Elsevier

Current Opinion in Neurobiology

Volume 43, April 2017, Pages 166-176
Current Opinion in Neurobiology

The temporal paradox of Hebbian learning and homeostatic plasticity

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2017.03.015Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Hebbian plasticity can be induced rapidly, while homeostatic plasticity is slow.

  • In models of synaptic plasticity this separation of timescales leads to instability.

  • We suggest that Hebbian plasticity is stabilized by rapid compensatory processes.

Hebbian plasticity, a synaptic mechanism which detects and amplifies co-activity between neurons, is considered a key ingredient underlying learning and memory in the brain. However, Hebbian plasticity alone is unstable, leading to runaway neuronal activity, and therefore requires stabilization by additional compensatory processes. Traditionally, a diversity of homeostatic plasticity phenomena found in neural circuits is thought to play this role. However, recent modelling work suggests that the slow evolution of homeostatic plasticity, as observed in experiments, is insufficient to prevent instabilities originating from Hebbian plasticity. To remedy this situation, we suggest that homeostatic plasticity is complemented by additional rapid compensatory processes, which rapidly stabilize neuronal activity on short timescales.

Introduction

More than half a century ago, Donald Hebb [1] laid down an enticing framework for the neurobiological basis of learning, which can be succinctly summarized in the well-known mantra, ‘neurons that fire together wire together’ [2]. However, such dynamics suffers from two inherent problems. First, Hebbian learning exhibits a positive feedback instability: those neurons that wire together will fire together more, leading to even stronger connectivity. Second, such dynamics alone would lead to all neurons in a recurrent circuit wiring together, precluding the possibility of rich patterns of variation in synaptic strength that can encode, through learning, the rich structure of experience. Two fundamental ingredients required to solve these problems are stabilization [3], which prevents runaway neural activity, and competition [4•, 5, 6•], in which the strengthening of a synapse may come at the expense of the weakening of others.

In theoretical models, competition and stability are often achieved by augmenting Hebbian plasticity with additional constraints [5, 7•, 3]. Such constraints are typically implemented by imposing upper limits on individual synaptic strengths, and by enforcing some constraint on biophysical variables, for example, the total synaptic strength or average neuronal activity [6•, 8, 7•, 9, 10, 11, 12]. In neurobiology, forms of plasticity exist which seemingly enforce such limits or constraints through synaptic scaling in response to firing rate perturbations [13, 14], or through stabilizing adjustments of the properties of plasticity in response to the recent synaptic history, a phenomenon known as homeostatic metaplasticity [15, 16, 6•, 11]. Overall, synaptic scaling and metaplasticity, as special cases of homeostatic mechanisms that operate over diverse spatiotemporal scales across neurobiology [17, 18, 19, 20, 21, 22], are considered key ingredients that contribute both stability and competition to Hebbian plasticity by directly affecting the fate of synaptic strength.

The defining characteristic of homeostatic plasticity is that it drives synaptic strengths so as to ensure a homeostatic set point [23, 24•], such as a specific neuronal firing rate or membrane potential. However, it is important that this constraint is implemented only on average, over long timescales, thereby allowing neuronal activity to fluctuate on shorter timescales, so that these neuronal activity fluctuations, which drive learning through Hebbian plasticity, can indeed reflect the structure of ongoing experience. This requisite separation of timescales is indeed observed experimentally; forms of Hebbian plasticity can be induced on the timescale of seconds to minutes [25, 26, 27, 28], whilst most forms of homeostatic synaptic plasticity operate over hours or days [14, 29, 24•]. This separation of timescales, however, raises a temporal paradox: homeostatic plasticity then may become too slow to stabilize the fast positive feedback instability of Hebbian learning. Indeed modeling studies that have attempted to use homeostatic plasticity mechanisms to stabilize Hebbian learning [11, 30, 31•, 32•, 33, 34] were typically required to speed up homeostatic plasticity to timescales that are orders of magnitude faster than those observed in experiments (Figure 1).

This temporal paradox could have two potential resolutions. First, the timescale of Hebbian plasticity, as captured by recent plasticity models fit directly to data from slice experiments [35, 28, 36, 37•, 38], may overestimate the rate of plasticity that actually occurs in vivo. This overestimate could arise from differences in slice and in vivo preps, or because complex nonlinear synaptic dynamics, both present in biological synapses and useful in learning and memory [39, 40, 41], are missing in most, but not all [42, 43, 44], data-driven models. While slow plasticity may be a realistic possibility in cortical areas exhibiting plastic changes over days [45, 46], it may not be a realistic resolution in other areas, like the hippocampus, which must rapidly encode new episodic information [47, 48]. The second potential resolution to the paradoxical separation of timescales between Hebbian and homeostatic plasticity may be the existence of as yet unidentified rapid compensatory processes (RCPs) that stabilize Hebbian learning. Below, we explore both the theoretical utility and potential neurobiological instantiations of these putative RCPs.

Section snippets

The temporal paradox of Hebbian and homeostatic plasticity

To understand the theoretical necessity for RCPs to stabilize Hebbian plasticity, it is useful to view a diversity of synaptic learning models through the unifying lens of control theory (Figure 2a). Here we can view the ‘fire together, wire together’ interplay of neuronal activity and Hebbian synaptic plasticity as an unstable dynamical system. Also, we can view a compensatory process as a feedback controller that observes some aspect of either neuronal activity or synaptic strength, and uses

Putative rapid compensatory processes

What putative RCPs could augment Hebbian plasticity with the requisite stability and competition? Here we focus on several possibilities, operating at either the network, the neuronal or the dendritic level (Figure 4a). However, we note that these possibilities are by no means exhaustive.

At the network level, recurrent or feedforward synaptic inhibition could influence and potentially stabilize plasticity at excitatory synapses directly. For instance, [74] demonstrated via dynamic-clamp that a

Conclusion

The trinity of Hebbian plasticity, competition and stability are presumed to be crucial for effective learning and memory. However, a detailed theoretical and empirical understanding of how these diverse elements conspire to functionally shape neurobiological circuits is still missing. Here we have focused on one striking difference between existing models and neurobiology: the paradoxical separation of timescales between Hebbian and homeostatic plasticity. In models, such a separation of

Conflict of interest statement

Nothing declared.

References and recommended reading

Papers of particular interest, published within the period of review, have been highlighted as:

  • • of special interest

Acknowledgements

FZ was supported by the SNSF (Swiss National Science Foundation). SG was supported by the Burroughs Wellcome, Sloan, McKnight, Simons and James S. McDonnell Foundations and the Office of Naval Research. WG was supported for this work by the European Research Council under grant agreement number 268689 (MultiRules) and by the European Community's Seventh Framework Program under grant no. 604102 (Human Brain Project).

References (117)

  • D.O. Hebb

    The Organization of Behavior: A Neuropsychological Theory

    (1949)
  • C.J. Shatz

    The developing brain

    Sci Am

    (1992)
  • L.F. Abbott et al.

    Synaptic plasticity: taming the beast

    Nat Neurosci

    (2000)
  • N. Rochester et al.

    Tests on a cell assembly theory of the action of the brain, using a large digital computer

    IEEE Trans Inf Theory

    (1956)
  • C. von der Malsburg

    Self-organization of orientation sensitive cells in the striate cortex

    Kybernetik

    (1973)
  • E. Bienenstock et al.

    Theory for the development of neuron selectivity: orientation specificity and binocular interaction in visual cortex

    J Neurosci

    (1982)
  • K.D. Miller et al.

    The role of constraints in Hebbian learning

    Neural Comput

    (1994)
  • E. Oja

    Simplified neuron model as a principal component analyzer

    J Math Biol

    (1982)
  • R. Kempter et al.

    Hebbian learning and spiking neurons

    Phys Rev E

    (1999)
  • S. Song et al.

    Competitive Hebbian learning through spike-timing-dependent synaptic plasticity

    Nat Neurosci

    (2000)
  • L.N. Cooper et al.

    Theory of Cortical Plasticity

    (2004)
  • C.S.N. Brito et al.

    Nonlinear Hebbian learning as a unifying principle in receptive field formation

    PLOS Comput Biol

    (2016)
  • G.G. Turrigiano et al.

    Activity-dependent scaling of quantal amplitude in neocortical neurons

    Nature

    (1998)
  • G.G. Turrigiano

    The self-tuning neuron: synaptic scaling of excitatory synapses

    Cell

    (2008)
  • W.C. Abraham et al.

    Metaplasticity: the plasticity of synaptic plasticity

    Trends Neurosci

    (1996)
  • W.C. Abraham

    Metaplasticity: tuning synapses and networks for plasticity

    Nat Rev Neurosci

    (2008)
  • I. Rabinowitch et al.

    Two opposing plasticity mechanisms pulling a single synapse

    Trends Neurosci

    (2008)
  • G. Turrigiano

    Too many cooks? Intrinsic and synaptic homeostatic mechanisms in cortical circuit refinement

    Annu Rev Neurosci

    (2011)
  • G.G. Turrigiano

    Homeostatic synaptic plasticity: local and global mechanisms for stabilizing neuronal function

    Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol

    (2012)
  • W.B. Cannon

    The Wisdom of the Body

    (1967)
  • G.G. Turrigiano

    The dialectic of Hebb and homeostasis

    Philos Trans R Soc B

    (2017)
  • G.-Q. Bi et al.

    Synaptic modifications in cultured hippocampal neurons: dependence on spike timing, synaptic strength, and postsynaptic cell type

    J Neurosci

    (1998)
  • L.I. Zhang et al.

    A critical window for cooperation and competition among developing retinotectal synapses

    Nature

    (1998)
  • R.C. Froemke et al.

    Spike-timing-dependent synaptic modification induced by natural spike trains

    Nature

    (2002)
  • A.J. Watt et al.

    Homeostatic plasticity and STDP: keeping a neuron's cool in a fluctuating world

    Front Synaptic Neurosci

    (2010)
  • J.-Y. Chen et al.

    Heterosynaptic plasticity prevents runaway synaptic dynamics

    J Neurosci

    (2013)
  • F. Zenke et al.

    Synaptic plasticity in neural networks needs homeostasis with a fast rate detector

    PLoS Comput Biol

    (2013)
  • T. Toyoizumi et al.

    Modeling the dynamic interaction of Hebbian and homeostatic plasticity

    Neuron

    (2014)
  • P. Yger et al.

    Models of metaplasticity: a review of concepts

    Front Comput Neurosci

    (2015)
  • F. Zenke et al.

    Hebbian plasticity requires compensatory processes on multiple timescales

    Philos Trans R Soc B

    (2017)
  • W. Senn et al.

    An algorithm for modifying neurotransmitter release probability based on pre- and postsynaptic spike timing

    Neural Comput

    (2001)
  • J.-P. Pfister et al.

    Triplets of spikes in a model of spike timing-dependent plasticity

    J Neurosci

    (2006)
  • M. Graupner et al.

    Calcium-based plasticity model explains sensitivity of synaptic changes to spike pattern, rate, and dendritic location

    Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A

    (2012)
  • S. Fusi et al.

    Cascade models of synaptically stored memories

    Neuron

    (2005)
  • S. Lahiri et al.

    A memory frontier for complex synapses

    (2013)
  • M.K. Benna et al.

    Computational principles of synaptic memory consolidation

    Nat Neurosci

    (2016)
  • C. Clopath et al.

    Tag-trigger-consolidation: a model of early and late long-term-potentiation and depression

    PLoS Comput Biol

    (2008)
  • A.B. Barrett et al.

    State based model of long-term potentiation and synaptic tagging and capture

    PLoS Comput Biol

    (2009)
  • L. Ziegler et al.

    Synaptic consolidation: from synapses to behavioral modeling

    J Neurosci

    (2015)
  • C.D. Rittenhouse et al.

    Monocular deprivation induces homosynaptic long-term depression in visual cortex

    Nature

    (1999)
  • Cited by (146)

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text