Elsevier

Cortex

Volume 46, Issue 2, February 2010, Pages 242-255
Cortex

Research report
Visual processing in pure alexia: A case study

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2009.03.013Get rights and content

Abstract

Whether pure alexia is a selective disorder that affects reading only, or if it reflects a more general visual disturbance, is highly debated. We have investigated the selectivity of visual deficits in a pure alexic patient (NN) using a combination of psychophysical measures, mathematical modelling and more standard experimental paradigms. NN's naming and categorization of line drawings were normal with regards to both errors and reaction times (RTs). Psychophysical experiments revealed that NN's recognition of single letters at fixation was clearly impaired, and recognition of single digits was also affected. His visual apprehension span was markedly reduced for letters and digits. His reduced visual processing capacity was also evident when reporting letters from words. In an object decision task with fragmented pictures, NN's performance was abnormal. Thus, even in a pure alexic patient with intact recognition of line drawings, we find evidence of a general visual deficit not selective to letters or words. This finding is important because it raises the possibility that other pure alexics might have similar non-selective impairments when tested thoroughly. We argue that the general visual deficit in NN can be accounted for in terms of inefficient build-up of sensory representations, and that this low level deficit can explain the pattern of spared and impaired abilities in this patient.

Introduction

Pure alexia is an acquired disorder of reading characterised by slow and effortful reading of words and text. Patients with pure alexia usually show a linear relationship between the number of letters in a word and the time taken to read it, an effect known as the word length effect (WLE). Other language functions – including writing – are unaffected. Pure alexia can be distinguished from global alexia, where patients are completely unable to identify even single letters (Binder and Mohr, 1992, Leff et al., 2001). Also, although the terms pure alexia and letter-by-letter (LBL) reading are often used interchangeably, they may refer to different entities. Pure alexia or alexia without agraphia refers to an acquired disorder of reading that leaves writing and other language functions intact. WLEs or LBL reading may be observed in patients suffering from other disorders (e.g., Price and Humphreys, 1992 for a discussion, see also Cumming et al., 2006). We use the term pure alexia to refer to a reading disorder in the absence of aphasia and agraphia, and this paper is concerned with theories of pure alexia, and not (necessarily) LBL reading.

Theories of pure alexia are usually divided into (i) alphabetical accounts, attributing the reading deficit to damage in a specialized system for word or letter processing, or the disconnection of this system from visual input (e.g., Cohen et al., 2004), and (ii) visual accounts, suggesting that a deficit that affects visual processing in general is at the core of the disorder (e.g., Behrmann et al., 1998a). As noted by Cumming et al. (2006; p. 1132) regarding these competing accounts, “the jury is still out because as yet relatively few studies of pure alexia have included adequate assessment of non-reading visual tasks”.

A classical view of pure alexia within cognitive neuropsychology is that it results from damage to a word form system, that “parses (multiply and in parallel) letter strings into ordered familiar units and characterizes these units visually” (Warrington and Shallice, 1980, p. 109). A more recent version of this hypothesis proposes that pure alexia arises after damage to an area in the left fusiform gyrus, often referred to as the visual word form area (VWFA), which is thought to be responsible for extracting abstract letter identities (Cohen et al., 2004). Most of the evidence for the existence of the VWFA comes from functional imaging studies of normal subjects, but so far there is little consensus regarding the existence of such an area, or which cognitive operations it may perform (e.g., Price and Devlin, 2003, Price and Devlin, 2004, Cohen and Dehaene, 2004, Starrfelt and Gerlach, 2007). There are some patient studies specifically addressing the selectivity of deficits after focal lesions in this particular brain region (Cohen et al., 2003, Hillis et al., 2005, Gaillard et al., 2006), but their results are inconsistent. The anatomical side of this question is beyond the scope of this paper, as our patient's lesion extends beyond the putative VWFA, but the cognitive issue of the selectivity of pure alexia is addressed by comparing a pure alexic patient's performance with letters, words, and digits, as well as objects.

Another main hypothesis suggests that pure alexia is the result of a deficit in processing many visual items in parallel (simultanagnosia) (Kinsbourne and Warrington, 1962, Farah, 1990). Indeed, pure alexia may even be referred to as ventral simultanagnosia (Duncan et al., 2003, Farah, 2004). Duncan et al. (2003) addressed the simultanagnosia hypothesis of pure alexia rather directly by using psychophysical measures and mathematical modelling – methods also employed in the present study. They found that their pure alexic patient did not have a severe problem with perception of multiple items as such, but showed decreased speed of processing even for single stimuli. Thus, a primary deficit in simultaneous perception did not seem to accurately describe the patient's deficit. However, Duncan et al. (2003) only used letters as stimuli in their experiments. Given that their patient was alexic the results leave open the question of whether the reported pattern of deficits characterizes the patient's visual perception in general. We try to overcome this limitation by measuring our patient's processing speed and visual span of apprehension with two kinds of stimuli – letters and digits. Although letters and digits are similar symbols, number reading can be selectively spared in alexia with agraphia (Anderson et al., 1990, Starrfelt, 2007). Also, it is sometimes assumed that number reading can be spared in pure alexia, as reading of multidigit numbers in free vision has been reported to be preserved in some of these patients (Warrington and Shallice, 1980, Leff et al., 2001). Thus, as a first sensitive test of the selectivity if NN's deficits, we compare his performance with letters and digits in displays of single and multiple stimuli. Following a suggestion by Duncan et al. (2003), we also aim to test how the processing of letters within words may be affected by capacity limitations.

The third main account of pure alexia suggests that a general visual deficit is at the core of this disorder. Behrmann et al. (1998a) have shown that pure alexic patients' object recognition abilities may depend on visual complexity, and that pure alexic patients show perceptual difficulties “under impoverished perceptual conditions where there is less support from organisational cues” (Sekuler and Behrmann, 1996, p. 968). They suggest that reading is one such impoverished condition, and therefore seems disproportionately affected. We address this question in the present study, by investigating NN's picture recognition abilities both under normal and “impoverished” perceptual conditions.

Section snippets

Aims and methods

The theories of pure alexia predict different degrees of selectivity of impairments, and different patterns of performance with non-alphabetical stimuli. We test these predictions in a patient with pure alexia in two series of experiments.

First we compare the patient's performance with letters and digits using a combination of psychophysical experiments and mathematical data modelling. The results are analysed in the framework of the Theory of Visual Attention (TVA: Bundesen, 1990) which

Medical history

NN was 49 years at the time of this experimental investigation. He is a right-handed man (Edinburgh Handedness Inventory – EHI laterality quotient (LQ) = +100, Oldfield, 1971). Following trombolysis-treatment of a lung-embolia on March 24th, 2005, NN suffered a cerebral haemorrhage affecting the posterior left hemisphere. A medullar haemorrhage occurred at the same time, causing a right side paresis, as well as left side paralysis of the lower extremity. Ophthalmological examination revealed no

Experimental investigation

The experimental investigation reported here was conducted during March and April 2007. NN and the control subjects gave informed written consent according to the Helsinki Declaration to participate in the study, and approval was given by the Biomedical research ethics committee in Copenhagen (project no.: KF 01-258988).

NN was tested in three sessions of about 2 h each on separate days. A group of five age and education matched controls (three males), with no history of dyslexia, visual

Discussion

We have described a patient (NN), who suffers from pure alexia after a haemorrhage in the posterior part of the left cerebral hemisphere. NN has no agraphia or other aphasic symptoms. His lesion includes striate cortex, the middle occipital gyrus, as well as the inferior part of the lingual gyrus and the posterior and mid portion of the fusiform gyrus. NN shows a WLE in single word reading of about 270 msec per letter, and his average time to read single words is elevated compared to a group of

Role of the funding source

The first author is supported by the Danish Research Council for the Humanities, and the second author by Copenhagen University's research priority area “Brain and Mind”. Neither had any role in designing or conducting the study, in writing the report, or in deciding to submit the paper for publication.

Acknowledgements

We thank NN for enthusiastic and enduring participation in this study. We are grateful to Hanne Udesen for referring the patient, to Karen-Inge Karstoft for testing four of the control subjects, and to Alex Leff for describing NN's lesion on the MRI images. Fakutsi was indispensable to the first author during this project.

References (54)

  • T. Habekost et al.

    Persisting asymmetries of vision after right side lesions

    Neuropsychologia

    (2006)
  • T. Habekost et al.

    Alexia and quadrant-amblyopia: Reading disability after a minor visual field deficit

    Neuropsychologia

    (2006)
  • A.E. Hillis et al.

    The roles of the “visual word form area” in reading

    NeuroImage

    (2005)
  • R.C. Oldfield

    The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory

    Neuropsychologia

    (1971)
  • C.J. Price et al.

    The myth of the visual word form area

    NeuroImage

    (2003)
  • C.J. Price et al.

    The pro and cons of labelling a left occipitotemporal region: “The visual word form area”

    NeuroImage

    (2004)
  • R. Starrfelt

    Selective alexia and agraphia sparing numbers: A case study

    Brain and Language

    (2007)
  • R. Starrfelt et al.

    The visual what for area: Words and pictures in the left fusiform gyrus

    NeuroImage

    (2007)
  • S.W. Anderson et al.

    Troubled letters but not numbers. Domain specific cognitive impairments following focal damage in frontal cortex

    Brain

    (1990)
  • M. Behrmann et al.

    The evolution of pure alexia: a longitudinal study of recovery

    Brain and Language

    (1990)
  • M. Behrmann et al.

    A literature review and new data supporting an interactive account of letter-by-letter reading

    Cognitive Neuropsychology

    (1998)
  • H. Bergenholtz

    Dansk frekvens ordbog

    (1992)
  • J.R. Binder et al.

    Topography of callosal reading pathways

    Brain

    (1992)
  • J.S. Bowers et al.

    A characterisation of the word superiority effect in a case of letter-by-letter surface alexia

    Cognitive Neuropsychology

    (1996)
  • C. Bundesen

    A theory of visual attention

    Psychological Review

    (1990)
  • C. Bundesen et al.

    A neural theory of visual attention: Bridging cognition and neurophysiology

    Psychological Review

    (2005)
  • L. Cohen et al.

    Visual word recognition in the left and right hemispheres: Anatomical and functional correlates of peripheral alexias

    Cerebral Cortex

    (2003)
  • Cited by (65)

    • Reading and alexia

      2021, Handbook of Clinical Neurology
    • Object recognition and visual object agnosia

      2021, Handbook of Clinical Neurology
    • Disorders of Higher Cortical Visual Function

      2018, Liu, Volpe, and Galetta's Neuro-Ophthalmology: Diagnosis and Management
    • Face perception in pure alexia: Complementary contributions of the left fusiform gyrus to facial identity and facial speech processing

      2017, Cortex
      Citation Excerpt :

      However, this was not found in our prosopagnosic subject, so this cannot be a non-specific general effect of brain damage. Rather, the result in our alexic subjects may parallel findings of other studies of impaired processing in alexia of line drawings of objects (Starrfelt, Habekost, & Gerlach, 2010; Starrfelt, Habekost, & Leff, 2009) or high-contrast line-like non-linguistic stimuli such as kanji characters and checkerboards (Roberts et al., 2013), though again these were all based only on prolonged reaction times rather than both reduced accuracy and prolonged reaction times. Does this difference between linear contour and surface data map onto a difference between high and low spatial frequency processing?

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text