Elsevier

NeuroImage

Volume 31, Issue 3, 1 July 2006, Pages 1177-1187
NeuroImage

From preparation to online control: Reappraisal of neural circuitry mediating internally generated and externally guided actions

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.041Get rights and content

Abstract

Action plans internally generated (IG) from memory are thought to be regulated by the supplementary motor area (SMA), whereas plans externally guided (EG) online using sensory cues are believed to be controlled by the premotor cortex. This theory was investigated in an event-related fMRI study that separated the time course of activation before and during movement to distinguish advance planning from online control. In contrast to prevailing theory, the SMA was not more important for online control of IG actions. EG movement was distinguished from IG movement by greater activation in a more distributed right hemisphere parietal–frontal network than previously reported. Comparisons between premovement and movement periods showed that frontostriatal networks are central for preparing actions before movement onset. However, unlike cortical and cerebellar regions, the basal ganglia exhibited planning-related activity before, but not during, movement. These findings indicate that the basal ganglia mediate planning and online control processes in different ways and suggest a specific role for the striatum in internally planning sequences of actions before they are implemented.

Introduction

Humans are remarkably proficient at learning large repertoires of skills, nearly all aspects of which require sequencing actions. Central to controlling action sequences are planning operations, which encompass a broad range of cognitive processes that allow us to anticipate events, select movements, specify their ordering, and control actions online. Plans for some sequential behaviors, like performing the tango, are internally generated (IG) since dance steps are implemented by retrieving a memory representation of an action sequence that fits the music. Plans for other sequential behaviors, like stepping on the brakes and steering to avoid an oncoming car, are externally guided (EG) because the actions are strongly associated with visual or other sensory cues that dictate when and how to act. It remains a matter of considerable debate, however, as to whether IG and EG actions involve fundamentally different planning processes that are mediated by distinct brain systems. The prospect that IG and EG movements come under different neural control is suggested by the observation that people with Parkinson's disease have problems performing movements generated from memory but often overcome this difficulty when provided with an external sensory cue (Glickstein and Stein, 1991).

To explore this issue, previous studies have used “free-movement”, “free-selection”, or “spontaneous willed-action” tasks wherein different movements or sequences of movements are self-generated on each trial (Hunter et al., 2003). Functional activity during these tasks is contrasted with tasks in which the same movements are guided by auditory or visual cues. An assumption is that plans for IG actions are intention-based because they are driven by an internal “urge” or desire whereas plans for EG actions are stimulus-based because they are guided online by external cues. The common finding is that supplementary motor area (SMA) activation is greater during IG movement and lateral premotor activation is greater during EG movement (Jenkins et al., 2000, Tanji, 1996). This suggests that the SMA is crucial for planning and executing actions generated from memory whereas the lateral premotor cortex mediates planning movements that are guided by visual or other sensory cues (Goldberg, 1985).

Still, evidence supporting neuroanatomically distinct mechanisms for these two routes to action is limited in part because the tasks used to study IG behaviors place greater demands on processes that are only peripherally related to planning movements from memory. This is because in IG tasks the subject selects an action from a repertoire of behaviors, with the constraint that the same action cannot be repeated on successive trials. For this reason, differences in activation patterns between IG and EG actions may be more related to the greater demands of IG actions on maintaining and updating working memory, attending to multiple actions, and/or response conflict monitoring (Lau et al., 2004). It has also been difficult to elucidate distinct neural mechanisms for intention- and stimulus-based planning because in the laboratory IG and EG actions are highly predictable since the repertoire of potential movements is typically quite small (e.g., one or two movements) to ensure that both types of actions are equivalent. As a consequence, IG actions are partly stimulus-guided in that they are well specified by external task instructions rather than “spontaneously” generated as sometimes implied. In fact, the distinction between these two routes of action may be overdrawn in the laboratory and real world (Waszak et al., 2005) since IG behaviors require some level of stimulus guidance from the environment to specify actions for a behavioral context. Similarly, though EG actions are guided online by stimulus information, they require some internal planning. Thus, a more pivotal distinction may be that IG-generated behaviors are planned and implemented from memory whereas EG behaviors are planned online with the aid of sensory information.

In the present study, subjects underwent event-related fMRI while performing IG and EG motor sequences. We asked whether IG and EG actions are modulated by neuroanatomically distinct mechanisms when executive processing demands of IG movements are minimized. To investigate this question, we focused on a central difference between the two routes to action, namely that IG action plans are prepared, retrieved, and implemented from memory whereas EG action plans are formulated online using sensory cues to guide performance. To increase planning and online control demands, a large repertoire of actions (i.e., 9 different sequences) was used so that movements were not highly predictable from trial to trial. We also separated the time course of activation associated with planning and executing movements, which enabled us to (1) identify the neural systems that were associated with planning IG movements and holding them in memory (IG premovement) from those involved in motor readiness/anticipation (EG premovement) and (2) determine whether the neural control of IG and EG actions differed during movement (IG versus EG movement). This design also allowed us to distinguish neural systems principally associated with generating action plans (IG premovement) from those involved in retrieving and implementing plans from memory (IG movement). Despite its theoretical importance, this issue has received little consideration in neuroimaging studies of motor control. Unlike previous studies, two levels of sequence complexity were also used to identify differences in neural activation patterns associated with increased difficulty in advanced planning and online control (Harrington et al., 2000). We reasoned that sequence complexity should exert a differential effect on brain activation for IG and EG sequences during movement if they differ in the difficulty of online control processes. Similarly, regions principally associated with formulating plans in memory should show a greater effect of sequence complexity before movement (IG premovement), whereas those more involved in retrieving and implementing plans from memory should show a greater effect of sequence complexity during movement (IG movement).

Section snippets

Participants

Twenty-six healthy volunteers participated in this study (14 females; mean age = 29.3 years, range = 19 and 50; mean education = 16.2 years, range = 12 to 22). All subjects were strongly right-handed according to the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971) (mean laterality quotient = 89.0, range = 65 to 100). Subjects were excluded if they had significant neurological, psychiatric, or other medical history, were taking psychoactive medications, or if their response accuracy fell below

Behavioral findings

All participants performed the sequences at a minimum accuracy level of 90% correct per sequence condition (Fig. 2, top panel). Simple sequences were performed more accurately than complex sequences [F(1,25) = 22.5, P < 0.0001], but neither the main effect of cue type (IG vs. EG) nor the interaction of cue type with complexity was significant. For reaction time (RT; Fig. 2, middle panel), significant effects were observed for cue type [F(1,25) = 470.0, P < 0.0001], complexity [F(1,25) = 86.5, P

Discussion

The present results provide new insight into the neural basis and functional significance of purported differences between IG and EG actions and, more generally, planning and online control processes. By separating the time course of brain activity during the premovement and movement periods and comparing the effects of sequence complexity, we showed that IG and EG actions could be distinguished by activation related to controlling movements online. While we did not find that medial premotor

Acknowledgments

All authors contributed equally to this paper. This research was supported in part by grants from the National Institutes of Health (P01 MH51358; M01 RR00058) and the Charles A. Dana Foundation to SMR and the Department of Veterans Affairs and the MIND Institute (2205) to DLH.

References (51)

  • N.D. Schluter et al.

    Cerebral dominance for action in the human brain: the selection of actions

    Neuropsychologia

    (2001)
  • J.D. Schmahmann et al.

    Three-dimensional MRI atlas of the human cerebellum in proportional stereotaxic space

    NeuroImage

    (1999)
  • S. Sternberg et al.

    The latency and duration of rapid movement sequences: comparisons of speech and typewriting

  • J. Tanji et al.

    Multiple cortical motor areas and temporal sequencing of movements

    Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res.

    (1996)
  • R. Benecke et al.

    Disturbance of sequential movements in patients with Parkinson's disease

    Brain

    (1987)
  • H. Boecker et al.

    Role of the human rostral supplementary motor area and the basal ganglia in motor sequence control: investigations with H2 15O PET

    J. Neurophysiol.

    (1998)
  • M.J. Catalan et al.

    The functional neuroanatomy of simple and complex sequential finger movements: a PET study

    Brain

    (1998)
  • M.V. Chafee et al.

    Matching patterns of activity in primate prefrontal area 8a and parietal area 7ip neurons during a spatial working memory task

    J. Neurophysiol.

    (1998)
  • Y.-C. Chen et al.

    The functions of the medial premotor cortex (SMA) II. The timing and selection of learned movements

    Exp. Brain Res.

    (1995)
  • J.D. Connolly et al.

    Human fMRI evidence for the neural correlates of preparatory set

    Nat. Neurosci.

    (2002)
  • M.P. Deiber et al.

    Mesial motor areas in self-initiated versus externally triggered movements examined with fMRI: effect of movement type and rate

    J. Neurophysiol.

    (1999)
  • J.E. Desmond et al.

    Lobular patterns of cerebellar activation in verbal working-memory and finger-tapping tasks as revealed by functional MRI

    J. Neurosci.

    (1997)
  • R.P. Dum et al.

    The origin of corticospinal projections from the premotor areas in the frontal lobe

    J. Neurosci.

    (1991)
  • R.P. Dum et al.

    An unfolded map of the cerebellar dentate nucleus and its projections to the cerebral cortex

    J. Neurophysiol.

    (2003)
  • J.M. Ellerman et al.

    Spatial patterns of functional activation of the cerebellum investigated using high field (4 T) MRI

    NMR Biomed.

    (1994)
  • Cited by (87)

    • Neural substrates of accurate perception of time duration: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study

      2022, Neuropsychologia
      Citation Excerpt :

      Finally, at the decision stage, the stored duration values are compared with those in the reference memory to make decisions (Pouthas et al., 2005). Several brain areas have been candidate neural substrates for perception of temporal duration such as the frontostriatal circuits involving the supplementary motor area (SMA), right prefrontal cortex, right posterior parietal cortex, insula, putamen, and cerebellum (Buhusi and Meck, 2005; Elsinger et al., 2006; Harrington et al., 2004a; Rubia and Smith, 2004; Wittmann, 2009; Wittmann and van Wassenhove, 2009). A recent meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies identified the consistent involvement of the insula, pre-SMA/SMA, inferior frontal gyrus, pre-central gyrus, cingulate gyrus, superior temporal gyrus, claustrum, putamen, and caudate body (Nani et al., 2019).

    • The Proprioceptive Neuromuscular Facilitation Concept in Parkinson Disease: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis

      2020, Journal of Chiropractic Medicine
      Citation Excerpt :

      The improvement in PD gait parameters in the El-Tamawy et al study19 has a potent effect from external cues linked to proprioceptive inputs by PNF and vibration. Such inputs can activate an alternate pathway involving the cerebellum, sensorimotor cortex, and lateral premotor cortex.31,32 This means that the recruitment of these structures can compensate for an inefficient basal ganglia in PD.

    • Single session of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation to left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex increased dual-task gait speed in chronic stroke: A pilot study

      2020, Gait and Posture
      Citation Excerpt :

      Another neural node that has been shown to be associated with dual-task behaviors is the supplementary motor area (SMA) [11]. The SMA is involved in motor planning [12] and modulates attention to motor task performance [13]. Further, the SMA has been found to be associated with gait performance and may play a role in modulating dual-task gait [7].

    • Disentangling motor planning and motor execution in unmedicated de novo Parkinson's disease patients: An fMRI study

      2019, NeuroImage: Clinical
      Citation Excerpt :

      These results suggest that STN-cerebellar connections are involved in the integration of basal ganglia and cerebellar functions, both in motor and non-motor domains (please see for review (Bostan et al., 2010; Wu and Hallett, 2013)). The main effects during the motor planning phase in the within-group analyses are highly consistent with previous literature reported in the context of motor planning processes (Cunnington et al., 2002; Elsinger et al., 2006; Jahanshahi et al., 1995; Jenkins et al., 2000; Weeks et al., 2001), including activation in fronto-parietal regions (i.e. the posterior aspect of the DLPFC (BA9), and Brodmann area 8 including the frontal eye fields, the superior parietal lobe and precuneus (BA5 and BA7) and the supra-marginal gyrus (BA40)), mesial and lateral PMC, contralateral M1, anterior putamen / anterior insula, and lateral cerebellum during FREEPLAN in the controls. The Parkinson's disease patients showed a similar cortical activation pattern, and in addition a pronounced activation of the DLPFC (observable bilaterally in FREEPLAN and more lateralized in REACTPLAN ipsilateral to the affected and contralateral to the non-affected hand, respectively).

    • Oscillation patterns of local field potentials in the dorsal striatum and sensorimotor cortex during the encoding, maintenance, and decision stages for the ordinal comparison of sub- and supra-second signal durations

      2018, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory
      Citation Excerpt :

      Although the pre-SMA/SMA is often thought to play a key role in timing (e.g., Coull, Nazarian, & Vidal, 2008, 2011; Macar et al., 1999; Macar & Vidal, 2003, 2009; Pouthas et al., 2000); recent reports have questioned the validity of this claim (e.g., Harrington et al., 2010; Kononowicz & van Rijn, 2011, 2014; Nenadic et al., 2003; van Rijn et al., 2011; van Rijn, Gu, & Meck, 2014), and it has been suggested that the pre-SMA/SMA is not specifically concerned with interval timing, but rather with response monitoring (e.g., Boehm, van Maanen, Forstmann, & van Rijn, 2014; Kononowicz & Penney, 2016). In other behavioral contexts, for example, the SMA and premotor cortex, but not the striatum, are influenced by task complexity during movement (Elsinger, Harrington, & Rao, 2006), consistent with a role in the management of temporal representations, but not necessary or sufficient for the perception and production of stimulus or response durations (Kononowicz et al., 2018; Nakajima, Hosaka, Mushiake, & Tanji, 2009). Consistent with previous findings, timing performance in the ordinal-comparison procedure for signal durations in the sub-second range was poorer than for signal durations in the supra-second range (see Cordes & Meck, 2014; Lusk, Petter, & Meck, in press).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text