Elsevier

NeuroImage

Volume 28, Issue 4, December 2005, Pages 996-1006
NeuroImage

Differential coding of hyperalgesia in the human brain: A functional MRI study

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.06.049Get rights and content

Abstract

Neuropathic pain can be both ongoing or stimulus-induced. Stimulus-induced pain, also known as hyperalgesia, can be differentiated into primary and secondary hyperalgesia. The former results from sensitization of peripheral nociceptive structures, the latter involves sensitization processes within the central nervous system (CNS). Hypersensitivity towards heat stimuli, i.e. thermal hyperalgesia, is a key feature of primary hyperalgesia, whereas secondary hyperalgesia is characterized by hypersensitivity towards mechanical (e.g. pin-prick) stimulation. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), we investigated if brain activation patterns associated with primary and secondary hyperalgesia might differ. Thermal and pin-prick hyperalgesia were induced on the left forearm in 12 healthy subjects by topical capsaicin (2.5%, 30 min) application. Equal pain intensities of both hyperalgesia types were applied during fMRI experiments, based on previous quantitative sensory testing. Simultaneously, subjects had to rate the unpleasantness of stimulus-related pain. Pin-prick hyperalgesia (i.e. subtraction of brain activations during pin-prick stimulation before and after capsaicin exposure) led to activations of primary and secondary somatosensory cortices (S1 and S2), associative-somatosensory cortices, insula and superior and inferior frontal cortices (SFC, IFC). Brain areas activated during thermal hyperalgesia (i.e. subtraction of brain activations during thermal stimulation before and after capsaicin exposure) were S1 and S2, insula, associative-somatosensory cortices, cingulate cortex (GC), SFC, middle frontal cortex (MFC) and IFC. When compared to pin-prick hyperalgesia, thermal hyperalgesia led to an increased activation of bilateral anterior insular cortices, MFC, GC (Brodmann area 24′ and 32′) and contralateral SFC and IFC, despite equal pain intensities. Interestingly, stronger activations of GC, contralateral MFC and anterior insula significantly correlated to higher ratings of the stimulus-related unpleasantness. We conclude that thermal and mechanical hyperalgesia produce substantially different brain activation patterns. This is linked to different psychophysical properties.

Introduction

The human pain experience is a complex sensation that is of paramount importance to maintain the body integrity and survival of human beings. It is a multidimensional phenomenon with sensory-discriminative, affective-motivational, motor and autonomic components (Treede et al., 1999). In the last decade, remarkable efforts have been undertaken to uncover the cortical processing of the human pain experience by non-invasive functional brain imaging techniques, such as positron emission tomography (PET), functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and magnetoencephalography (MEG). These techniques provide evidence for an involvement of the thalamus, primary somatosensory cortex (S1), secondary somatosensory cortex (S2), insula, forebrain and cingulate cortex (gyrus cinguli; GC; for review, see Treede et al., 1999, Ingvar, 1999, Peyron et al., 2000). There is accumulating evidence that these areas process different aspects of pain (Rainville et al., 1997, Coghill et al., 1999, Hofbauer et al., 2001). In this context, it has been suggested that a medial pain system (e.g. GC and the forebrain) particularly encodes the affective-motivational pain aspects, whereas a lateral system (e.g. S1, S2) is likely to account for the sensory-discriminative pain dimension (Treede et al., 1999, Vogt BA and Sikes, 2000, Price, 2000, Sewards TV and Sewards, 2002). Basically, pain can be classified as nociceptive or neuropathic (Woolf and Mannion, 1999). Whereas most functional imaging studies were undertaken in healthy subjects during experimental nociceptive pain, only few investigations focused on the neural correlates of neuropathic pain syndromes (Hsieh et al., 1995, Peyron et al., 1998, Petrovic et al., 1999, Maihofner et al., 2003, Peyron et al., 2004, Maihofner et al., 2005). This type of chronic pain may result from damage to the peripheral or central nervous system (Woolf and Mannion, 1999). Neuropathic pain can be both ongoing or stimulus-induced (Koltzenburg et al., 1992, Ochoa JL and Yarnitsky, 1993). Stimulus-induced pain, also known as hyperalgesia, can be differentiated according to the kind of stimulation, such as chemical, thermal and mechanical. Following tissue injury, two types of hyperalgesias may develop. Primary hyperalgesia occurs at the site of tissue injury and is characterized by hyperalgesia to heat and tonic pressure. In contrast, secondary hyperalgesia clearly extends into uninjured tissue and is associated with increased pain in response to punctuate mechanical stimulation, i.e. pin-prick hyperalgesia (Koltzenburg et al., 1992), or pain elicited by gently stroking the skin, i.e. dynamic-mechanical allodynia (Ochoa and Yarnitsky, 1993). The underlying pathophysiology of primary and secondary hyperalgesia is substantially different as primary hyperalgesia results from sensitization of C-fibers in the periphery, whereas pin-prick hyperalgesia is maintained by sensitization of nociceptive pathways within the central nervous system (CNS) (Schmidt et al., 1995, Ziegler et al., 1999, Klede et al., 2003). Both types of hyperalgesia can also be provoked in healthy subjects following topical application of algogens, such as capsaicin and mustard oil (Koltzenburg et al., 1994, Kilo et al., 1994). Especially, application of capsaicin was demonstrated as a reliable and valid model to explore mechanisms of stimulus-evoked pain (Simone et al., 1989, Maihofner et al., 2004). In the present study, we used the capsaicin sensitization model in order to provoke primary and secondary hyperalgesia in healthy subjects. As the underlying pathophysiology of both hyperalgesias differs, we hypothesized that the corresponding brain processing may diverge as well. In order to investigate this hypothesis, we used fMRI to explore brain activations associated with the processing of primary and secondary hyperalgesia. Pin-prick and thermal hyperalgesia were provoked using topical capsaicin application. Equal pain intensities of both hyperalgesias were applied. We here report substantially different brain activation patterns during pin-prick and thermal hyperalgesia. This difference was linked to the perceived pain unpleasantness of the subjects.

Section snippets

Methods

Twelve healthy right-handed volunteers (11 males, 1 female, mean age 32.2 years, range 24–47 years; mean height 176 cm, range 161–189 cm) participated in the experiment. None was taking drugs that may have interfered with itch or pain sensations and flare response (i.e. analgesics, antihistamines, calcium or sodium channel blockers). Informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to the experiments, and the study adhered to the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki. The study

Psychophysical test session

The pain elicited by the topical capsaicin solution on the left forearm was mild to moderate and was rated on average at 19.2 ± 12 on the NRS by the subjects. The maximum pain rating during the capsaicin application was 42.1 ± 4. Following 30 min of capsaicin application, the heat pain thresholds were significantly lowered for the whole group (44.4 ± 0.6 C versus 39.3 ± 1.1°C, pre and after capsaicin, respectively; P < 0.05, Wilcoxon signed rank test; Fig. 2A). Furthermore, the stimulus

Discussion

In the present study, we used fMRI in order to identify the neuronal matrix involved in the processing of primary (i.e. thermal) and secondary (i.e. mechanical) hyperalgesia. We were able to show that both types of stimulus-evoked pains produce substantially different brain activation patterns at equal levels of perceived pain intensity. Stronger activations of cingulate, insular and frontal cortices during thermal hyperalgesia were significantly linked to higher ratings of the stimulus-related

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by the German Research Network “Neuropathic Pain Syndromes” (German Federal Ministry of Education and Research; BMBF). We thank Ms. Gabi Waldeck-Göhring and Ms. Iris Schäfer for excellent technical assistance.

References (71)

  • P. Petrovic et al.

    A PET activation study of dynamic mechanical allodynia in patients with mononeuropathy

    Pain

    (1999)
  • P. Petrovic et al.

    Pain-related cerebral activation is altered by a distracting cognitive task

    Pain

    (2000)
  • R. Peyron et al.

    Functional imaging of brain responses to pain. A review and meta-analysis (2000)

    Neurophysiol. Clin.

    (2000)
  • P. Rainville et al.

    Dissociation of sensory and affective dimensions of pain using hypnotic modulation

    Pain

    (1999)
  • M. Schmelz et al.

    Topical acetylsalicylate attenuates capsaicin induced pain, flare and allodynia but not thermal hyperalgesia

    Neurosci. Lett.

    (1996)
  • T.V. Sewards et al.

    The medial pain system: neural representations of the motivational aspect of pain

    Brain Res. Bull.

    (2002)
  • D.A. Simone et al.

    Dose-dependent pain and mechanical hyperalgesia in humans after intradermal injection of capsaicin

    Pain

    (1989)
  • R.D. Treede et al.

    The cortical representation of pain

    Pain

    (1999)
  • B.A. Vogt et al.

    The medial pain system, cingulate cortex, and parallel processing of nociceptive information

    Prog. Brain Res.

    (2000)
  • C.J. Woolf et al.

    Neuropathic pain: aetiology, symptoms, mechanisms, and management

    Lancet

    (1999)
  • S. Zhang et al.

    Inhibitory effects of electrical stimulation of ventrolateral orbital cortex on the rat jaw-opening reflex

    Brain Res.

    (1998)
  • S.J. Bantick et al.

    Imaging how attention modulates pain in humans using functional MRI

    Brain

    (2002)
  • R. Baron et al.

    Brain processing of capsaicin-induced secondary hyperalgesia: a functional MRI study

    Neurology

    (1999)
  • A. Biemond

    The conduction of pain above the level of the thalamus opticus

    AMA Arch. Neurol. Psychiatry

    (1956)
  • K. Bornhovd et al.

    Painful stimuli evoke different stimulus-response functions in the amygdala, prefrontal, insula and somatosensory cortex: a single-trial fMRI study

    Brain

    (2002)
  • G.M. Boynton et al.

    Linear systems analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging in human V1

    J. Neurosci.

    (1996)
  • S.A. Bunge et al.

    Prefrontal regions involved in keeping information in and out of mind

    Brain

    (2001)
  • H. Burton et al.

    The posterior thalamic region and its cortical projection in New World and Old World monkeys

    J. Comp. Neurol.

    (1976)
  • K.L. Casey

    Forebrain mechanisms of nociception and pain: analysis through imaging

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A.

    (1999)
  • R.C. Coghill et al.

    Distributed processing of pain and vibration by the human brain

    J. Neurosci.

    (1994)
  • R.C. Coghill et al.

    Pain intensity processing within the human brain: a bilateral, distributed mechanism

    J. Neurophysiol.

    (1999)
  • S.J. Cooper

    Anaesthetisation of prefrontal cortex and response to noxious stimulation

    Nature

    (1975)
  • A.D. Craig et al.

    Functional imaging of an illusion of pain

    Nature

    (1996)
  • A.D. Craig et al.

    Thermosensory activation of insular cortex

    Nat. Neurosci.

    (2000)
  • K.D. Davis et al.

    Functional MRI of pain- and attention-related activations in the human cingulate cortex

    J. Neurophysiol.

    (1997)
  • Cited by (98)

    • Nociceptors: thermal allodynia and thermal pain

      2018, Handbook of Clinical Neurology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Mechanic pain, but not heat pain, can also increase at adjacent, uninjured areas, and is called secondary hyperalgesia (Raja et al., 1984). Secondary mechanic hyperalgesia is thought to reflect the sensitization of spinal nociceptive neurons (Klede et al., 2003; Woolf, 2011) and leads to substantially different brain activation patterns than thermal hyperalgesia (Maihofner and Handwerker, 2005). Both allodynia and hyperalgesia are clinical terms that do not imply a particular underlying mechanism (Sandkuhler, 2009).

    • Functional reorganisation in chronic pain and neural correlates of pain sensitisation: A coordinate based meta-analysis of 266 cutaneous pain fMRI studies

      2016, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
      Citation Excerpt :

      Given the core role of the posterior insula for pain perception, increased activation likely reflects hyperalgesia in chronic pain. Moreover, INS activation correlates with unpleasantesness of thermal hyperalgesia (Maihofner and Handwerker, 2005) and is involved in modulation of the affective aspect of sensory perception by pain expectation (Sawamoto et al., 2000). Multiple functional associations have been reported for the middle frontal gyrus (BA9), including attention to negative emotional stimuli (Kerestes et al., 2012).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text