Skip to main content

Towards a Cognitive and Neurobiological Model of Motivated Forgetting

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Book cover True and False Recovered Memories

Part of the book series: Nebraska Symposium on Motivation ((NSM,volume 58))

Abstract

Historically, research on forgetting has been dominated by the assumption that forgetting is passive, reflecting decay, interference, and changes in context. This emphasis arises from the pervasive assumption that forgetting is a negative outcome. Here, we present a functional view of forgetting in which the fate of experience in memory is determined as much by motivational forces that dictate the focus of attention as it is by passive factors. A central tool of motivated forgetting is retrieval suppression, a process whereby people shut down episodic retrieval to control awareness. We review behavioral, neurobiological, and clinical research and show that retrieval suppression leads us to forget suppressed experiences. We discuss key questions necessary to address to develop this model, relationships to other forgetting phenomena, and the implications of this research for understanding recovered memories. This work provides a foundation for understanding how motivational forces influence what we remember of life experience.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Adams, Z. W., Derefinko, K. J., Milich, R., & Fillmore, M. T. (2008). Inhibitory functioning across ADHD subtypes: Recent findings, clinical implications, and future directions. Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 14(4), 268–275. doi:10.1002/ddrr.37.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Allen, G. A., Mahler, W. A., & Estes, W. K. (1969). Effects of recall tests on long-term retention of paired associates. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8(4), 463–470.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, M. C. (2001). Active forgetting: Evidence for functional inhibition as a source of memory failure. Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment, and Trauma, 4(2), 185–210.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, M. C. (2003). Rethinking interference theory: Executive control and the mechanisms of forgetting. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 415–445.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, M. C. (2005). The role of inhibitory control in forgetting unwanted memories: A consideration of three methods. In C. MacLeod & B. Uttl (Eds.), Dynamic cognitive processes (pp. 159–190). Tokyo: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, M. C., Bjork, R., & Bjork, E. (1994). Remembering can cause forgetting: Retrieval dynamics in long-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(5), 1063–1087.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, M. C., & Green, C. (2001). Suppressing unwanted memories by executive control. Nature, 410, 366–369.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, M. C., Reinholz, J., Kuhl, B., & Mayr, U. (2011). Intentional suppression of unwanted memories grows more difficult as we age. Psychology and Aging, 26, 397–405.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, M. C., & Levy, B. J. (2006). Encouraging the nascent cognitive neuroscience of repression. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29(5), 511–513.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, M. C., Ochsner, K. N., Kuhl, B., Cooper, J., Robertson, E., Gabrieli, S. W., et al. (2004). Neural systems underlying the suppression of unwanted memories. Science, 303(5655), 232–235. doi:10.1126/science.1089504.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, M. C., & Spellman, B. A. (1995). On the status of inhibitory mechanisms in cognition: Memory retrieval as a model case. Psychological Review, 102(1), 68–100.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, M. C., & Weaver, C. (2009). Inhibitory control over action and memory. In L. R. Squire (Ed.), The new encyclopedia of neuroscience (pp. 153–163). Oxford: Elsevier Ltd. doi:10.1016/B978-008045046- 9.00421-6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arnold, M. M., & Lindsay, D. S. (2002). Remembering remembering. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 521–529.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aron, A. R., Fletcher, P. C., Bullmore, E. T., Sahakian, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (2003). Stop-signal inhibition disrupted by damage to right inferior frontal gyrus in humans. Nature Neuroscience, 6(2), 115–116. doi:10.1038/nn1003.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aslan, A., & Bäuml, K. T. (2010). Retrieval-induced forgetting in young children. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 17(5), 704–709. doi:10.3758/PBR.17.5.704.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D., Eysenck, M., & Anderson, M. C. (2009). Memory. Hove: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Band, G. P., & van Boxtel, G. J. (1999). Inhibitory motor control in stop paradigms: Review and reinterpretation of neural mechanisms. Acta Psychologica, 101(2–3), 179–211.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barkley, R. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121(1), 65–94.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Basden, B. H., Basden, D. R., & Gargano, G. J. (1993). Directed forgetting in implicit and explicit memory tests: A comparison of methods. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19(3), 603–616.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. (2003). Ego depletion and self-regulation failure: A resource model of self-control. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 27(2), 281–284. doi:10.1097/01.ALC.0000060879.61384.A4.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(5), 1252–1265.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bekker, E. M., Kenemans, J. L., & Verbaten, M. N. (2005). Source analysis of the N2 in a cued Go/NoGo task. Cognitive Brain Research, 22(2), 221–231. doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.08.011.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Benjamin, A. (2010). In Benjamin Aaron (Ed.), Successful remembering and successful forgetting: A festschrift in honor of Robert A. Bjork. New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bergstrom, Z. M., Anderson, M. C., Buda, M., Simons, J., & Richardson-Klavehn, A. (submitted). Intentional retrieval suppression can conceal guilty knowledge in ERP memory detection tests.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bergström, Z. M., de Fockert, J. W., & Richardson-Klavehn, A. (2009). ERP and behavioural evidence for direct suppression of unwanted memories. NeuroImage, 48(4), 726–737. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.051.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bergström, Z. M., Velmans, M., de Fockert, J., & Richardson-Klavehn, A. (2007). ERP evidence for successful voluntary avoidance of conscious recollection. Brain Research, 1151, 119–133. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2007.03.014.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bjork, R. A. (1972). Theoretical implications of directed forgetting. In A. W. Melton & E. Martin (Eds.), Coding processes in human memory (pp. 217–235). Washington, DC: Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjork, R. A. (1975). Retrieval as a memory modifier. In R. Solso (Ed.), Information processing and cognition: The Loyola Symposium (pp. 123–144). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjork, R. A. (1989). Retrieval inhibition as an adaptive mechanism in human memory. In H. L. Roediger & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Varieties of memory and consciousness: Essays in honour of Endel Tulving (pp. 309–330). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjork, E. L., & Bjork, R. A. (2003). Intentional forgetting can increase, not decrease, the residual influences of to-be-forgotten information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 524–531.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bjork, E. L., Bjork, R. A., & Anderson, M. C. (1998). Varieties of goal-directed forgetting. In J. M. Golding & C. M. MacLeod (Eds.), Intentional forgetting: Interdisciplinary approaches (pp. 103–137). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bokura, H., Yamaguchi, S., & Kobayashi, S. (2001). Electrophysiological correlates for response inhibition in a Go/NoGo task. Clinical Neurophysiology, 112(12), 2224–2232.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Booth, J. R., Burman, D. D., Meyer, J. R., Lei, Z., Trommer, B. L., Davenport, N. D., et al. (2005). Larger deficits in brain networks for response inhibition than for visual selective attention in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(1), 94–111. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00337.x.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brewin, C. R. (2012, this volume). A theoretical framework for understanding recovered memory experiences. In R. F. Belli (Ed.), True and false recovered memories: Toward a reconciliation of the debate (pp. 149–173). Vol. 58: Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A. S. (1976). Spontaneous recovery and human learning. Psychological Bulletin, 83, 321–338.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bulevich, J. B., Roediger, H. L., Balota, D. A., & Butler, A. C. (2006). Failures to find suppression of episodic memories in the think/no-think paradigm. Memory & Cognition, 34(8), 1569–1577.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butler, A. J., & James, K. H. (2010). The neural correlates of attempting to suppress negative versus neutral memories. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 10(2), 182–194. doi:10.3758/CABN.10.2.182.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cameron, C. (1993, April). Recovering memories of childhood sexual abuse: A longitudinal report. Paper presented at the Western Psychological Association convention, Phoenix, AZ, USA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carrier, M., & Pashler, H. (1992). The influence of retrieval on retention. Memory & Cognition, 20(6), 633–642.

    Google Scholar 

  • Casey, B. J., Castellanos, F. X., Giedd, J. N., Marsh, W. L., Hamburger, S. D., Schubert, A. B., et al. (1997). Implication of right frontostriatal circuitry in response inhibition and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(3), 374–383. doi:10.1097/00004583-199703000-00016.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chan, J. C. K. (2009). Long-term effects of testing on the recall of nontested materials. Memory, 18(1), 49–57. doi:10.1080/09658210903405737.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Conroy, R., & Salmon, K. (2005). Selective postevent review and childrens’ memory for nonreviewed materials. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 90(4), 185–207. doi:10.1016/j.jecp. 2004.11.004.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Conroy, R., & Salmon, K. (2006). Talking about parts of a past experience: The impact of discussion style and event structure on memory for discussed and nondiscussed information. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 95(4), 278–297. doi:10.1016/j.jecp. 2006.06.001.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DePrince, A., Brown, L., Cheit, R., Freyd, J., Gold, S. N., Pezdek, K., & Quina, K. (2012, this volume). Motivated forgetting and misremembering: Perspectives from betrayal trauma theory. In R. F. Belli (Ed.), True and false recovered memories: Toward a reconciliation of the debate (pp. 193–242). Vol. 58: Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Depue, B. E., Banich, M. T., & Curran, T. (2006). Suppression of emotional and nonemotional content in memory: Effects of repetition on cognitive control. Psychological Science, 17(5), 441–447. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01725.x.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Depue, B. E., Burgess, G. C., Willcutt, E. G., Bidwell, L. C., Ruzic, L., & Banich, M. T. (2010). Symptom-correlated brain regions in young adults with combined-type ADHD: Their organization, variability, and relation to behavioral performance. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 182(2), 96–102. doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2009.11.011.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Depue, B. E., Burgess, G. C., Willcutt, E. G., Ruzic, L., & Banich, M. T. (2010). Inhibitory control of memory retrieval and motor processing associated with the right lateral prefrontal cortex: Evidence from deficits in individuals with ADHD. Neuropsychologia, 48(13), 3909–3917. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.09.013.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Depue, B. E., Curran, T., & Banich, M. T. (2007). Prefrontal regions orchestrate suppression of emotional memories via a two-phase process. Science, 317(5835), 215–219. doi:10.1126/science.1139560.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Detre, G. J., Natarajan, A., & Norman, K. A. (2010, November). Moderate memory activation leads to forgetting in the Think-No Think paradigm. Poster presented at the Annual Meeting of the Society for Neuroscience, San Diego, CA, USA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dieler, A. C., Plichta, M. M., Dresler, T., & Fallgatter, A. J. (2010). Suppression of emotional words in the Think/No-Think paradigm investigated with functional near-infrared spectroscopy. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 78(2), 129–135. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.06.358.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Donkers, F., & van Boxtel, G. J. M. (2004). The N2 in go/no-go tasks reflects conflict monitoring not response inhibition. Brain and Cognition, 56, 165–176.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Duzel, E., Cabeza, R., Picton, T. W., Yonelinas, A. P., Scheich, H., Heinze, H. J., et al. (1999). Task-related and item-related brain processes of memory retrieval. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 96, 1794–1799.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eimer, M. (1993). Effects of attention and stimulus probability on ERPs in a Go/Nogo task. Biological Psychology, 35(2), 123–138.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eldridge, L. L., Knowlton, B. J., Furmanski, C. S., Bookheimer, S. Y., & Engel, S. A. (2000). Remembering episodes: A selective role for the hippocampus during retrieval. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 1149.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Erdelyi, M. H. (1996). The recovery of unconscious memories: Hypermnesia and reminiscence. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Erdelyi, M. H. (2006). The unified theory of repression. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29(5), 499–551. doi:10.1017/S0140525X06009113.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Erdelyi, M., & Kleinbard, J. (1978). Has Ebbinghaus decayed with time? The growth of recall (hypermnesia) over days. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4(4), 275–289.

    Google Scholar 

  • Falkenstein, M. (2006). Inhibition, conflict and the Nogo-N2. Clinical Neurophysiology, 117(8), 1638–1640. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2006.05.002.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Falkenstein, M., Hoormann, J., & Hohnsbein, J. (1999). ERP components in Go/Nogo tasks and their relation to inhibition. Acta Psychologica, 101(2–3), 267–291.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fawcett, J. M., & Taylor, T. L. (2008). Forgetting is effortful: Evidence from reaction time probes in an item-method directed forgetting task. Memory & Cognition, 36(6), 1168–1181.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman-Summers, S., & Pope, K. S. (1994). The experience of ‘forgetting’ child abuse: A national survey of psychologists. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 3, 626–639.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ford, R. M., Keating, S., & Patel, R. (2004). Retrieval-induced foregetting: A developmental study. Developmental Psychology, 22(4), 5850–603.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freyd, J. J. (1996). Betrayal trauma: The logic of forgetting childhood abuse. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freyd, J. J., DePrince, A. P., & Gleaves, D. (2007). The state of betrayal trauma theory: Reply to McNally – conceptual issues and future directions. Memory, 15, 295–311.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freyd, J. J., Deprince, A. P., & Zurbriggen, E. L. (2006). Self-reported memory for abuse depends upon victim-perpetrator relationship. Journal of Trauma & Dissociation, 2(3), 5–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, D., & Johnson, R. (2000). Event-related potential (ERP) studies of memory encoding and retrieval: A selective review. Microscopy Research and Technique, 51(1), 6–28. doi:10.1002/1097-0029(20001001)51:1<6::AID-JEMT2>3.0.CO;2-R.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Garavan, H., Ross, T. J., Murphy, K., Roche, R. A. P., & Stein, E. A. (2002). Dissociable executive functions in the dynamic control of behavior: Inhibition, error detection, and correction. NeuroImage, 17(4), 1820–1829.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Garcia-Bajos, E., Migueles, M., & Anderson, M. C. (2009). Script knowledge modulates retrieval-induced forgetting for eyewitness events. Memory, 17(1), 92–103. doi:10.1080/09658210802572454.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Geiselman, R. E., Bjork, R. A., & Fishman, D. L. (1983). Disrupted retrieval in directed forgetting: A link with posthypnotic amnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 112(1), 58–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geraerts, E., Arnold, M. M., Lindsay, D. S., Merckelbach, H., Jelicic, M., & Hauer, B. (2006). Forgetting of prior remembering in people reporting recovered memories of childhood sexual abuse. Psychological Science, 17, 1002–1008.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Geraerts, E. (2012, this volume). Cognitive underpinnings of recovered memories of childhood abuse. In R. F. Belli (Ed.), True and false recovered memories: Toward a reconciliation of the debate (pp. 175–191). Vol. 58: Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geraerts, E., McNally, R. J., Jelicic, M., Merckelbach, H., & Raymaekers, L. (2008). Linking thought suppression and recovered memories of childhood sexual abuse. Memory, 16(1), 22–28. doi:10.1080/09658210701390628.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goernert, P. (2005). Source-monitoring accuracy across repeated tests following directed forgetting. British Journal of Psychology, 96(2), 231–247.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goernert, P. N., & Wolfe, T. (1997). Is there hypermnesia and reminiscence for information intentionally forgotten? Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51(3), 231–240.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, L. R., & Freyd, J. J. (2006). Self-reports of potentially traumatic experiences in an adult community sample: Gender differences and test-retest stabilities of the items in a brief betrayal-trauma survey. Journal of Trauma & Dissociation, 7(3), 39–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Golding, J. M., & MacLeod, C. M. (1998). Intentional forgetting: Interdisciplinary approaches. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodman, G. S., Ghetti, S., Quas, J. A., Edelstein, R. S., Alexander, K. W., Redlich, A. D., et al. (2003). A prospective study of memory for child sexual abuse: New findings relevant to the repressed-memory controversy. Psychological Science, 14, 113–118.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goodmon, L. B., & Anderson, M. C. (2011). Semantic integration as a boundary condition on inhibitory processes in episodic retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37(2), 416–436. doi:10.1037/a0021963.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hagger, M. S., Wood, C., Stiff, C., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2010). Ego depletion and the strength model of self-control: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136(4), 495–525. doi:10.1037/a0019486.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hanslmayr, S., Leipold, P., & Bauml, K. (2010). Anticipation boosts forgetting of voluntarily suppressed memories. Memory, 18, 252–257. doi:10.1080/09658210903476548.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hanslmayr, S., Leipold, P., Pastötter, B., & Bäuml, K. (2009). Anticipatory signatures of voluntary memory suppression. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29(9), 2742–2747. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4703-08.2009.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harnishfeger, K. K., & Pope, R. S. (1996). Intending to forget: The development of cognitive inhibition in directed forgetting. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 62(2), 292–315. doi:10.1006/jecp. 1996.0032.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1988). Working memory, comprehension, and aging: A review and a new view. In H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 22, pp. 193–225). San Diego, CA: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henkel, L. A. (2004). Erroneous memories arising from repeated attempts to remember. Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 26–46.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Henkel, L., & Koffman, K. J. (2004). Memory distortions in coerced false confessions: A source monitoring framework analysis. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18(5), 567–588.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hertel, P. T. (1994). Depressive deficits in memory: Implications of research and theory for memory improvement following traumatic brain injury. NeuroRehabilitation, 4, 143–150.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hertel, P. T. (1998). Relation between rumination and impaired memory in dysphoric moods. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107(1), 166–172.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hertel, P. T., Large, D., Dahl, E., & Levy, A. (2011). Suppression-induced forgetting on a free-association test.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hertel, P. T., & Calcaterra, G. (2005). Intentional forgetting benefits from thought substitution. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 12(3), 484–489.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hertel, P. T., & Gerstle, M. (2003). Depressive deficits in forgetting. Psychological Science, 14(6), 573–578.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hertel, P., & Mahan, A. (2008). Depression-related differences in learning and forgetting responses to unrelated cues. Acta Psychologica, 127(3), 636–644. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2007.11.004.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hertel, P., & McDaniel, L. (2010). The suppressive power of positive thinking: Aiding suppression-induced forgetting in repressive coping. Cognition and Emotion, 24(7), 1239–1249.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hotta, C., & Kawaguchi, J. (2009). Self-initiated use of thought substitution can lead to long term forgetting. Psychologia, 52(1), 41–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hourihan, K. L., & Taylor, T. L. (2006). Cease remembering: Control processes in directed forgetting. Journal Of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(6), 1354–1365. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.32.6.1354.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Huddleston, E., & Anderson, M.C. (in preparation). Retrieval suppression modulates activation in content-specific neocortical areas.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hulbert, J. C., Anderson, M. C., & Kuhl, B. (in preparation). Enhanced inhibitory control over memory in people with extensive traumatic experience.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hulbert, M. C., Shivde, G. S., & Anderson, M. C. (2011). Evidence against associative blocking as a cause of cue-independent retrieval-induced forgetting. Experimental Psychology.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Iversen, S., & Mishkin, M. (1970). Perseverative interference in monkeys following selective lesions of inferior prefrontal convexity. Experimental Brain Research, 11(4), 376–386.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M. K. (1994). Binding complex memories: The role of reactivation and the hippocampus. In D. L. Schacter & E. Tulving (Eds.), Memory systems (pp. 311–350). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, R., Kreiter, K., Russo, B., & Zhu, J. (1998). A spatio-temporal analysis of recognition-related event-related brain potentials. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 29(1), 83–104.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Joormann, J., Hertel, P. T., Brozovich, F., & Gotlib, I. H. (2005). Remembering the good, forgetting the bad: Intentional forgetting of emotional material in depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114(4), 640–648. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.114.4.640.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Joormann, J., Hertel, P. T., Lemoult, J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2009). Training forgetting of negative material in depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118(1), 34–43. doi:10.1037/a0013794.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Joormann, J., Yoon, K. L., & Zetsche, U. (2007). Cognitive inhibition in depression. Applied and Preventive Psychology, 12, 128–139.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karpicke, J. D., & Roediger, H. L. (2008). The critical importance of retrieval for learning. Science, 319(5865), 966–968. doi:10.1126/science.1152408.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kikuchi, H., Fujii, T., Abe, N., Suzuki, M., Takagi, M., Mugikura, S., et al. (2010). Memory repression: Brain mechanisms underlying dissociative amnesia. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(3), 602–613. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21212.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, K., & Yi, D. (2008, November). Perceptual consequences of memory suppression. Poster presented at the Annual Meeting of the Society for Neuroscience, Washington, DC, USA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kim, K., Yi, D., Yang, E., & Lee, K. (2007). What makes repressors good suppressors? The effect of trait anxiety. Korean Journal of Psychology, 26, 261–277.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kopp, B., Mattler, U., Goertz, R., & Rist, F. (1996). N2, P3 and the lateralized readiness potential in a nogo task involving selective response priming. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 99(1), 19–27.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lambert, A. J., Good, K. S., & Kirk, I. J. (2010). Testing the repression hypothesis: Effects of emotional valence on memory suppression in the think – no think task. Consciousness and Cognition, 19(1), 281–293. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2009.09.004.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Landauer, T. K., & Bjork, R. A. (1978). Optimum rehearsal patterns and name learning. In M. M. Gruneberg, P. E. Morris, & R. N. Sykes (Eds.), Practical aspects of memory (pp. 625–632). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lavric, A., Pizzagalli, D. A., & Forstmeier, S. (2004). When ‘go’ and ‘nogo’ are equally frequent: ERP components and cortical tomography. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 20(9), 2483–2488. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03683.x.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, Y., Lee, H., & Tsai, S. (2007). Effects of post-cue interval on intentional forgetting. British Journal of Psychology, 98(2), 257–272. doi:10.1348/000712606X120410.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LeMoult, J., Hertel, P. T., & Joorman, J. (2010). Training the forgetting of negative words: The role of direct suppression and the relation to stress reactivity. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24, 365–375.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levy, B. J., & Anderson, M. C. (2002). Inhibitory processes and the control of memory retrieval. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(7), 299–305.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levy, B. J., & Anderson, M. C. (2008). Individual differences in the suppression of unwanted memories: The executive deficit hypothesis. Acta Psychologica, 127(3), 623–635. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2007.12.004.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levy, B. J., & Wagner, A. D. (2011). Cognitive control and right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex: reflexive reorienting, motor inhibition, and action updating. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1224, 40–62.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D., Cowan, W., & Davis, K. (1994). On the ability to inhibit thought and action: A users’ guide to the stop-signal paradigm. In D. D. Carr & T. H. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, and language (pp. 189–239). San Diego, CA: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A. R. (1966). Higher cortical functions in man. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lustig, C., Hasher, L., & Tonev, S. T. (2001a). Inhibitory control over the present and the past. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 13, 107–122.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald, A. W., Cohen, J. D., Stenger, V. A., & Carter, C. S. (2000). Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. Science, 288(5472), 1835–1838.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, M., & Macrae, C. (2001). Gone but not forgotten: The transient nature of retrieval-induced forgetting. Psychological Science, 12(2), 148–152.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, M. D., & Saunders, J. (2008). Retrieval inhibition and memory distortion: Negative consequences of an adaptive process. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(1), 26–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marx, B. P., Marshall, P. J., & Castro, F. (2008). The moderating effects of stimulus valence and arousal on memory suppression. Emotion, 8(2), 199–207. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.8.2.199.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McNally, R. J. (2007). Betrayal trauma theory: A critical appraisal. Memory, 15, 280–294.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mecklinger, A., Parra, M., & Waldhauser, G. T. (2009). ERP correlates of intentional forgetting. Brain Research, 1255(C), 132–147. doi:doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2008.11.073.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meier, B., König, A., Parak, S., & Henke, K. (2011). Suppressed, but not forgotten. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 70(1), 5–11. doi:10.1024/1421-0185/a000033.

    Google Scholar 

  • Menon, V., Adleman, N. E., White, C. D., Glover, G. H., & Reiss, A. L. (2001). Error-related brain activation during a Go/NoGo response inhibition task. Human Brain Mapping, 12(3), 131–143.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, B. D., Muscatell, K. A., & Kensinger, E. A. (2011). Effects of Emotion and Age on Performance During a Think/No-Think Memory Task Psychology and Aging in press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nigg, J. (2000). On inhibition/disinhibition in developmental psychopathology: Views from cognitive and personality psychology and a working inhibition taxonomy. Psychological Bulletin, 126(2), 220–246.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nigg, J. (2001). Is ADHD a disinhibitory disorder? Psychological Bulletin, 127(5), 571–598. doi:10.1037//0033-2909.127.5.571.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nørby, S., Lange, M., & Larsen, A. (2010). Forgetting to forget: On the duration of voluntary suppression of neutral and emotional memories. Acta Psychologica, 133(1), 73–80. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.10.002.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Norman, W., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action. In R. J. Davidson, G. E. Schwartz, & D. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and self regulation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 4, pp. 1–18). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ogle, C. M., & Paz-Alonso, P. M. (in preparation). Developmental changes in the suppression of emotional memories.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oosterlaan, J., Logan, G. D., & Sergeant, J. A. (1998). Response inhibition in AD/HD, CD, comorbid AD/HD  +  CD, anxious, and control children: A meta-analysis of studies with the stop task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39(3), 411–425.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pavlov, I. P. (1927) Conditioned reflexes (G. V. Anrep, Trans.). London: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Payne, D. G. (1987). Hypermnesia and reminiscence in recall: A historical and empirical review. Psychological Bulletin, 101(1), 5–27. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.101.1.5.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paz-Alonso, P. M., Ghetti, S., Wendelken, C., Anderson, M. C., & Bunge, S. (2011). Mnemonic control relies on a frontal-parietal-hippocampal network that is strengthened over childhood.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paz-Alonso, P. M., Ghetti, S., Matlen, B. J., Anderson, M. C., & Bunge, S. A. (2009). Memory suppression is an active process that improves over childhood. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 3, 24. doi:10.3389/neuro.09.024.2009.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Quay, H. C. (1997). Inhibition and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 25(1), 7–13.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Racsmány, M., Conway, M. A., & Demeter, G. (2010). Consolidation of episodic memories during sleep: Long-term effects of retrieval practice. Psychological Science, 21(1), 80–85. doi:10.1177/0956797609354074.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ramautar, J., Kok, A., & Ridderinkhof, K. (2004). Effects of stop-signal probability in the stop-signal paradigm: The N2/P3 complex further validated. Brain and Cognition, 56(2), 234–252. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2004.07.002.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, R. A. (2004). Spontaneous recovery. Learning & Memory, 11(5), 501–509. doi:10.1101/lm.77504.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rubia, K., Overmeyer, S., Taylor, E., Brammer, M., Williams, S., Simmons, A., et al. (1999). Hypofrontality in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder during higher-order motor control: A study with functional MRI. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 891–896.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rubia, K., Smith, A. B., Brammer, M. J., Tonne, B., & Taylor, E. (2005). Abnormal brain activation during inhibition and error detection in medication-naive adolescents with ADHD. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 1067–1075.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sahakyan, L., & Kelley, C. (2002). A contextual change account of the directed forgetting effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28(6), 1064–1072. doi:10.1037//0278-7393.28.6.1064.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sahakyan, L., Waldum, E. R., Benjamin, A. S., & Bickett, S. P. (2009). Where is the forgetting with list-method directed forgetting in recognition? Memory & Cognition, 37(4), 464–476. doi:10.3758/MC.37.4.464.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salamé, P., & Danion, J. (2007). Inhibition of inappropriate responses is preserved in the Think-No-Think and impaired in the random number generation tasks in schizophrenia. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13(2), 277–287. doi:10.1017/S1355617707070300.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sasaki, K., Gemba, H., & Tsujimoto, T. (1989). Suppression of visually initiated hand movement by stimulation of the prefrontal cortex in the monkey. Brain Research, 495(1), 100–107.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saunders, J., & MacLeod, M. D. (2002). New evidence on the suggestibility of memory: The role of retrieval-induced forgetting in misinformation effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8(2), 127–142.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schmajuk, M., Liotti, M., Busse, L., & Woldorff, M. G. (2006). Electrophysiological activity underlying inhibitory control processes in normal adults. Neuropsychologia, 44(3), 384–395. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.06.005.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schooler, J. W., Bendiksen, M., & Ambadar, Z. (1997). Taking the middle line: Can we accommodate both fabricated and recovered memories of sexual abuse? In M. Conway (Ed.), Recovered Memories and False Memories. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schultz, T. M., Passmore, J., & Yodor, C. Y. (2003). Emotional closeness with perpetrators and amnesia for child sexual abuse. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 12, 67–88.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shobe, K. K., & Schooler, J. W. (2001). Discovering fact and fiction: Case-based analyses of authentic and fabricated memories of abuse. In G. M. Davies & T. Dalgleish (Eds.), Recovered memories: Seeking the middle ground (pp. 95–151). Chichester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, M. (1993). Neurophysiological manifestations of recollective experience during recognition memory judgments. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5(1), 1–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, M., & Guster, K. (1993). Decomposition of recognition memory event-related potentials yields target, repetition, and retrieval effects. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 86(5), 335–343.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, S. M., & Moynan, S. C. (2008). Forgetting and recovering the unforgettable. Psychological Science, 19(5), 462–468. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02110.x.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, S. M., & Vela, E. (1991). Incubated reminiscence effects. Memory & Cognition, 19(2), 168–176.

    Google Scholar 

  • Squire, L. R. (1992). Memory and the hippocampus: A synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychological Review, 99(2), 195–231.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Storm, B. C. (2010). Retrieval-induced forgetting and the resolution of competition. In A. Benjamin (Ed.), Successful remembering and successful forgetting: A festschrift in honor of Robert A. Bjork (pp. 89–105). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Storm, B. C., Bjork, E. L., Bjork, R. A., & Nestojko, J. F. (2006). Is retrieval success a necessary condition for retrieval-induced forgetting? Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13(6), 1023–1027.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Storm, B. C., & White, H. A. (2010). ADHD and retrieval-induced forgetting: Evidence for a deficit in the inhibitory control of memory. Memory, 18(3), 265–271. doi:10.1080/09658210903547884.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tamm, L., Menon, V., Ringel, J., & Reiss, A. (2004). Event-related fMRI evidence of frontotemporal involvement in aberrant response inhibition and task switching in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 43(11), 1430–1440. doi:10.1097/01.chi.0000140452.51205.8d.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tandoh, K., & Naka, M. (2007). Durability of retrieval-induced forgetting. Shinrigaku Kenkyu: The Japanese Journal of Psychology, 78(3), 310–315.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomlinson, T. D., Huber, D. E., Rieth, C. A., & Davelaar, E. J. (2009). An interference account of cue-independent forgetting in the no-think paradigm. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(37), 15588–15593. doi:10.1073/pnas.0813370106.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tramoni, E., Aubert-Khalfa, S., Guye, M., Ranjeva, J. P., Felician, O., & Ceccaldi, M. (2009). Hypo-retrieval and hyper-suppression mechanisms in functional amnesia. Neuropsychologia, 47(3), 611–624.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Trott, C. T., Friedman, D., Ritter, W., Fabiani, M., & Snodgrass, J. G. (1999). Episodic priming and memory for temporal source: event-related potentials reveal age-related differences in prefrontal functioning. Psychology and Aging, 14, 390–413.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Underwood, B. J. (1949). Proactive inhibition as a function of time and degree of prior learning. Journal Of Experimental Psychology, 39(1), 24–34.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van Boxtel, G. J., van der Molen, M. W., Jennings, J. R., & Brunia, C. H. (2001). A psychophysiological analysis of inhibitory motor control in the stop-signal paradigm. Biological Psychology, 58(3), 229–262.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Waldhauser, G.T., Johansson, M., & Lindgren, M. (submitted). The effects of intentional suppression on recognition memory.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waldhauser, G. T., Johansson, M., Backstrom, M., & Mecklinger, A. (2011). Trait anxiety, working memory capacity, and the effectiveness of memory suppression. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 52(1), 21–27. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9450.2010.00845.x.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological Review, 101, 34–52.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D. J., Carter, S., & White, T. (1987). Paradoxical effects of thought suppression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 5–13.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wenzlaff, R. M., & Wegner, D. M. (2000). Thought suppression. In S. T. Fiske (Ed.), Annual review of psychology (Vol. 51, pp. 51–91). Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wessel, I., Huntjens, R. J. C., & Verwoerd, J. R. L. (2010). Cognitive control and suppression of memories of an emotional film. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 41(2), 83–89. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep. 2009.10.005.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wessel, I., Wetzels, S., Jelicic, M., & Merckelbach, H. (2005). Dissociation and memory suppression: A comparison of high and low dissociative individuals’ performance on the Think-No Think task. Personality and Individual Differences, 39(8), 1461–1470.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wheeler, M. A. (1995). Improvement in recall over time without repeated testing: Spontaneous recovery revisited. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(1), 173–184.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilding, E. L., Doyle, M. C., & Rugg, M. D. (1995). Recognition memory with and without retrieval of context: An event-related potential study. Neuropsychologia, 33(6), 743–767.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilding, E., & Rugg, M. (1996). An event-related potential study of recognition memory with and without retrieval of source. Brain, 119, 889–905.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, L. M. (1994). Recall of childhood trauma: A propective study of women’s memories of child sexual abuse. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62(6), 1177–81.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, S. P., & Kipp, K. (1998). The development of efficient inhibition: Evidence from directed-forgetting tasks. Developmental Review, 18(1), 86–123.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wylie, G. R., Fox, J. J., & Taylor, T. L. (2008). Forgetting as an active process: An fMRI investigation of item-method directed forgetting. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 670–682.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Preparation of this article was supported by National Science Foundation grant 0643321. The authors would like to thank Robert Bjork, Steve Smith, Karl-Heinz Bauml, Lili Sahakyan, Tracy Taylor-Hemick, Paula Hertel, Jutta Joormann, Kepa Paz-Alanso, Roland Benoit, and Zara Bergstrom for useful comments on this manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michael C. Anderson .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Appendix A

Appendix A

The 32 published articles on which the meta-analysis in Fig. 3 (right panel) is based. Note that four published articles are not included either because they used different dependent measures or did not fully report recall data (see caption, Fig. 3).

figure a_3

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2012 Springer Science+Business Media, LLC

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Anderson, M.C., Huddleston, E. (2012). Towards a Cognitive and Neurobiological Model of Motivated Forgetting. In: Belli, R. (eds) True and False Recovered Memories. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, vol 58. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-1195-6_3

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics