From a single decision to a multi-step algorithm

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2012.05.006Get rights and content

Humans can perform sequential and recursive computations, as when calculating 23 × 74. However, this comes at a cost: flexible computations are slow and effortful. We argue that this competence involves serial chains of successive decisions, each based on the accumulation of evidence up to a threshold and forwarding the result to the subsequent step. Such serial ‘programs’ require a specific neurobiological architecture, approximating the operation of a slow serial Turing machine. We review recent progress in understanding how the brain implements such multi-step decisions and briefly examine how they might be realized in models of primate cortex.

Highlights

► We discuss how the brain implements multi-step algorithms. ► We examine how they might be realized in model neural networks. ► Mental algorithms may consist in serial chains of stochastic evidence accumulation. ► Dual-task paradigms point to a serial organization of decision processes. ► Unlike a digital computer, the brain implements an approximate form of seriality.

Section snippets

Introduction: Turing's hunches and rational machines

The mathematician Alan Turing made at least two seminal contributions to computing. First, to help break the German Enigma code, Turing designed a sequential decision algorithm that consists in accumulating votes for the different options and using a threshold criterion to commit to a choice. This framework has become ubiquitous in the psychology and neuroscience of decision making [1, 2]. Second, Turing formalized a sequential computing device, the famous ‘Turing machine’. Although Turing

From isolated decisions to mental programs: the brain's Turing machine

We have proposed a framework for the neural basis of serial computations that puts together Turing's two seminal contributions [4, 5•]. Our framework (Figure 1) assumes that serial tasks (e.g. computing 23 × 14) are assembled as sequences of elementary decision steps, each involving a parallel competition between a subset of ‘productions rules’, implemented by pools of neurons that accumulate relevant evidence. When one of the competing pools encoding a production reaches a threshold, the race

Two theories on how the brain implements multiple decisions

Over the past years, Cisek, Kalaska, Shadlen and others have argued in favor of a parallel decision framework, and questioning the role of an executive system coordinating decision making. Their argument rests on the following observations. First, during a decision process, the same neuron may, at different times, encode stimulus properties, a decision signal, or a motor response [8, 9, 10•]. This argues in favor of distributed instead of centralized organization of decision variables. Second,

Evidence for serial operations in the human brain

A classical psychological observation, the psychological refractory period (PRP), illustrates the limits that human brain architecture imposes on parallel processing. When a human subject is asked to process two near-simultaneous stimuli as fast as possible, a striking seriality emerges: one stimulus is processed with no trace of interference, as if it was presented in isolation, but the response to the second is massively delayed, often by hundreds of milliseconds [17, 18•, 19, 20, 21]. This

Seriality and conscious control

Psychologists have long speculated that serial effortful performance is associated with the deployment of strategies that are consciously controllable and reportable [44, 45]. Indeed, the parieto-prefrontal network that is tied during dual tasking overlaps strongly with the ‘global neuronal workspace’ postulated to underlie conscious reportability [46]. Furthermore, the mechanisms that lead to the postponement of the second task during the PRP are very similar to those that lead to the loss of

Chaining: a missing link

Of particular importance for building a mental program is the capacity to ‘chain’ operations, such that the output of the first one becomes the input to the second. Chaining is an understudied yet essential operation. It implies a flexible neural architecture for ‘routing’ information across any two brain processors, which is likely to involve prefrontal cortex [40, 41, 60, 61].

Using behavioral response-time measurements, Fan et al. [62] demonstrated that in humans, even for identical tasks,

Approximate seriality in a parallel brain

Unlike a digital computer, the brain only implements an approximate form of seriality. Not only do a variety of automatized perceptual and motor operations operate in parallel, but even central decisions may be occasionally subject to partial time sharing [72]. During the PRP, the response to the second item may interfere with the first, suggesting the presence of ‘cross-talk’ [22, 23]. During curve tracing, the successive stages of amplification may partially overlap in visual cortex [69••].

Where is the program? Neural mechanisms for coordinating multiple operations

The precise mechanism by which the executive system organizes serial decisions remains unknown. We emphasize that we do not advocate a homunculus or any dedicated localized structure that will perform such a role. As a first step to understand how executive control is deployed over many parallel cortical processors, we modeled how a network of spiking neurons could implement a sequence of two independent tasks (Figure 4) [5]. In the model, seriality in dual-task performance results as a

Concluding remarks

How the architecture of the brain supports multi-step operations remains largely unexplored. We conclude by pointing to two prominent challenges for further research.

First, dissecting the electrophysiological mechanisms of seriality is hindered by the intrinsic difficulty of training non-human primates in complex multi-step tasks. Indeed, such tasks tap rostral and mesial prefrontal cortical systems [77, 84•] that are more expanded and connected in the human brain than in other primates [85, 86

References and recommended reading

Papers of particular interest, published within the period of review, have been highlighted as:

  • • of special interest

  • •• of outstanding interest

Acknowledgements

Supported by INSERM, CEA, Collège de France, and an ERC grant ‘NeuroConsc’ to S.D. The authors are very grateful to Ariel Zylberberg and Mike Shadlen for stimulating discussions.

References (94)

  • M.N. Tombu et al.

    A unified attentional bottleneck in the human brain

    Proc Natl Acad Sci USA

    (2011)
  • S. Dehaene et al.

    Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: a testable taxonomy

    Trends Cogn Sci

    (2006)
  • S. Dehaene et al.

    A neuronal model of a global workspace in effortful cognitive tasks

    Proc Natl Acad Sci USA

    (1998)
  • J. Duncan

    The multiple-demand (MD) system of the primate brain: mental programs for intelligent behaviour

    Trends Cogn Sci

    (2010)
  • N.I. Posner et al.

    Attention and cognitive control

  • S. Dehaene et al.

    Experimental and theoretical approaches to conscious processing

    Neuron

    (2011)
  • E.K. Vogel et al.

    Electrophysiological evidence for a postperceptual locus of suppression during the attentional blink

    J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform

    (1998)
  • K.F.E. Wong

    The relationship between attentional blink and psychological refractory period

    J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform

    (2002)
  • J. Sackur et al.

    The cognitive architecture for chaining of two mental operations

    Cognition

    (2009)
  • M. Sigman et al.

    Top-down reorganization of activity in the visual pathway after learning a shape identification task

    Neuron

    (2005)
  • S.M. Fleming et al.

    Relating introspective accuracy to individual differences in brain structure

    Science

    (2010)
  • K.R. Gegenfurtner et al.

    Information transfer in iconic memory experiments

    J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform

    (1993)
  • N. Fujii et al.

    Time-varying covariance of neural activities recorded in striatum and frontal cortex as monkeys perform sequential-saccade tasks

    Proc Natl Acad Sci USA

    (2005)
  • J. von Neumann

    The Computer and the Brain

    (1958)
  • A.M. Turing

    On computable numbers, with an application to the Entscheidungsproblem

    Proc London Math Soc

    (1936)
  • A. Zylberberg et al.

    The brain's router: a cortical network model of serial processing in the primate brain

    PLoS Comput Biol

    (2010)
  • C.D. Gilbert et al.

    Brain states: top-down influences in sensory processing

    Neuron

    (2007)
  • D.J. Crammond et al.

    Modulation of preparatory neuronal activity in dorsal premotor cortex due to stimulus-response compatibility

    J Neurophysiol

    (1994)
  • A. Gail et al.

    Implementation of spatial transformation rules for goal-directed reaching via gain modulation in monkey parietal and premotor cortex

    J Neurosci

    (2009)
  • A. Hernandez et al.

    Decoding a perceptual decision process across cortex

    Neuron

    (2010)
  • L.P. Selen et al.

    Deliberation in the motor system: reflex gains track evolving evidence leading to a decision

    J Neurosci

    (2012)
  • J. Requin

    Looking forward to moving soon: ante factum selective processes in motor control

  • M. Roca et al.

    Executive function and fluid intelligence after frontal lobe lesions

    Brain

    (2010)
  • A. Woolgar et al.

    Fluid intelligence loss linked to restricted regions of damage within frontal and parietal cortex

    Proc Natl Acad Sci USA

    (2010)
  • R. Marois et al.

    Capacity limits of information processing in the brain

    Trends Cogn Sci

    (2005)
  • H. Pashler

    Processing stages in overlapping tasks: evidence for a central bottleneck

    J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform

    (1984)
  • A.T. Welford

    The “psychological refractory period” and the timing of high speed performance – a review and a theory

    Br J Psychol

    (1952)
  • C.W. Telford

    The refractory phase of voluntary and associative responses

    J Exp Psychol

    (1931)
  • G.D. Logan et al.

    Parallel memory retrieval in dual-task situations: I. Semantic memory

    J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform

    (2000)
  • G. Hesselmann et al.

    Probing the cortical network underlying the psychological refractory period: a combined EEG-fMRI study

    Neuroimage

    (2011)
  • Y.K. Shin et al.

    Is the psychological refractory period effect for ideomotor compatible tasks eliminated by speed-stress instructions?

    Psychol Res

    (2007)
  • M.C. Lien et al.

    Confirming and disconfirming theories about ideomotor compatibility in dual-task performance: a reply to Greenwald (2005)

    J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform

    (2005)
  • P.E. Dux et al.

    Training improves multitasking performance by increasing the speed of information processing in human prefrontal cortex

    Neuron

    (2009)
  • J.E. Kamienkowski et al.

    Effects of practice on task architecture: combined evidence from interference experiments and random-walk models of decision making

    Cognition

    (2011)
  • M. Sigman et al.

    Parsing a cognitive task: a characterization of the mind's bottleneck

    PLoS Biol

    (2005)
  • K.M. Arnell et al.

    Dissociating sources of dual-task interference using human electrophysiology

    Psychon Bull Rev

    (2004)
  • P. Sessa et al.

    P3 latency shifts in the attentional blink: further evidence for second target processing postponement

    Brain Res

    (2007)
  • Cited by (0)

    View full text