Review
Predicting the future: From implicit learning to consolidation

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.11.012Get rights and content

Abstract

Sequence learning can be differentiated according to phases (rapid and slower), modalities (perceptual and motor), and whether or not it is conscious (implicit and explicit). Implicit sequence learning occurs when information is acquired from an environment of complex stimuli without conscious access either to what was learned or to the fact that learning occurred. In everyday life, this learning mechanism is crucial for adapting to the environment and for predicting events unconsciously. Implicit sequence learning underlies not only motor, but also cognitive and social skills; it is therefore an important aspect of life from infancy to old age. Moreover, this kind of learning does not occur only during practice, in the so-called online periods, but also between practice periods, during the so-called offline periods. The process that occurs during the offline periods is referred to as consolidation, which denotes the stabilization of a memory trace after the initial acquisition; this can result in increased resistance to interference or even improvement in performance following an offline period. Understanding the multiple aspects and influencing factors of consolidation can help us to reveal the nature of memory and changes in brain plasticity. Our review focuses on how consolidation varies with factors such as awareness, the length of offline periods, the type of information to be learned, and the age of participants. We highlight that consolidation is not a single process; instead, there are multiple mechanisms in the offline period, which are differently influenced by these factors.

Highlights

►Predicting subsequent events is one of the most fundamental functions of the brain. ►The most of these predictions are based on implicit learning. ►We summarize the findings regarding consolidation of implicit sequence learning. ►Multiple mechanisms exist in consolidation (e.g., general skill/sequence-specific). ►Influencing factors: awareness, length of delay, perceptual-motor learning and age.

Introduction

Prediction is one of the most fundamental functions of the brain. During every moment of our waking life, the brain is trying to anticipate future sensations. In order to recognize time-based patterns and predict subsequent events, storing and recalling of sequences are required (Hawkins et al., 2009). Without these skills, it would be impossible to carry out evolutionary adaptive behaviors. Most predictions are based on the implicit learning that occurs when information is acquired from an environment of complex stimuli, without conscious access either to what was learned or to the fact that learning occurred (Cleeremans et al., 1998, Reber, 1993). Despite the growing interest in implicit learning in the past decades, there has been relatively little research on offline processing of implicitly learned information (i.e., consolidation). Here, we review recent work on implicit sequence learning and its consolidation, with an emphasis on the last 10 years. More selectively, we address four of the most important factors that influence the consolidation of this fundamental learning mechanism. These factors must be taken into consideration before planning and performing brain imaging, psychophysiological, and behavioral studies on sequence learning and its consolidation.

Implicit sequence learning underlies not only motor, but also cognitive and social skills (Kaufman et al., 2010, Lieberman, 2000, Nemeth et al., 2011, Romano Bergstrom et al., in press, Ullman, 2004); it is therefore an important aspect of life from infancy to old age. Implicit sequence learning is essential for learning languages, as well as learning to operate appliances, computer applications or musical instruments (Howard et al., 2004, Romano et al., 2010). Social skills appear in compound behaviors (including series of perceptions, emotions as well as motor actions) realized in proper sequences and under appropriate circumstances. These skills – for example, dialogs, decision making in social context, communication of emotions, predicting others' behavior based on previous verbal and nonverbal social communication, and adjusting our own behavior based on these predictions – are needed for normal social functioning in various sorts of situations: in the workplace, in the family, in the neighborhood, during recreation, shopping, or in the context of medical and mental care (Heerey and Velani, 2010, Lieberman, 2000, Nemeth and Janacsek, 2011). Furthermore, these skills are crucial for effective participation in educational, training, and rehabilitation programs, for instance in relearning how to walk, reach for objects, and speak after brain injury (Howard et al., 2004, Nemeth et al., 2010a).

Most models and empirical studies of skill learning highlight the role of the basal ganglia and the cerebellum (Dennis and Cabeza, 2011, Doyon et al., 2009a, Hikosaka et al., 1999, Hikosaka et al., 2002, Keele et al., 2003, Kincses et al., 2008, Rieckmann et al., 2010, Sefcsik et al., 2009); in contrast, the role of the hippocampus remains inconclusive (Albouy et al., 2008, Schendan et al., 2003). A major approach to this research is through brain imaging and neuropsychological studies; in addition to these, experiments investigating the effects of pharmacological agents provide an opportunity for the better understanding of the biological background of implicit learning (for review see Uddén et al., 2010). For example, a study by Frank et al. (2006) showed that the benzodiazepine midazolam, which inactivates the hippocampus, causes explicit memory deficits in healthy participants, but enhances implicit learning. In contrast, a more recent study found impaired implicit learning after the exogenous administration of the stress hormone cortisol (Römer et al., 2011). The engagement of specific brain structures in these phenomena needs to be clarified.

In experimental settings, implicit learning is defined as the acquisition of co-occurrence/dependencies between stimuli or trials, and is expressed only through performance (Frensch, 1998, Howard et al., 2004, Rieckmann and Bäckman, 2009). In the past decades, several tasks have been developed to tap into implicit learning. These tasks can be organized into two main groups based on whether the covariation or the temporal sequence of stimuli has predictive information. For example, in artificial grammar learning, participants are exposed to strings of letters. They are not informed that the strings follow a set of rules; yet, it has been found that they can apply these rules at a later stage of practice (Dienes et al., 1991, Reber, 1989). In the weather prediction task, individuals have to decide whether a specific combination of cards predicts rainy or sunny weather. They are unaware that each combination of cards is probabilistically related to a particular weather outcome. During the task, participants learn gradually which of two outcomes will occur, although they have no conscious knowledge of the rule (Gluck et al., 2002, Kemény and Lukács, 2009, Kincses et al., 2004, Poldrack and Rodriguez, 2004). Similarly, in the contextual cueing task, the global configuration of a display cues the location of a search target (Chun and Jiang, 1998, Howard et al., 2006).

In these tasks, the covariation of certain stimuli (e.g., in a letter string/a set of cards) has predictive information, in contrast to sequence learning tasks, where participants have to predict the onset of a stimulus based on the preceding stimuli (Rieckmann and Bäckman, 2009). Evidence suggests that the latter type of task has partly different underlying mechanisms and activates partly different brain structures (Greene et al., 2007, Jimenez and Vázquez, 2011, Poldrack et al., 2005); therefore, it is important to differentiate between these two types of tasks. In recent years, a growing body of data has emerged regarding the consolidation of implicit sequence learning, while covariation learning has received less attention. In our review, we focus on the perceptual–motor learning of sequences. First, we describe the sequence learning tasks in more details. We follow this with a discussion of the consolidation processes and its potentially influencing factors. Finally, we consider developing questions and future directions on this research field.

Section snippets

Measures of implicit sequence learning

A widely used sequence learning task is the finger tapping task (Fig. 1A). Here, participants are instructed to produce a particular sequence of finger movements either on a response box or by opposing their fingers to their thumb (Doyon et al., 2002, Karni et al., 1995). Performance is measured by the number of correctly produced sequences over a certain time interval (e.g., 30 s). Similarly to the previously mentioned tasks, participants' performance becomes better with practice. The main

Consolidation of sequence knowledge

Sequence learning does not occur only during practice, in the so-called online periods, but also between practice periods, during the so-called offline periods. The process that occurs during the offline periods is referred to as consolidation, which denotes the stabilization of a memory trace after the initial acquisition; this can result increased resistance to interference or even improvement in performance following an offline period (Krakauer and Shadmehr, 2006, Nemeth et al., 2010b,

Conclusion and remaining questions

In view of the above, we can conclude that consolidation is not a single process; instead, there are multiple mechanisms in the offline period (e.g. general skill, sequence-specific processes), which are differently influenced by the task demand, awareness of the sequence, the length of the delay period, perceptual and motor factors, and the age of the participant (Table 1). Contradictions in this field can occur due to low or absent control of these factors of sequence learning. For example,

Acknowledgment

Thanks to Gabor Orosz for helpful comments on the manuscript. This review was supported by OTKA K 82068 (Hungarian Scientific Research Fund).

References (121)

  • G. Albouy et al.

    Both the hippocampus and striatum are involved in consolidation of motor sequence memory

    Neuron

    (2008)
  • H. Bearpark et al.

    Cognitive and psychological dysfunction in sleep apnea before and after treatment with CPAP

    Journal of Sleep Research

    (1987)
  • M.A. Bedard et al.

    Obstructive sleep apnea syndrome: pathogenesis of neuropsychological deficits

    Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology

    (1991)
  • M.A. Bedard et al.

    Persistent neuropsychological deficits and vigilance impairment in sleep apnea syndrome after treatment with continuous positive airways pressure (CPAP)

    Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology

    (1993)
  • I.J. Bennett et al.

    Age-related differences in implicit learning of subtle third-order sequential structure

    The Journals of Gerontology. Series B: Psychological Sciences

    (2007)
  • R.M. Brown et al.

    Off-line processing: reciprocal interactions between declarative and procedural memories

    The Journal of Neuroscience

    (2007)
  • R.M. Brown et al.

    Sequence skill acquisition and off-line learning in normal aging

    PLoS One

    (2009)
  • M.M. Chun et al.

    Contextual cueing: implicit learning and memory of visual context guides spatial attention

    Cognitive Psychology

    (1998)
  • A. Cleeremans et al.

    Implicit learning: news from the front

    Trends in Cognitive Sciences

    (1998)
  • N.J. Cohen et al.

    Preserved learning and retention of pattern-analyzing skills in amnesia: dissociation of knowing how and knowing that

    Science

    (1980)
  • A. Cohen et al.

    Attention and structure in sequence learning

    Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition

    (1990)
  • F.I.I. Cosentino et al.

    The APOE e4 allele increases the risk of impaired spatial working memory in obstructive sleep apnea

    Sleep Medicine

    (2000)
  • T. Curran

    Effects of aging on implicit sequence learning: accounting for sequence structure and explicit knowledge

    Psychological Research

    (1997)
  • Debas, K., Carrier, J., Orban, P., Barakat, M., Lungu, O., Vandewalle, G., Tahar, A.H., Bellec, P., Karni, A.,...
  • N. Dennis et al.

    Age-related dedifferentiation of learning systems: an fMRI study of implicit and explicit learning

    Neurobiology of Aging

    (2011)
  • N. Deroost et al.

    Perceptual or motor learning in SRT tasks with complex sequence structures

    Psychological Research

    (2006)
  • Z. Dienes et al.

    Implicit and explicit knowledge bases in artificial grammar learning

    Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning

    (1991)
  • J. Doyon

    Motor sequence learning and movement disorders

    Current Opinion in Neurology

    (2008)
  • J. Doyon et al.

    Experience-dependent changes in cerebellar contributions to motor sequence learning

    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America

    (2002)
  • J. Doyon et al.

    Contributions of the basal ganglia and functionally related brain structures to motor learning

    Behavioural Brain Research

    (2009)
  • J. Doyon et al.

    Contribution of night and day sleep vs. simple passage of time to the consolidation of motor sequence and visuomotor adaptation learning

    Experimental Brain Research

    (2009)
  • H.M. Engelman et al.

    Cognitive function in the sleep apnea/hypopnea syndrome (SAHS)

    Sleep

    (2000)
  • S. Fischer et al.

    Sleep forms memory for finger skills

    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America

    (2002)
  • S. Fischer et al.

    Motor memory consolidation in sleep shapes more effective neuronal representations

    The Journal of Neuroscience

    (2005)
  • S. Fischer et al.

    Developmental differences in sleep's role for implicit off-line learning: comparing children with adults

    Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

    (2007)
  • M.J. Frank et al.

    When memory fails, intuition reigns: Midazolam enhances implicit inference in humans

    Psychological Science

    (2006)
  • S.A. Fraser et al.

    A comparison of motor skill learning and retention in younger and older adults

    Experimental Brain Research

    (2009)
  • P.A. Frensch

    One concept, multiple meanings

  • P.A. Frensch et al.

    Effects of presentation rate and individual differences in short-term memory capacity on an indirect measure of serial learning

    Memory & Cognition

    (1994)
  • J.M. Galea et al.

    Disruption of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex facilitates the consolidation of procedural skills

    Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

    (2010)
  • P. Gerván et al.

    Sleep dependent learning in contour integration

    Journal of Vision

    (2007)
  • M.A. Gluck et al.

    How do people solve the “weather prediction” task?: individual variability in strategies for probabilistic category learning

    Learning & Memory

    (2002)
  • M. Gomez Beldarrain et al.

    Prefrontal lesions impair the implicit and explicit learning of sequences on visuomotor tasks

    Experimental Brain Research

    (2002)
  • M. Gomez Beldarrain et al.

    Sleep improves sequential motor learning and performance in patients with prefrontal lobe lesions

    Clinical Neurology and Neurosurgery

    (2008)
  • T. Goschke

    Implicit learning of perceptual and motor sequences: evidence for independent learning systems

  • N. Gosselin et al.

    Attentional deficits in patients with obstructive sleep apnea syndrome: an event-related potential study

    Clinical Neurophysiology

    (2006)
  • A.J. Greene et al.

    Hippocampal differentiation without recognition: an fMRI analysis of the contextual cueing task

    Learning & Memory

    (2007)
  • Hallgató, E., Gyori-Dani, D., Pekár, J., Janacsek, K., Nemeth, D., in press. The differential consolidation of...
  • J. Hawkins et al.

    Sequence memory for prediction, inference and behaviour

    Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences

    (2009)
  • E.A. Heerey et al.

    Implicit learning of social predictions

    Journal of Experimental Social Psychology

    (2010)
  • K. Henke

    A model for memory systems based on processing modes rather than consciousness

    Nature Reviews. Neuroscience

    (2010)
  • O. Hikosaka et al.

    Parallel neural networks for learning sequential procedures

    Trends in Neurology Science

    (1999)
  • O. Hikosaka et al.

    Central mechanisms of motor skill learning

    Current Opinion in Neurobiology

    (2002)
  • J.A. Hobson

    REM sleep and dreaming: towards a theory of protoconsciousness

    Nature Reviews. Neuroscience

    (2009)
  • A. Hoekema et al.

    Simulated driving in obstructive sleep apnoea–hypopnoea; effects of oral appliances and continuous positive airway pressure

    Sleep & Breathing

    (2007)
  • D.V. Howard et al.

    Age differences in learning serial patterns: direct versus indirect measures

    Psychology and Aging

    (1989)
  • D.V. Howard et al.

    Adult age differences in the rate of learning serial patterns: evidence from direct and indirect tests

    Psychology and Aging

    (1992)
  • J.H. Howard et al.

    Age differences in implicit learning of higher-order dependencies in serial patterns

    Psychology and Aging

    (1997)
  • D.V. Howard et al.

    Implicit sequence learning: effects of level of structure, adult age, and extended practice

    Psychology and Aging

    (2004)
  • J.H. Howard et al.

    Dyslexics are impaired on implicit higher-order sequence learning, but not on implicit spatial context learning

    Neuropsychologia

    (2006)
  • Cited by (0)

    The authors report no conflict of interest and have no financial disclosure.

    View full text