Elsevier

Neurobiology of Aging

Volume 34, Issue 9, September 2013, Pages 2164-2174
Neurobiology of Aging

Regular article
Distinct manifestations of executive dysfunction in aged rats

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.03.019Get rights and content

Abstract

Different components of executive function such as working memory, attention, and cognitive flexibility can be dissociated behaviorally and mechanistically; however, the within-subject influences of normal aging on different aspects of executive function remain ill-defined. To better define these relationships, young adult and aged male F344 rats were cross-characterized on an attentional set-shifting task that assesses cognitive flexibility and a delayed response task that assesses working memory. Across tasks, aged rats were impaired relative to young; however, there was significant variability in individual performance within the aged cohort. Notably, performance on the set-shifting task and performance at long delays on the delayed response task were inversely related among aged rats. Additional experiments showed no relationship between aged rats' performance on the set-shifting task and performance on a hippocampal-dependent spatial reference memory task. These data indicate that normal aging can produce distinct manifestations of executive dysfunction, and support the need to better understand the unique mechanisms contributing to different forms of prefrontal cortical-supported executive decline across the lifespan.

Introduction

Across species, aging is accompanied by a decline in neurocognitive functions, including learning and memory mediated by medial temporal lobe structures and executive functions mediated by prefrontal cortex (PFC; Alexander et al., 2012; Bizon et al., 2012; Buckner, 2004). Research in animal models has made considerable strides in understanding the neural basis of age-related decline in learning and memory (Burke et al., 2012; Engle and Barnes, 2012; Foster et al., 2012); however, there has been less progress in understanding the neural mechanisms that contribute to impaired executive functioning across the lifespan. This relative paucity of data stems in part from the complexity in defining the distinct cognitive processes that are subserved by the PFC and our still limited understanding of how these processes integrate to effectively organize and guide behavior. Executive functions have been operationalized in a variety of ways but can include attention, planning, cognitive flexibility, working memory, inhibitory control, and decision-making (Fuster, 2000; Glisky, 2007; Kesner and Churchwell, 2011; Miller and Cohen, 2001; Robbins, 1996). Among these processes, age-related decline in working memory and cognitive flexibility are particularly well described. Though many definitions for working memory exist, this term is most often used in reference to the maintenance of a representation “in mind” of a stimulus that is no longer present in the environment (e.g., Goldman-Rakic, 1996). In contrast, cognitive or behavioral flexibility refers to the ability to effectively update internal representations and shift behavioral responses to accommodate changes in environmental contingencies (e.g., Dias et al., 1996).

Cognitive flexibility can be assessed in primates and rodents using “set-shifting” tasks. The prototypical set-shifting task, designed for human subjects, is the Wisconsin Card Sorting task (Berg, 1948), in which subjects are required to sort a deck of cards that contain multiple stimulus features (e.g., shape and color). Subjects must initially learn through trial and error which stimulus feature governs the correct choice (e.g., red indicates correct choice, ignore shape). After acquisition of this rule, an unsignaled ‘shift’ occurs such that the external contingencies are altered and the subjects must now inhibit the initial rule and shift their response strategies to accommodate the new contingencies (e.g., ignore color, square signals correct choice). Analogues of the Wisconsin Card Sorting task have been developed for use in nonhuman primates and rodents, and across species, damage to the dorsolateral PFC or its rodent homologue, medial PFC (mPFC), does not affect acquisition of the initial rule but selectively impairs the ability to set-shift (Birrell and Brown, 2000; Bissonette et al., 2008; Darrah et al., 2008; Demakis, 2003; Dias et al., 1996; Floresco et al., 2008; Owen et al., 1991; Ragozzino, 2007; Ragozzino et al., 1999; Uylings et al., 2003). Working memory tasks are generally designed such that to-be-remembered information varies across trials, requiring active resistance to proactive interference and distraction. Working memory is commonly assessed using delayed response tasks in which subjects are required to remember information about a spatial location over some delay interval, and then to accurately recall that information in a choice setting. As with set-shifting tasks, performance on working memory tasks is impaired after damage to primate dorsolateral or rodent medial PFC, and such lesions tend to disproportionately affect performance at long delays (Floresco et al., 1997; Freedman and Oscar-Berman, 1986; Goldman and Rosvold, 1970; Mishkin, 1957; Ragozzino et al., 1998).

Previous work in humans, nonhuman primates, and rodents has shown that cognitive flexibility and working memory decline across the lifespan. Notably, however, there is considerable variability among aged subjects, such that some perform on par with young whereas others demonstrate varying degrees of impairment (Barense et al., 2002; Bizon et al., 2009; Gallagher et al., 1993; Glisky, 2007; Morrison and Baxter, 2012; Park, 2000; Robbins et al., 1998). Despite such well-documented individual differences, the relationship between the presence and severity of impairment on tasks that assay these different components of executive function is not well defined. The fact that PFC damage impairs working memory and set-shifting performance, and that both functions are compromised in disease states such as schizophrenia, suggest that age-related impairments in working memory and cognitive flexibility are mediated by common neural mechanisms and might be expected to covary (Chudasama and Robbins, 2006). However, these processes can also be dissociated using a variety of PFC manipulations that include modulation of dopaminergic and GABAergic signaling, both of which can be compromised at advanced ages (Cools and D'Esposito, 2011; Durstewitz and Seamans, 2008; Enomoto et al., 2011; Floresco and Magyar, 2006; Li et al., 2010; McQuail et al., 2012). A primary goal of the current study was to determine the relationship between age-related impairments in cognitive flexibility (assessed using a set-shifting task) and working memory (assessed using a delayed response task) in aged Fischer 344 rats. In addition, performance on the set-shifting task was compared with performance on the Morris water maze, an assay that is sensitive to medial temporal lobe-mediated mnemonic dysfunction in aged rats (Bizon et al., 2009; Frick et al., 1995; Gallagher et al., 1993).

Section snippets

Subjects

Young (5 months old; n = 20) and aged (22 months old; n = 25) male Fischer 344 rats were obtained from the National Institute on Aging colony (Taconic Farms, Hudson, NY, USA) and housed in the Association for Assessment and Accreditation of Laboratory Animal Care International (AAALAC)-accredited vivarium facility in the McKnight Brain Institute Building at University of Florida in accordance with the rules and regulations of the University of Florida Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee

Experiment 1: effects of aging on set-shifting

Young and aged rats required comparable numbers of sessions in the two stages of shaping which included a performance criterion (stage 2: mean [standard error of the mean (SEM)], young = 2.90 [0.22], aged = 2.64 [0.18], t(43) = 0.93, p = 0.36; stage 3: young = 5.40 [0.57], aged = 5.32 [0.46], t(43) = 0.11, p = 0.93). Likewise, there were no differences between young and aged rats in the number of trials required to reach criterion performance on the initial (visual) discrimination (t(43) =

Discussion

The overarching goal of these experiments was to determine the relationship between age-associated performance deficits in 2 aspects of PFC-dependent executive functions: set-shifting and working memory. In agreement with previous population-based studies in humans, nonhuman primates, and rodents (Barense et al., 2002; Bizon et al., 2009, 2012; Glisky, 2007; Mizoguchi et al., 2009; Morrison and Baxter, 2012; Park, 2000; Ramos et al., 2003; Robbins et al., 1998; Segovia et al., 2008), aged rats

Disclosure statement

The authors have no actual or potential conflicts of interest to disclose. All work was conducted in accordance with the rules and regulations of the University of Florida Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee and National Institutes of Health guidelines.

Acknowledgements

Supported by R01AG029421 and the McKnight Brain Research Foundation (JLB), and a NSF Graduate Research Fellowship (BSB).

References (78)

  • G.E. Alexander et al.

    Characterizing cognitive aging in humans with links to animal models

    Front. Aging Neurosci.

    (2012)
  • A.F. Arnsten et al.

    Dopamine's influence on prefrontal cortical cognition: actions and circuits in behaving primates

  • A.F. Arnsten et al.

    Neuromodulation of thought: flexibilities and vulnerabilities in prefrontal cortical network synapses

    Neuron

    (2012)
  • L. Ashendorf et al.

    Exploring age-related decline on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test

    Clin. Neuropsychol.

    (2008)
  • L. Backman et al.

    Linking cognitive aging to alterations in dopamine neurotransmitter functioning: recent data and future avenues

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (2010)
  • C. Banuelos et al.

    Age-related changes in rostral basal forebrain cholinergic and GABAergic projection neurons: relationship with spatial impairment

    Neurobiol. Aging

    (2013)
  • M.D. Barense et al.

    Aged rats are impaired on an attentional set-shifting task sensitive to medial frontal cortex damage in young rats

    Learn. Mem.

    (2002)
  • E.A. Berg

    A simple objective technique for measuring flexibility in thinking

    J. Gen. Psychol.

    (1948)
  • J.M. Birrell et al.

    Medial frontal cortex mediates perceptual attentional set shifting in the rat

    J. Neurosci.

    (2000)
  • G.B. Bissonette et al.

    Double dissociation of the effects of medial and orbital prefrontal cortical lesions on attentional and affective shifts in mice

    J. Neurosci.

    (2008)
  • J.L. Bizon et al.

    Characterizing cognitive aging of working memory and executive function in animal models

    Front. Aging Neurosci.

    (2012)
  • J.L. Bizon et al.

    Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis function and corticosterone receptor expression in behaviourally characterized young and aged Long-Evans rats

    Eur. J. Neurosci.

    (2001)
  • J.L. Bizon et al.

    Spatial reference and working memory across the lifespan of male Fischer 344 rats

    Neurobiol. Aging

    (2009)
  • T.J. Brozoski et al.

    Cognitive deficit caused by regional depletion of dopamine in prefrontal cortex of rhesus monkey

    Science

    (1979)
  • R.L. Buckner

    Memory and executive function in aging and AD: multiple factors that cause decline and reserve factors that compensate

    Neuron

    (2004)
  • S.N. Burke et al.

    Characterizing cognitive aging of recognition memory and related processes in animal models and in humans

    Front. Aging Neurosci.

    (2012)
  • Y. Chudasama et al.

    Functions of frontostriatal systems in cognition: comparative neuropsychopharmacological studies in rats, monkeys and humans

    Biol. Psychol.

    (2006)
  • L.S. Colzato et al.

    The flexible mind is associated with the catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) Val158Met polymorphism: evidence for a role of dopamine in the control of task-switching

    Neuropsychologia

    (2010)
  • R. Cools et al.

    Inverted-U-shaped dopamine actions on human working memory and cognitive control

    Biol. Psychiatry

    (2011)
  • H.S. Crofts et al.

    Differential effects of 6-OHDA lesions of the frontal cortex and caudate nucleus on the ability to acquire an attentional set

    Cereb. Cortex

    (2001)
  • J.M. Darrah et al.

    Interaction of N-methyl-D-aspartate and group 5 metabotropic glutamate receptors on behavioral flexibility using a novel operant set-shift paradigm

    Behav. Pharmacol.

    (2008)
  • G.J. Demakis

    A meta-analytic review of the sensitivity of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test to frontal and lateralized frontal brain damage

    Neuropsychology

    (2003)
  • R. Dias et al.

    Dissociation in prefrontal cortex of affective and attentional shifts

    Nature

    (1996)
  • D. Durstewitz et al.

    The dual-state theory of prefrontal cortex dopamine function with relevance to catechol-o-methyltransferase genotypes and schizophrenia

    Biol. Psychiatry

    (2008)
  • J.R. Engle et al.

    Characterizing cognitive aging of associative memory in animal models

    Front. Aging Neurosci.

    (2012)
  • T. Enomoto et al.

    Reducing prefrontal gamma-aminobutyric acid activity induces cognitive, behavioral, and dopaminergic abnormalities that resemble schizophrenia

    Biol. Psychiatry

    (2011)
  • S.B. Floresco et al.

    Inactivation of the medial prefrontal cortex of the rat impairs strategy set-shifting, but not reversal learning, using a novel, automated procedure

    Behav. Brain Res.

    (2008)
  • S.B. Floresco et al.

    Mesocortical dopamine modulation of executive functions: beyond working memory

    Psychopharmacology (Berl.)

    (2006)
  • S.B. Floresco et al.

    Selective roles for hippocampal, prefrontal cortical, and ventral striatal circuits in radial-arm maze tasks with or without a delay

    J. Neurosci.

    (1997)
  • T.C. Foster et al.

    Characterizing cognitive aging of spatial and contextual memory in animal models

    Front. Aging Neurosci.

    (2012)
  • M. Freedman et al.

    Bilateral frontal lobe disease and selective delayed response deficits in humans

    Behav. Neurosci.

    (1986)
  • K.M. Frick et al.

    Age-related spatial reference and working memory deficits assessed in the water maze

    Neurobiol. Aging

    (1995)
  • J.M. Fuster

    Executive frontal functions

    Exp. Brain Res.

    (2000)
  • M. Gallagher et al.

    Severity of spatial learning impairment in aging: development of a learning index for performance in the Morris water maze

    Behav. Neurosci.

    (1993)
  • M. Gallagher et al.

    The use of animal models to study the effects of aging on cognition

    Annu. Rev. Psychol.

    (1997)
  • N. Gamboz et al.

    The role of switching, inhibition and working memory in older adults' performance in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test

    Neuropsychol. Dev. Cogn. B Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn.

    (2009)
  • Glisky, E.L., 2007. Changes in cognitive function in human aging, in: Riddle, D.R. (Ed.), Brain Aging: Models, Methods,...
  • P.S. Goldman et al.

    Localization of function within the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of the rhesus monkey

    Exp. Neurol.

    (1970)
  • P.S. Goldman-Rakic

    The prefrontal landscape: implications of functional architecture for understanding human mentation and the central executive

    Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci.

    (1996)
  • Cited by (57)

    • Acute vagus nerve stimulation enhances reversal learning in rats

      2021, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory
      Citation Excerpt :

      Given our rapidly changing environment, this neurocognitive process is essential for effectively navigating everyday life. Behavioral rigidity and perseveration occur in a host of neuropsychiatric conditions (including schizophrenia, obsessive compulsive disorder, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, autism, and substance use disorders) as well as during the normal aging process (Beas, Setlow, & Bizon, 2013; Bizon, Foster, Alexander, & Glisky, 2012; Stuchlik & Sumiyoshi, 2014). As such, therapeutic strategies to enhance cognitive flexibility could have far-reaching benefits (Groman et al., 2013; Izquierdo & Jentsch, 2012).

    • Attenuated NMDAR signaling on fast-spiking interneurons in prefrontal cortex contributes to age-related decline of cognitive flexibility

      2021, Neuropharmacology
      Citation Excerpt :

      In rodents, cognitive flexibility depends on the mPFC and may be evaluated through set-shifting tasks implemented across a variety of modalities and experimental settings (Birrell and Brown, 2000; Floresco et al., 2008; Ragozzino et al., 1999). While decline of hippocampus-dependent spatial memory is also a reliable feature of rodent brain aging (Barnes et al., 1997; Bizon et al., 2009; Gallagher et al., 1993; McQuail and Nicolle, 2015), individual differences in set-shift performance do not correlate with hippocampal-dependent learning and memory as evaluated in the Morris water maze (Barense et al., 2002; Beas et al., 2013; Nieves-Martinez et al., 2012). Further, analysis of errors committed by aged rats during the set-shift phase of the task reveals that slower acquisition of the new rule is due to perseverative errors made in response to the previously reinforced stimulus (i.e. location of the illuminated light; Beas et al., 2013, 2017; and see also Beas et al., 2016), mirroring the pattern of behavioral deficits produced by mPFC inactivation (Floresco et al., 2008).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text