Review
Cognitive benefits of right-handedness: A meta-analysis

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.01.003Get rights and content

Highlights

  • We performed a meta-analysis of verbal and spatial ability in left- vs. right-handers.

  • There was no difference between the full sample of left and right-handers for verbal ability.

  • Subgroup analysis of children showed a small right-hand benefit for verbal ability.

  • We found a modest but significant effect favouring right-handedness for overall spatial ability.

  • An interaction effect with sex is not confirmed for either ability.

Abstract

Hand preference – which is related to cerebral dominance – is thought to be associated with cognitive skills; however, findings on this association are inconsistent and there is no consensus whether left- or right-handers have an advantage in either spatial or verbal abilities. In addition, it is not clear whether an interaction between sex and hand preference exists in relation to these cognitive abilities. As these matters are relevant from a neurodevelopmental perspective we performed a meta-analysis of the available literature. We searched PubMed and Embase, and included 14 studies (359,890 subjects) in the verbal ability meta-analysis and 16 studies (218,351 subjects) in the spatial ability meta-analysis. There was no difference between the full sample of left and right-handers for verbal ability, nor was there a hand preference-by-sex interaction. Subgroup analysis of children showed a small right-hand benefit. Our results further revealed a modest but significant effect favouring right-handedness for overall spatial ability, which was more pronounced when analysis was restricted to studies applying the mental rotation test. We could not identify a specific interaction with sex. Our results indicate that there is a small but significant cognitive advantage of right-handedness on spatial ability. In the verbal domain, this advantage is only significant in children. An interaction effect with sex is not confirmed.

Introduction

Hand preference or ‘handedness’ refers to the phenomenon that most humans use either the right or left hand for unimanual actions, such as writing. Across societies, the vast majority of individuals is right handed and in most countries the prevalence of left-handedness is estimated to be roughly 10 per cent (Gilbert and Wysocki, 1992, McManus, 2009, Peters et al., 2006). Left-handedness has never gone extinct, nor has it reached a majority in any population studied. There is evidence that the frequency of left-handers has been relatively stable at least since the Palaeolithic period (35,000–10,0000 YBP) (Faurie and Raymond, 2004). A sex-difference in handedness is observed, showing a 25% higher prevalence of left-handedness in males (Sommer et al., 2008).

The origins of left-handedness have been studied extensively. Several mechanisms have been investigated, including pre-natal testosterone levels (Geschwind and Galaburda, 1985a, Geschwind and Galaburda, 1985b, Geschwind and Galaburda, 1985c, Medland et al., 2005, Witelson and Nowakowski, 1991), birth trauma (Coren and Porac, 1980) and cultural influences (Medland et al., 2004). Further, genetic mechanisms have been shown to play a role in the development of hand preference, with heritability estimated at 25% (Medland et al., 2009). Always having been a minority, many tales have been told about left-handers.

Left-handedness has been argued to be both disadvantageous and advantageous in literature. For example, left-handedness has been commonly associated with more “sinister” characteristics such as criminal inclination (Bogaert, 2001, Green and Young, 2001), unusual sexual preferences (Bogaert, 2001, Ellis and Ames, 1989), dyslexia (Tønnessen et al., 1993) and stuttering (Kushner, 2012). Until a few decades ago, left-handed children in many societies were forced to use their right-hand for writing as left-handedness was considered an aberrant trait (Hugdahl et al., 1993). It has been suggested that left-handers are at an increased risk for accidents in Western societies (Daniel and Yeo, 1994, Halpern and Coren, 1991), because the industrial environments in these settings are designed for right-handers (Coren and Halpern, 1991, Porac and Cohen, 1981). The most notorious association with left-handedness, shorter life expectancy, was found to be an incorrect association (Aggleton et al., 1994, Hicks et al., 1994, McManus, 2009), as it was a subtle increase in the prevalence of left-handedness in the twentieth century, and not an earlier death, that has caused the relative paucity of elderly left-handers. In contrast to these (often false) accusations, several positive qualities have also been attributed to left-handedness. For example, the consistent presence of left-handedness over time and across societies has been attributed to an evolutionary benefit for this trait. One model to explain such a benefit is frequency dependent selection. This refers to an advantage of the minority group within a population due to a certain trait or skill. The advantage depends on the frequency of these individuals and disappears if their numbers increase (Vallortigara and Rogers, 2005). Raymond et al. proposed that left-handers have a frequency dependent advantage in fighting interaction, which may have resulted in increased fitness. Indeed, a higher proportion of left-handers was found in sports that reflected fighting interaction (Billiard et al., 2005, Raymond et al., 1996). Additional examples of a left-hand benefit include (inconsistent) findings of left-handers to be more talented in fast ball sports (Holtzen, 2000, Wood and Aggleton, 1989), have better musical skills (Byrne, 1974, Oldfield, 1969) and higher income earnings compared to right-handers (Ruebeck et al., 2007).

Of particular interest is the possibility of a cognitive benefit for left-handers. For example, popular belief still holds the left-handed to be more intelligent and creative (see for example: http://www.independent.co.uk/life-style/health-and-families/health-news/lefthanders-more-creative-but-forgetful-710626.html for an article in the press that describes great advantages of left-handedness). Within the cognitive domain, research from the fields of neuroscience and psychology has focused on the association of hand-preference with specific cognitive skills. Of all cognitive skills, spatial and verbal ability have been the most extensively investigated (e.g. Annett and Turner, 1974, Burnett et al., 1982, Crow et al., 1998, Hardyck et al., 1976, Mc Gee, 1976). However, the debate as to whether a difference between left and right-handers exists remains to be resolved, despite numerous studies comparing verbal and spatial abilities of left and right-handed individuals. The importance of this unresolved issue lies in the fact that conclusive results might shape policies related to handedness, for example with regard to in- or exclusion of left-handers in scientific research. Further, if left-handers do indeed show a specific cognitive benefit or deficit, this may have some implications; for example, in educational settings, it might be helpful to build in support structures to help left-handed children strengthen certain skills.

In most humans the neural representation of verbal and spatial ability is lateralized to either of the two cerebral hemispheres (Szaflarski et al., 2006). This phenomenon, cerebral lateralization, correlates with hand preference, though this is not a strong association. Up to 95% of right-handed people display left-cerebral dominance for language functions (Szaflarski et al., 2006). In left-handed individuals, about 75% show left-cerebral dominance, 10% show right dominance and 15% show a bilateral pattern of language lateralization (Jesús Pujol et al., 1999). Although the relationship between hand preference and spatial lateralization has not been extensively investigated (Jesús Pujol et al., 1999) a meta-analysis showed that the right-hemisphere controls spatial tasks in right-handers, while no hemispheric preference was found in left-handers (Vogel et al., 2003). The differences in lateralization patterns of cognitive functions may underlie a benefit or deficit pertaining to either spatial or verbal ability in left-handers.

Levy was the first to propose that left-handers may have a deficit in spatial function as measured by Performal IQ (PIQ) (Levy, 1969). She suggested that left-handers would have more right-hemispheric language function, which competes with spatial functions for neural resources in the right hemisphere, and consequently impedes spatial abilities. Several subsequent studies, however, were unable to replicate this finding (Briggs et al., 1976, Fagan-Dubin, 1974, Inglis and Lawson, 1984). Some studies found left-handers performing equally well as right-handers (Newcombe and Ratcliff, 1973), whereas other studies found left-handers having lower spatial, but higher verbal ability (McKeever, 1986). Most studies have assessed handedness as a dichotomous trait, but different associations emerge when hand preference is considered as a trichotomous trait (Peters, 1998) or as sub-classified trait as in the Annett 7–8 group model (Annett, 2004, Crow et al., 1998). For example, one study demonstrated that extreme left- and right-handers had lower spatial skills compared to mixed- and moderately strong right-handers (Burnett et al., 1982). In contrast, in a large cohort study of British school children, Crow et al. (1998) showed that subjects displaying equal skills with both hands had slightly lower intellectual ability (specifically on verbal and mathematical skills) in comparison to strong left- and right-handers, while the latter two groups did not differ substantially. This finding was partially replicated in a large internet survey which did not identify any significant differences between left- and right-handers on verbal or spatial ability (Peters et al., 2006). Subjects who lacked a clear preference for the right or left hand performed significantly lower on a test of mental rotation, but not on verbal ability. Although several studies did not find differences in verbal ability between left- or right handers (e.g. Annett and Turner, 1974, Eme et al., 1978, Hardyck et al., 1976, Kocel, 1977), one very large study (n = 174,547) showed that left-handers had higher verbal reasoning skills and were over-represented in the upper tail of Medical College Admission test scores.

A number of studies have identified a small but consistent sex effect on certain spatial tasks in which men tend to outperform women, particularly on the mental rotation task (Koscik et al., 2009, Voyer et al., 1995). Although sex-effects in verbal ability are less consistently reported, there is some evidence that women outperform men with regard to specific verbal skills (e.g. verbal fluency) (Gordon and Lee, 1986) and perhaps on verbal skills (Crow et al., 1998, Hyde and Linn, 1988). Previous studies that have specifically investigated cognitive benefits or disadvantages in left-handers between males and females have been inconsistent: left-handed men were found to show poorer spatial skills than right-handed men, while in the same study, no handedness effect for women was found (Snyder and Harris, 1993). Another study found that left-handed women had poorer spatial skills compared to right-handed women, while the reverse was observed in men (Mc Gee, 1976). Thus, the actual relation between spatial ability, verbal ability, and hand-preference remains unclear in between the sexes.

In order to investigate the presence of a possible benefit or deficit for lateralized skills in left-handers compared to right-handers, we conducted a meta-analysis on spatial and verbal ability in relation to hand preference. Despite findings that advocate for the non-dichotomous division of handedness (Annett, 2004) or consider handedness as a quantitative trait (Van Agtmael et al., 2003), this meta-analysis focuses on a left-right subdivision in order to include the large body of older literature that has primarily investigated hand preference as a dichotomous trait. With this meta-analysis, we aim to elucidate whether a difference in spatial or verbal ability exists between left- and right-handers. In addition, we aim to assess any associations between handedness and cognitive performance within the sexes.

Section snippets

Search criteria

We conducted a systematic search in PubMed and EMBASE from 1966 to December 2011. Combinations of the following search terms were used: ‘left-handedness’, ‘hand preference’, ‘cognitive ability’, ‘spatial ability’, and ‘verbal ability’. In addition, reference lists of journal articles were searched for any additional studies, which our searches might have missed. Our search was limited to articles in English. By convention, we required at least three suitable studies to perform a meta-analysis

Included articles

A total of 191 journal articles were identified that assessed hand preference in relation to verbal ability and/or spatial ability or cognitive function in general. Of these, 14 studies with a total of 359,890 subjects met inclusion criteria for the verbal ability meta-analysis (for an overview, see Table 2) and 16 studies with a total of 218,351 subjects met inclusion criteria for the spatial ability meta-analysis (for an overview, see Table 3).

Overall verbal ability

The analysis of ‘overall verbal ability’ included

Discussion

This meta-analysis investigated the association of verbal and spatial abilities with handedness. We could include 14 studies for the analysis of ‘overall verbal ability’, reporting on 316,986 right-handed and 42,904 left-handed individuals. We found no significant effect of hand preference on overall verbal ability. A sensitivity analysis without the large Peters et al. (2006) and Halpern et al. (1998) studies yielded the same result. The sub analysis for children, informed by results from the

Studies included in the verbal ability meta-analysis (see Table 2)

Annett and Turner (1974), Kocel (1977), Hicks and Beveridge (1978), Eme et al. (1978), Sherman (1979), Gordon and Kravetz (1991), Van Strien and Bouma (1995), Halpern et al. (1998), Crow et al. (1998), Natsopoulos et al. (1998), Coulson and Lovett (2004), Tremblay et al. (2004), Peters et al. (2006) and Bradshaw et al. (1981)

Studies included in the spatial ability meta-analysis (see Table 3)

Yen (1975), Mc Gee (1976), Gilbert (1977), Kocel (1977), Sherman (1979), Eme et al. (1978), Gregory et al. (1980), Gordon and Kravetz (1991), Natsopoulos et al. (1992), Snyder and Harris (1993), Karapetsas and Vlachos (1997), Van Strien (2002), Peters et al. (2006), Bradshaw et al. (1981), Freedman and Rovegno (1981) and Herrmann and Dyke (1978).

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to Dr. M. Peters and Dr. S.J. Leask for kindly providing us with data that could be used for meta-analysis

References (102)

  • C. Gilbert

    Non-verbal perceptual abilities in relation to left-handedness and cerebral lateralization

    Neuropsychologia

    (1977)
  • H.W. Gordon et al.

    The influence of gender, handedness, and performance level on specialized cognitive functioning

    Brain Cogn.

    (1991)
  • H.W. Gordon et al.

    A relationship between gonadotropins and visuospatial function

    Neuropsychologia

    (1986)
  • R.J. Gregory et al.

    Left-handedness and spatial reasoning abilities: the deficit hypothesis revisited

    Intelligence

    (1980)
  • D.F. Halpern et al.

    Handedness and sex differences in intelligence: evidence from the Medical College Admission Test

    Brain Cogn.

    (1998)
  • C. Hardyck et al.

    Left-handedness and cognitive deficit

    Cortex

    (1976)
  • D. Herrmann et al.

    Handedness and the mental rotation of perceived patterns

    Cortex

    (1978)
  • R.A. Hicks et al.

    Handedness and intelligence

    Cortex

    (1978)
  • K. Hugdahl et al.

    Left-handedness and old age: do left-handers die earlier?

    Neuropsychologia

    (1993)
  • J. Inglis et al.

    Handedness, sex and intelligence

    Cortex

    (1984)
  • T. Koscik et al.

    Sex differences in parietal lobe morphology: relationship to mental rotation performance

    Brain Cogn.

    (2009)
  • J.M. Lust et al.

    Sex specific effect of prenatal testosterone on language lateralization in children

    Neuropsychologia

    (2010)
  • R.S. Marshall et al.

    Changes in regional cerebral blood flow related to line bisection discrimination and visual attention using HMPAO-SPECT

    Neuroimage

    (1997)
  • W.F. McKeever

    The influences of handedness, sex, familial sinistrality and androgyny on language laterality, verbal ability, and spatial ability

    Cortex

    (1986)
  • S.E. Medland et al.

    Genetic influences on handedness: data from 25,732 Australian and Dutch twin families

    Neuropsychologia

    (2009)
  • D. Natsopoulos et al.

    Do the hands talk on mind's behalf? Differences in language ability between left- and right-handed children

    Brain Lang.

    (1998)
  • F. Newcombe et al.

    Handedness, speech lateralization and ability

    Neuropsychologia

    (1973)
  • M.W. O’Boyle et al.

    Mathematically gifted male adolescents activate a unique brain network during mental rotation

    Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res.

    (2005)
  • S. Ocklenburg et al.

    Handedness: a neurogenetic shift of perspective

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (2013)
  • A. Oh et al.

    The role of the insula in speech and language processing

    Brain Lang.

    (2014)
  • R.C. Oldfield

    The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory

    Neuropsychologia

    (1971)
  • M. Peters et al.

    Hand preference for writing and associations with selected demographic and behavioral variables in 255,100 subjects: the BBC internet study

    Brain Cogn.

    (2006)
  • J.L. Powell et al.

    Association between language and spatial laterality and cognitive ability: an fMRI study

    Neuroimage

    (2012)
  • R. Rosenthal

    The file drawer problem and tolerance for null results

    Psychol. Bull.

    (1979)
  • P.J. Snyder et al.

    Handedness, sex, and familial sinistrality effects on spatial tasks

    Cortex

    (1993)
  • I.E. Sommer et al.

    Sex differences in handedness, asymmetry of the Planum Temporale and functional language lateralization

    Brain Res.

    (2008)
  • T. Tremblay et al.

    Phonological processing of words in right- and left-handers

    Brain Cogn.

    (2004)
  • J.W. Van Strien et al.

    Sex and familial sinistrality differences in cognitive-abilities

    Brain Cogn.

    (1995)
  • M. Vigneau et al.

    Meta-analyzing left hemisphere language areas: phonology, semantics, and sentence processing

    Neuroimage

    (2006)
  • J.J. Vogel et al.

    Cerebral lateralization of spatial abilities. A meta-analysis

    Brain Cogn.

    (2003)
  • R. Westerhausen et al.

    Effects of handedness and gender on macro- and microstructure of the corpus callosum and its subregions: a combined high-resolution and diffusion-tensor MRI study

    Cogn. Brain Res.

    (2004)
  • S.F. Witelson et al.

    Left out axons make men right: a hypothesis for the origin of handedness and functional asymmetry

    Neuropsychologia

    (1991)
  • J.P. Aggleton et al.

    Handedness and longevity: archival study of cricketers

    Br. Med. J.

    (1994)
  • Altman, D., Duval, S., Lipsey, M., Pigott, T., n.d. Comprehensive Meta Analysis Version...
  • M. Annett

    A classification of hand preference by association analysis

    Br. J. Psychol.

    (1970)
  • M. Annett

    Hand preference observed in large healthy samples: classification, norms and interpretations of increased non-right-handedness by the right shift theory

    Br. J. Psychol.

    (2004)
  • M. Annett et al.

    Laterality and the growth of intellectual abilities

    Br. J. Educ. Psychol.

    (1974)
  • J.R. Binder et al.

    Human brain language areas identified by functional magnetic resonance imaging

    J. Neurosci.

    (1997)
  • G.G. Briggs et al.

    Intellectual differences in relation to personal and family handedness

    Q. J. Exp. Psychol.

    (1976)
  • P.J. Bryden et al.

    A developmental analysis of the relationship between hand preference and performance: II. A performance-based method of measuring hand preference in children

    Brain Cogn.

    (2000)
  • Cited by (71)

    • The relation between mental rotation and handedness is a consequence of how handedness is measured

      2019, Brain and Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      For all kinds of handedness and hand performance measures we found the typical increase in reaction time with increasing angular disparity (Shepard & Metzler, 1971). In contrast to Somers et al. (2015), we found no general advantage for right-handers in mental rotation tasks. However, most of the studies in their meta-analysis used different forms of paper-and-pencil tests instead of chronometric mental rotation tasks.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text