Elsevier

Vision Research

Volume 34, Issue 19, October 1994, Pages 2547-2559
Vision Research

Temporal summation of visual motion

https://doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(94)90241-0Get rights and content

Abstract

The sensitivity of the visual system to changes in velocity over time was investigated using the approach that Rashbass [(1970) Journal of Physiology, 210, 165–186] applied to luminance. Pairs of motion impulses (jumps) were presented, and thresholds for discriminating these pairs of impulses from a stationary display were determined. The results were consistent with a model that posits linear filtering of the input velocity, squaring, and integration over some duration. According to the model, the degree of interaction between the impulses reveals the autocorrelation of the impulse response of the motion system. The data were well fit by a three-element cascade of leaky integrators. In the temporal frequency domain, the visual motion system is a lowpass filter. This means that the visual system is quite insensitive to acceleration.

References (50)

  • ScobeyR.P. et al.

    Displacement thresholds for unidirectional and oscillatory movement

    Vision Research

    (1981)
  • ShioiriS. et al.

    Visual persistence of figures defined by relative motion

    Vision Research

    (1992)
  • SnowdenR.J. et al.

    Extension of displacement limits in multiple-exposure sequences of apparent motion

    Vision Research

    (1989)
  • SnowdenR.J. et al.

    The combination of motion signals over time

    Vision Research

    (1989)
  • SnowdenR.J. et al.

    The temporal integration and resolution of velocity signals

    Vision Research

    (1991)
  • WatamaniukS.N.J. et al.

    The human visual system averages speed information

    Vision Research

    (1992)
  • AdelsonE.H. et al.

    Spatiotemporal energy models for the perception of motion

    Journal of the Optical Society of America A

    (1985)
  • BallK. et al.

    Human vision favors centrifugal motion

    Perception

    (1980)
  • BowneS.F. et al.

    Motion interference in speed discrimination

    Journal of the Optical Society of America A

    (1989)
  • ChangJ.J. et al.

    Cooperative and noncooperative processes of apparent movement of random-dot cinematograms

    Spatial Vision

    (1985)
  • DettmanJ.W.

    Mathematical methods in science and engineering

    (1969)
  • Di LolloV.

    Temporal characteristics of iconic memory

    Nature

    (1977)
  • van DoornA.J. et al.

    Perception of movement and correlation in stroboscopically presented noise patterns

    Perception

    (1985)
  • DuffG.F.D. et al.

    Differential equations of applied mathematics

    (1966)
  • EgelhaafM. et al.

    Dynamic response properties of movement detectors: Theoretical analysis and electrophysiological investigation in the visual system of the fly

    Biological Cybernetics

    (1987)
  • Cited by (28)

    • Is conscious perception a series of discrete temporal frames?

      2018, Consciousness and Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      Thus, one could have a percept of a stimulus of given brightness persisting for 50 ms or for 150 ms, but that does not happen: instead, the percept is of a stimulus that is brighter when its objective duration is 150 ms than when it is 50 ms. Something similar appears to be the case for perceived loudness of auditory stimuli, on a time scale of more than 200 ms under some conditions (Räsänen & Laine, 2013; Rimmele et al., 2015; Zwislocki, 1969). Visual motion percepts involve summation or integration on a time scale of about 80–130 ms (McKee & Welch, 1985; Simpson, 1994; Snowden & Braddick, 1991), but under some conditions temporal integration for both biological and non-biological motion perception can occur on a time scale up to about 3000 ms (Burr & Santoro, 2001; Neri, Morrone, & Burr, 1998). I have already discussed the example of visible persistence, showing that the duration of visible persistence is not fixed but varies depending on both stimulus duration and motion properties (Di Lollo, 1980; Farrell, 1984), which represents a compromise between the processing objectives of feature analysis and minimisation of visual smear (Farrell, 1984).

    • Marker correspondence, not processing latency, determines temporal binding of visual attributes

      2002, Current Biology
      Citation Excerpt :

      This is, at least in part, we believe, because of temporal limits on the allocation of attention needed to link transitions and turning points. There is also considerable evidence showing that changes in speed (second-order changes) are poorly detected by the visual system [24–27]. Given that neural sensors specialized for temporal change exist only for transitions, but not for turning points, it is reasonable that the visual system can use only transitions at high temporal rates.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text