Elsevier

Neuroscience Research

Volume 44, Issue 3, November 2002, Pages 231-236
Neuroscience Research

Update article
Neural responses in the primary visual cortex of the monkey during perceptual filling-in at the blind spot

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-0102(02)00149-9Get rights and content

Abstract

The phenomenon of perceptual filling-in demonstrates that physical stimuli presented on the retina do not necessarily correspond to surface perception, and that our visual system has mechanisms with which to interpolate missing information in order to construct continuous surfaces. Among its various forms, filling-in at the blind spot is one of the most remarkable. To study the neural mechanisms involved in filling-in at the blind spot, we recently conducted a recording experiment aimed at determining whether the neurons in the primary visual cortex (V1) that represent the visual field corresponding to the blind spot are activated when filling-in occurs. We found that neurons located in deep layers of the V1, particularly layer 6, respond to large stimuli that cover the blind spot and induce perceptual filling-in. These neurons tended to have very large receptive fields, which extended out of the blind spot, and preferred relatively large stimuli. We believe that neurons in the V1 region representing the blind spot encode information essential for perceptual filling-in at the blind spot.

Section snippets

Retinotopic map

A remarkable feature of vision that distinguishes it from other sensory modalities is its ability to precisely transmit information about the relative locations of objects in the outer world. This ability has its basis in the topographic organization of the visual system. Neural projections that send retinal information to the visual cortex have a precise retinotopic organization that faithfully reproduce a map of the visual field on the visual cortical surface (retinotopic map). When we see an

Filling-in and surface perception

Perceptual filling-in indicates certain kinds of phenomena in which one perceives visual attributes in a region of the visual field where the information is actually missing. Many forms of this phenomenon have been identified, with filling-in at the blind spot being one of the most remarkable examples. The optic disk on the retina is an area in which the blood vessels enter and the optic nerve exits (Fig. 1). This area of the retina contains no photoreceptors; consequently, when one sees a

Neuron activities in the retinotopic representation of the blind spot in V1

We recorded the activity of single neurons in V1 of awake Japanese monkeys (macaca fuscata) performing a visual fixation task. Before the recording experiment, we determined the location of the blind spot in the visual field using a visual saccade paradigm (Komatsu and Murakami, 1994): it was on the horizontal meridian at about 15° in eccentricity, and its size was about 5° horizontally and 7° vertically. We then used a small spot (0.3–1°) to map the visual receptive fields of the neurons

A possible mechanism of perceptual filling-in at the blind spot

So far, we have no direct evidence of a causal relationship between the activity of V1 neurons we observed and perceptual filling-in at the blind spot. However, a recent fMRI study in human subjects suggests that indeed neuronal activation in V1 closely correlates with perceptual filling-in at the blind spot (Tong and Engel, 2001). If so, how are neurons in the BS representation involved? One fact that we find particularly interesting is that these neurons are mainly located in layer 6, a

Relation to other completion phenomena

There have been several studies investigating neural correlates of various kinds of perceptual filling-in and completion in the visual cortex. For instance, it has been shown that neural activity in V1 correlates with the perceptual outcome of some phenomena related to filling-in, including brightness induction (Rossi et al., 1996, Kinoshita and Komatsu, 2001) or amodal completion (Sugita, 1999). Other studies, however, have suggested the involvement of higher visual areas in such phenomena as

Acknowledgements

This work is supported by the RFTF (‘Research for the Future’ Program) from the JSPS (The Japan Society for the Promotion of Science; JSPS-RFTF96L00202) and by Grant 08279102 from the Japanese Ministry of Education.

References (27)

  • P. De Weerd et al.

    Perceptual filling-in: a parametric study

    Vision Res.

    (1998)
  • S. He et al.

    Filling-in at the natural blind spot contributes to binocular rivalry

    Vision Res.

    (2001)
  • H. Komatsu et al.

    Surface representation in the visual system

    Cognitive Brain Res.

    (1996)
  • Y.M. Chino et al.

    Receptive-field properties of deafferentiated visual cortical neurons after topographic map reorganization in adult cats

    J. Neurosci.

    (1995)
  • T.N. Cornsweet

    Visual Perception

    (1970)
  • P. De Weerd et al.

    Responses of cells in monkey visual cortex during perceptual filling-in of an artificial scotoma

    Nature

    (1995)
  • M. Fiorani et al.

    Dynamic surrounds of receptive fields in primate striate cortex: a physiological basis for perceptual completion?

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA

    (1992)
  • C.D. Gilbert

    Laminar differences in receptive field properties of cells in cat primary visual cortex

    J. Physiol. (Lond.)

    (1977)
  • C.D. Gilbert et al.

    The projections of cells in different layers of the cat's visual cortex

    J. Comp. Neurol.

    (1975)
  • C.D. Gilbert et al.

    Receptive field dynamics in adult primary visual cortex

    Nature

    (1992)
  • J.H. Kaas et al.

    Reorganization of retinotopic cortical maps in adult mammals after lesions of the retina

    Science

    (1990)
  • M. Kinoshita et al.

    Neural representation of the luminance and brightness of a uniform surface in the macaque primary visual cortex

    J. Neurophysiol.

    (2001)
  • H. Komatsu et al.

    Behavioral evidence of filling-in at the blind spot of the monkey

    Vis. Neurosci.

    (1994)
  • Cited by (35)

    • Central mechanisms of perceptual filling-in

      2019, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences
      Citation Excerpt :

      The same adapting pattern induced multiple colored afterimages depending on the configuration of the test contours [58–61]. Some previous imaging studies indicated a role for retinotopic activity in early visual areas such as V1 in the processing of uniform surfaces [62–64] and also in the processing of patterns presented at the natural blind spot [65–67]. This interpretation has been disputed [68] by showing that surround induced responses in V1, in fact, depend on an extended edge response.

    • Characteristics of the filled-in surface at the blind spot

      2012, Vision Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      This filling-in process is rather remarkable given the relatively large size of the blind spot, which encompasses an ellipse with long and short axes subtending about 6° in width and 8° in height (Armaly, 1969). Neurophysiological studies have demonstrated that the filled-in information is not merely conceptually noted (Dennett, 1992) but is represented by lateral propagation of neural activity (Komatsu, Kinoshita, & Murakami, 2002; Matsumoto & Komatsu, 2005; Pessoa, Thompson, & Noë, 1998). Due to the active propagation of neural activity from the regions surrounding the blind spot, cortical neurons that have receptive fields corresponding to the blind spot can respond to stimulation imaged within the blind spot region.

    • Primary sensory cortices, top-down projections and conscious experience

      2011, Progress in Neurobiology
      Citation Excerpt :

      In assessing the role of the primary visual cortex (V1) in the generation of conscious visual experience, neuroscientists have exploited the fact that experience is not a facsimile of external reality: under certain conditions, what we see is different from what physically impacts the retina. Such conditions include, for instance, visual illusions (von der Heydt et al., 1984; Grosof et al., 1993; Lee and Nguyen, 2001; Komatsu et al., 2002; Sasaki and Watanabe, 2004; Meng et al., 2005; Muckli et al., 2005; Murray et al., 2006; Sterzer et al., 2006; Watkins et al., 2006), masking paradigms (Macknik and Haglund, 1999; Haynes et al., 2005), binocular rivalry (Leopold and Logothetis, 1996; Fries et al., 1997; Sheinberg and Logothetis, 1997; Polonsky et al., 2000; Tong and Engel, 2001; Gail et al., 2004; Lee et al., 2005, 2007; Meng et al., 2005), generalized flash suppression (Wilke et al., 2006; Maier et al., 2008), near-threshold stimuli (which, although present, are not always perceived; Pins and ffytche, 2003; Ress and Heeger, 2003) and situations in which we “see with the mind's eye” (Le Bihan et al., 1993; Kosslyn et al., 1995, 1999; Klein et al., 2000; Harrison and Tong, 2009; Serences et al., 2009). The crucial question, in each case, is whether neural activity in V1 reflects more closely the visual information present in the retina or, instead, the visual mental images of the subject.

    • A new taxonomy for perceptual filling-in

      2011, Brain Research Reviews
      Citation Excerpt :

      Moreover, spatial alignment thresholds in healthy humans are lower across the blind spot than across intact retina (Crossland and Bex, 2009), consistent with involvement of a low-level mechanism in perceptual filling-in at the blind spot. Single cell recordings from anaesthetized monkeys show that when filling-in takes place at the blind spot, neural responses are generated at the retinotopic representation of the blind spot in primary visual cortex (Fiorani et al., 1992; Matsumoto and Komatsu, 2005; Komatsu et al., 2002). Some V1 neurons activated during perceptual filling-in at the blind spot have large receptive fields, extending out of the blind spot (Komatsu et al., 2002), suggesting the passive importing of information from the surrounding visual field.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    1

    Present address: The Rockefeller University, 1230 York Avenue, New York, NY 10021, USA.

    2

    Present address: Human and Information Science Laboratory, NTT Communication Science Laboratories, NTT Corporation, 3-1 Morinosato Wakamiya, Atsugi, Kanagawa 243-0198, Japan.

    View full text