuations in sensitivity were not attributable to decision bias as there was no periodicity in the criterion-prime, for both the valid and invalid conditions. Tuning curves from reverse correlation analysis were used as proxy for neural computations (Ahumada, 2002; Eckstein et al., 2002; Paltoglou and Neri, 2012; Wyart et al., 2012; Li et al., 2016; Tu et al., 2023), accounting for the influence of specific participants’ responses.
Previous decades of attention research have concentrated on the effects of attention on perception and showed evidence for both signal enhancement and external noise reduction. Attention can enhance signals through contrast gain (Lee et al., 1999; Cameron et al., 2002; Huang and Dobkins, 2005; Ling and Carrasco, 2006), response gain (Huang and Dobkins, 2005; Ling and Carrasco, 2006), orientation sensitivity gain (Lee et al., 1997, 1999), spatial resolution gain (Yeshurun and Carrasco, 1998, 1999; Carrasco et al., 2002; Golla et al., 2004), or information processing speed gain (Carrasco and McElree, 2001). Similarly, active suppression of irrelevant information occurs, also called external noise reduction. Alpha-band brain oscillations seem to prominently contribute to this noise reduction (Foxe and Snyder, 2011; Händel et al., 2011; Bonnefond and Jensen, 2013; Wöstmann et al., 2019). Given the accumulated evidence for both attentional mechanisms (enhancement and suppression), they are likely not mutually exclusive. Dosher and Lu (2000) indeed showed that either mechanism can arise in the same task under different levels of difficulty: stronger signal enhancement effect with low external noise and stronger external noise reduction effect with higher external noise. They have also argued that which of the two mechanisms is engaged depends on the type of attention deployed (Lu and Dosher, 2000).
Here we found both signal enhancement and external noise reduction. When attention was directed to the target location, attention suppressed irrelevant features far from the target orientation (baseline shift) and compensated with increased gain (amplitude). This resulted in equal decision weights at the target orientation for both high- and low-performance phases of d-prime sensitivity, but lower decision weights far from the target orientation at periods of high compared with low performance. When attention is oriented and sustained at a target location, alpha rhythms in performance primarily reflect fluctuations in the suppression of irrelevant features—advantageous suppression at peaks and poorer suppression at troughs—while a gain mechanism keeps the influence of relevant stimulus information constant over time. When attention reorients from an irrelevant location to a relevant one, theta rhythms in sensitivity are associated only with changes in the amplitude of the tuning function, most strongly influencing sensitivity around the target feature, such that high-performance phases have higher decision weights at orientations close to target feature compared with low-performance phases.
We found external noise reduction and signal enhancement in separate modes of voluntary spatial attention. This pattern likely reflects the distinct functional roles of sustained and exploratory attention. Because attention efficiently allocates resources based on task demands (Lennie, 2003; Carrasco, 2011; Merholz et al., 2022), finding distinct behavioral trade-offs during different modes of attention suggests the two modes have distinct roles in aiding perception and performance. When attention shifts from one location to another, i.e., attention is exploring space, signal enhancement of the most relevant features may aid in locating the target for further discrimination. When attention is sustained on a single location, i.e., the target location has already been identified, perception is enhanced via external noise reduction, as it is no longer necessary to find the target via signal enhancement.
The particular frequency of either sensory tuning modulation may be related to their corresponding attention modes’ functional roles (see reviews Dugué and VanRullen, 2017; Kienitz et al., 2022). Alternatively, either sensory mechanism may operate at a particular frequency due to metabolic or neural processing constraints. The link between attention mode, the frequencies of performance modulation, and the underlying sensory tuning mechanisms requires further investigation.
Using a reverse correlation analysis of participants’ responses and random noise information, we inferred the underlying neural mechanism. The reverse correlation tuning curves are comparable with those from neural recordings (Neri and Levi, 2006; Fernández et al., 2022). Critically, if the decision weights derived from reverse correlation are a true estimate of sensory weighting, then these weights’ fluctuations can be logically related to fluctuations of neural representations. This statement aligns with studies linking attentional rhythms to brain oscillations at the same frequencies (Dugué et al., 2015a; Fiebelkorn et al., 2018; Helfrich et al., 2018; Kienitz et al., 2018). Yet, due to the constraints imposed by such an intensive protocol, we do not have direct measurements of neural activity. This study will, however, provide valuable insights for further neurophysiological research.
Footnotes
This work was supported by the European Research Council (ERC) under the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme (grant agreement No 852139, L.D.). We also thank Laetitia Grabot, Kirsten Petras, and Marisa Carrasco and her lab for their useful comments on the manuscript and advice on analyses.
↵*L.G. and I.D. contributed equally to this work.
↵‡L.D. is the senior author.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Laurie Galas at laurie.galas{at}gmail.com.