Elsevier

Brain and Language

Volume 124, Issue 1, January 2013, Pages 56-65
Brain and Language

Functional connectivity changes in second language vocabulary learning

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2012.11.008Get rights and content

Abstract

Functional connectivity changes in the language network (Price, 2010), and in a control network involved in second language (L2) processing (Abutalebi & Green, 2007) were examined in a group of Persian (L1) speakers learning French (L2) words. Measures of network integration that characterize the global integrative state of a network (Marrelec, Bellec et al., 2008) were gathered, in the shallow and consolidation phases of L2 vocabulary learning. Functional connectivity remained unchanged across learning phases for L1, whereas total, between- and within-network integration levels decreased as proficiency for L2 increased.

The results of this study provide the first functional connectivity evidence regarding the dynamic role of the language processing and cognitive control networks in L2 learning (Abutalebi et al., 2005, Altarriba and Heredia, 2008, Leonard et al., 2011, Parker-Jones et al., 2011). Thus, increased proficiency results in a higher degree of automaticity and lower cognitive effort (Segalowitz & Hulstijn, 2005).

Highlights

► Twelve healthy adults were trained in an intensive computerized French lexical program. ► Shallow and consolidation learning phases were evaluated by a picture-naming task in fMRI. ► Changes of integration value of language and control networks in L1 and L2 were measured. ► For L1, total, inter and intra integration values remained unchanged across learning phases. ► For L2, they decreased as the level of proficiency improved and cognitive load decreased.

Introduction

Although some parts of the human brain (e.g., Broca’s and Wernicke’s areas) have long been known to be responsible for language processing, it is now believed that language production and comprehension, like many other complex behaviors, are also supported by non-specific circuits. In other words, the language system is viewed as a dynamic system (Liberman, 2000, Liberman, 2003), subserved by a number of regions, which contribute differently according to processing demands. Over the last 20 years, functional neuroimaging studies have focused on determining which brain areas are involved in language production and comprehension.

In a recent review of 100 fMRI studies on speech comprehension and production, Price (2010) lists the areas that showed significant activation in a variety of language comprehension and production tasks, at the word and sentence levels. This review shows that areas involved in language comprehension include the superior temporal gyri bilaterally, the middle and the inferior temporal cortices, the left angular gyrus and pars orbitalis, the superior temporal sulci bilaterally, the inferior frontal regions, the posterior planum temporale, and the ventral supramarginal gyrus. As for language production, the left middle frontal cortex, the left anterior insula, the left putamen, the pre-SMA (Supplementary motor area), the SMA, the motor cortex, the anterior cingulate and the bilateral head of the caudate nuclei are also involved. This review neatly summarizes our understanding of the neurobiology of the language system; however, despite the behavioral, psycholinguistic and neurolinguistic evidence accumulated in recent decades, much remains to be studied about the details of language and the brain.

Specifically with regard to bilingual people, neurocognitive studies on bilingualism have frequently focused on the neural basis of second language processing, as a function of age of acquisition (e.g., Baker & Trofimovich, 2005; ; Bosch and Sebastián-Gallés, 2003, De Diego Balaguer et al., 2005, Doiz and Lasagabaster, 2004, Fabbro, 2001a or Fabbro, 2001b?; Paradis, 2001, Sebastián-Gallés and Echeverría, 2005, Silverberg, 2004), and proficiency attained (Chee et al., 1999, Perani et al., 1998, Yetkin et al., 1996). The results are controversial. Thus, some authors claim that the age of L2 acquisition determines functional organization of L1 and L2 in the brain (Kim, Relkin, Lee, & Hirsch, 1997), whereas others claim that language proficiency is more important than age of acquisition (Perani et al., 1998, Yetkin et al., 1996). Specifically, according to some authors (Chee et al., 1999, Klein et al., 1995, Klein et al., 1995, Perani et al., 1996, Perani et al., 1998), first (L1) and second (L2) languages are supported by common brain areas. Conversely, Kim et al. (1997) argue that this only holds true of early L2 learners.

More recently, it has been argued that the puzzle might be solved by taking proficiency into account. Thus, according to Abutalebi and Green (2007), there is sufficient evidence that both L1 and L2 are represented and processed in the same network (Abutalebi and Green, 2007, Chee et al., 1999, Klein et al., 1995, Klein et al., 1995, Perani et al., 1996, Perani et al., 1998), and that different degrees of activation in the left prefrontal areas for L2 (e.g., Crinion et al., 2006, Frenck-Mestre, 2005; Rodriguez-Fornells, 2005; Raboyeau, Marcotte, Adrover-Roig, & Ansaldo, 2010) can be accounted for by different proficiency levels (Abutalebi & Green, 2007). More specifically, it has been suggested that functional integration between different areas involved in language and cognitive control should vary as proficiency increases (Abutalebi & Green, 2007). Furthermore, Abutalebi and Green point to the need for longitudinal studies to examine changes in connectivity patterns among different regions of interest (ROIs), or a better understanding of changes that may occur during the acquisition of L2.

Functional integration between brain areas can be studied by means of functional connectivity analysis. Functional connectivity allows us to understand how brain areas involved in the processing of specific tasks operate within a system, and how different systems interact within a specific task; functional connectivity has also been related to information flow in the neural system (Anders et al., 2011, Babiloni et al., 2005, Ramnani et al., 2004, Shinkareva et al., 2010). Functional connectivity changes are expressed in terms of functional integration, a measure that characterizes the global integrative state of a network (Marrelec et al., 2008). This approach allows one to examine the dynamic links between the language and control networks involved in L2 vocabulary learning, as proficiency in L2 picture naming increases.

Studies of functional connectivity first appeared rather recently. A few authors have examined the functional connectivity of language networks in healthy monolinguals performing language comprehension tasks (Leff et al., 2008, Van de Ven et al., 2009, Warren et al., 2009) and language production tasks (Bitan et al., 2005; Just, Cherkassky, Keller, & Minshew 2004; van de Ven et al., 2009), whereas others have focused on people with aphasia (Abutalebi et al., 2009, Marcotte et al., 2012, Sonty et al., 2007). Studies of functional connectivity in bilinguals are scarce (Dodel et al., 2005, Majerus et al., 2008, Prat et al., 2007, Veroude et al., 2010). To date, no study has examined the functional connectivity profiles associated with L2 vocabulary learning.

Prat et al. (2007) examined functional connectivity profiles as a function of processing demands in a group of monolinguals who performed a reading task. Based on an fMRI test, subjects were divided into two groups, with either high or low working memory capacity. The results showed greater efficiency, increased adaptability and greater synchronization of the language network for the high-capacity readers, whereas low-capacity readers showed either no reliable differentiation, or a decrease in functional connectivity with increasing demands.

Studies with bilingual populations have mostly focused on the impact of cognitive load (i.e., task difficulty and cognitive capacity) on functional connectivity within the language processing network. Specifically, Dodel et al. (2005) focused on the syntactic processing level, and showed that differences in syntactic proficiency in L2 were associated with differences in the functional connectivity patterns in low- and high-proficiency L2 speakers. The authors used a condition-dependent functional interaction approach, a psychophysiological interaction technique introduced by Friston et al. (1997). This approach allows one to compare two conditions by computing a weighted correlation between the time courses of each pair of regions from a set of pre-determined ROIs. The authors reported that differences observed within these networks were correlated with TOEFL scores, reflecting low or high syntactic proficiency. Hence, this study provides evidence for links between functional connectivity and proficiency at the syntactic level of L2 processing.

In another study, Majerus et al. (2008) examined the links between short-term memory (STM) capacity and bilingual language achievement, in two groups of German–French bilinguals differing in L2 proficiency. They focused on connectivity between the left intra-parietal sulcus and bilateral superior temporal and temporo-parietal areas. Compared to the high-proficient group, the low-proficient group showed enhanced functional connectivity between the latter areas, which the authors interpreted as evidence of poorer storage and learning capacity for verbal sequences in that group.

One shortcoming of these studies is that L2 proficiency (high and low) is measured in different groups of participants, and thus a number of individual factors across groups could influence the connectivity patterns observed. Longitudinal studies with a single group of participants are better suited to measuring proficiency effects and their neurofunctional correlates (Abutalebi & Green, 2007). Moreover, by examining the functional connectivity patterns of networks that are known to contribute to L2 learning, a better understanding of the dynamic roles of the language and cognitive control systems can be achieved.

The aim of the present study is to describe the functional connectivity patterns that characterize L2 vocabulary learning in a group of Persian (L1) speakers who learnt French (L2). The language processing network described by Price (2010) and the control network described by Abutalebi and Green (2007) were identified with a ROI approach. The functional connectivity patterns of these two regions were described at two points in time during the process of learning L2 vocabulary: the shallow phase and the consolidation phase. These patterns were compared to those of the mother tongue, which was tested at both points. No changes in L1 functional connectivity patterns were expected.

Furthermore, in line with the psycholinguistic literature on L2 learning, and with previous functional connectivity studies on motor learning, reading and syntactic processing tasks, it was expected that functional connectivity levels would decrease with increased proficiency. Moreover, in accordance with Abutalebi and Green (2007), it was expected that higher proficiency would result in less effortful, and thus more automatic, processing, reflected in decreased functional integration between the language and control networks.

Section snippets

Experimental design

This was a longitudinal group study, with repeated behavioral, fMRI and functional connectivity measures at two points in time: (a) the shallow phase: after one week of computerized training and a 35% success rate in naming trained items; and (b) the consolidation phase: following 30 days of training and attaining a 100% success rate in naming trained items. Participants completed a pre-experimental assessment of bilingualism and cognitive status before inclusion.

Results

Behavioral data analysis was completed with SPSS 17.0. ARs and RTs for picture naming were calculated at each measure point. A paired-sample t-test was conducted to compare ARs and RTs at the shallow and consolidation phases. The results show that L2 words were named faster (MRT = 1.7, SD = 0.23) and more accurately (MAR = 89.74%, SD = 5.3%) at the consolidation phase (MRT = 2.1, SDRT = 0.32), (MAR = 69.9%, SDAR = 22.85%). The paired-sample t-test showed that there was a significant difference between the two

Discussion

The purpose of the present study was to describe brain connectivity patterns in a group of Persian speakers learning new vocabulary in French. For each measure point (T1 and T2), measures of functional integration (Marrelec et al., 2008) were calculated for the language network (Price, 2010) and the control network (Abutalebi & Green, 2007), and they were compared to those of the mother tongue. It was expected that increased proficiency at T2 would be observed concurrently with decreased

References (83)

  • S. Majerus et al.

    Neural networks for short-term memory for order differentiate high and low proficiency bilinguals

    Neuroimage

    (2008)
  • K. Marcotte et al.

    Default-mode network functional connectivity in aphasia: Therapy-induced Neuroplasticity and Prognosis value

    Neuropsychologia

    (2012)
  • G. Marrelec et al.

    Regions, systems, and the brain: Hierarchical measures of functional integration in fMRI

    Medical Image Analysis

    (2008)
  • G. Marrelec et al.

    Partial correlation for functional brain interactivity investigation in functional MRI

    Neuroimage

    (2006)
  • R.C. Oldfield

    The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory

    Neuropsychologia

    (1971)
  • V. Perlbarg et al.

    CORSICA: correction of structured noise in fMRI by automatic identification of ICA components

    Magnetic Resonance Imaging

    (2007)
  • G. Raboyeau et al.

    Brain activation and lexical learning: The impact of learning phase and word type

    Neuroimage

    (2010)
  • L.C. Scherer et al.

    Syntactic processing in bilinguals: An fNIRS study

    Brain and Language

    (2012)
  • J. Schrouff et al.

    Brain functional integration decreases during propofol-induced loss of consciousness

    Neuroimage

    (2011)
  • V. Van de Ven et al.

    Neural network of speech monitoring overlaps with overt speech production and comprehension networks: A sequential spatial and temporal ICA study

    NeuroImage

    (2009)
  • K. Veroude et al.

    Functional connectivity between brain regions involved in learning words of a new language

    Brain and Language

    (2010)
  • Abutalebi, J., Cappa, S., Perani, D. (2005). In J. F. Kroll, A. M. B. De Greoot (Eds.), Handbook of bilingualism,...
  • Altarriba, J., & Heredia, R. R. (2008). An introduction to bilingualism: Principles and processes (Google eBook)....
  • W. Baker et al.

    Interaction of native- and second-language vowel system(s) in early and late bilinguals

    Language and Speech

    (2005)
  • M.J. Beauchemin et al.

    Increased non-verbal Stroop interference in aging

    Brain and Cognition

    (1996)
  • T. Bitan et al.

    Shifts of effective connectivity within a language network during rhyming and spelling

    Journal of Neuroscience

    (2005)
  • M. Boly et al.

    Hierarchical clustering of brain activity during human non rapid eye movement sleep

    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America

    (2012)
  • L. Bosch et al.

    Simultaneous bilingualism and the perception of a language-specific vowel contrast in the first year of life

    Language and Speech

    (2003)
  • A. Caramazza et al.

    Domain-specific knowledge systems in the brain: The animate–inanimate distinction

    Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

    (1998)
  • M.W.L. Chee et al.

    Mandarian and English single word processing studied with functional magnetic resonance imaging

    The Journal of Neuroscience

    (1999)
  • D. Coynel et al.

    Dynamics of motor-related functional integration during motor sequence learning

    Neuroimage

    (2009)
  • J. Crinion et al.

    Language control in the bilingual brain

    Science

    (2006)
  • R. DeKeyser

    Automaticity and automatization

  • S. Dodel et al.

    Condition-dependent functional connectivity: Syntax networks in bilinguals

    Philosophical Transactions: Biological Sciences

    (2005)
  • A. Doiz et al.

    The effect of the early teaching of English on writing proficiency

    International Journal of Bilingualism

    (2004)
  • M. Favreau et al.

    Automatic and controlled processes in reading a second language

    Memory & Cognition

    (1983)
  • P. Feyereisen et al.

    Gestures and Speech in referential communication by aphasic subjects: Channel use and efficacy

    Aphasiology

    (1988)
  • I. Fischler

    Attention and language

  • J.E. Flege

    Age of learning and second language speech

  • Frenck-Mestre

    Eye-movement recording as a tool for studying syntactic processing in a second language: A review of methodologies and experimental findings

    Second Language Research

    (2005)
  • M.A. Gernsbacher

    Resolving 20 years of inconsistent interactions between lexical familiarity and orthography, concreteness and polysemy

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: General

    (1984)
  • Cited by (39)

    • Nonverbal cognitive control training increases the efficiency of frontal-subcortical collaboration for bilingual language control

      2022, Neuropsychologia
      Citation Excerpt :

      Furthermore, based on our recent study (Wu et al., 2019), we adopted extended unified structure modeling (euSEM) in a confirmatory way to estimate the strength of the connections in the language control network for each participant at the pre-and post-tests. According to the (at least partly) shared mechanisms for bilingual language control and domain-general cognitive control documented in the literature, we expected that only participants in the experimental group would show improved neural efficiency or/and capacity (indexed by less connectivity at comparable/better performance levels or/and more connectivity with better performance levels, respectively; see Ghazi Saidi et al., 2013; Giehl et al., 2020; Heinzel et al., 2014; Karim et al., 2017) in the brain network for bilingual language control. Forty-seven Chinese-English bilinguals were recruited in the present study.

    • Functional and structural neuroplasticity associated with second language proficiency: An MRI study of Chinese-English bilinguals

      2020, Journal of Neurolinguistics
      Citation Excerpt :

      Whether bilingual mental control of two languages may enhance cognitive control has been intensely debated recently (Green, 1998; Green & Abutalebi, 2013; Kroll & Bialystok, 2013; Liu et al., 2019; Paap, Anders-Jefferson, Mason, Alvarado, & Zimiga, 2018; Paap, Johnson, & Sawi, 2015; Sulpizio, Del Maschio, Del Mauro, Fedeli, & Abutalebi, 2020; Wang, Fan, Liu, & Cai, 2016; see Antoniou, 2019; Dong & Li, 2019; Li & Dong, 2020, for recent reviews). However, there has been ample evidence that bilingual experience induces changes in brain function, structure, and networks (e.g., Chai et al., 2016; DeLuca, Rothman, Bialystok, & Pliatskas, 2019; Klein, Mok, Chen, & Watkins, 2014; Mårtensson et al., 2012; Pliatsikas, 2019; Saidi et al., 2013; Qi, Han, Garel, San Chen, & Gabrieli, 2015; Veroude, Norris, Shumskaya, Gullberg, & Indefrey, 2010; Yang, Gates, Molenaar, & Li, 2015; Yang & Li, 2019; see Li, Legault, & Litcofsky, 2014, for review). The past decade has witnessed a rapid growth of neuroimaging studies on L2 learning, yet results have been inconsistent across those studies.

    • Structural brain changes as a function of second language vocabulary training: Effects of learning context

      2019, Brain and Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      Our findings are also consistent with previous findings associating GMV in the left putamen with late L2 learning performance (Abutalebi et al.2013). This is also consistent with fMRI studies of short-term (1 month) L2 training, showing effective L2 learning was associated with neural activity in the putamen (Callan et al., 2003) and functional connectivity between the putamen and other regions of the brain (Ghazi Saidi et al., 2013). Altogether, these findings point to structural changes in the putamen in response to various types of L2 training and experience.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text