Elsevier

NeuroImage

Volume 47, Issue 4, 1 October 2009, Pages 2073-2082
NeuroImage

Evidence of a modality-dependent role of the cerebellum in working memory? An fMRI study comparing verbal and abstract n-back tasks

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.005Get rights and content

Abstract

In working memory (WM), functional imaging studies demonstrate cerebellar involvement indicating a cognitive role of the cerebellum. These cognitive contributions were predominantly interpreted as part of the phonological loop within the Baddeley model of WM. However, those underlying investigations were performed in the context of visual verbal WM which could pose a bias when interpreting the results. The aim of this fMRI study was to address the question of whether the cerebellum supports additional aspects of WM in the context of higher cognitive functions. Furthermore, laterality effects were investigated to further disentangle the cerebellar role in the context of the phonological loop and the visuospatial sketchpad. A direct comparison of verbal and abstract visual WM was performed in 17 young volunteers by applying a 2-back paradigm and extracting the % change in BOLD signal from the fMRI data. To minimize potential verbal strategies, Attneave and Arnoult shapes of non-nameable objects were chosen for the abstract condition. The analyses revealed no significant differences in verbal vs. abstract WM. Moreover, no laterality effects were demonstrated in both verbal and abstract WM. These results provide further evidence of a broader cognitive involvement of the cerebellum in WM that is not only confined to the phonological loop but also supports central executive subfunctions. The fact that no lateralization effects are found might be attributed to the characteristics of the n-back paradigm which emphasizes central executive subfunctions over the subsidiary slave systems.

Introduction

It has been repeatedly demonstrated that the cerebellum is essentially involved in the control and integration of motor activity. However, over the last two decades, evidence has also been generated with regard to a cerebellar role in cognition (Desmond and Fiez, 1998, Fiez, 2001b, Marien et al., 2001, Schmahmann, 1991, Schmahmann, 2004, Schmahmann and Sherman, 1998). Studies on patients with cerebellar lesions and neuroimaging studies on healthy volunteers revealed a cerebellar contribution to executive functions such as planning, temporal sequencing, attention, learning and memory as well as an involvement in language processes (for review see e.g. Ackermann et al., 2007, Bellebaum and Daum, 2007, Ben-Yehudah et al., 2007, Haarmeier and Thier, 2007, Hokkanen et al., 2006, Leggio et al., 2008, Marien et al., 2001, Timmann and Daum, 2007). Accordingly, a number of studies focusing on working memory (WM) reported neuronal activity in the cerebellum in addition to the well-known fronto-parietal neocortical network (Cabeza and Nyberg, 2000, Desmond et al., 1997, Fiez, 2001a, Gruber, 2001, Gruber and von Cramon, 2003, Hautzel et al., 2002Hautzel et al., 2003, Krause et al., 2006, Owen et al., 2005, Paulesu et al., 1993). This neuronal network activated by WM tasks is discussed predominantly in the highly influential work of Baddeley (1986), Baddeley and Hitch (1974), and Repovs and Baddeley (2006). From this theoretical perspective, the cerebellum and Broca's area were interpreted to form a subvocal rehearsal process (Chen and Desmond, 2005a, Chen and Desmond, 2005b; Desmond et al., 1997, Paulesu et al., 1993). In combination with the phonological store, this subvocal rehearsal builds the phonological loop — one of the content-specific components used to maintain different types of information (Baddeley, 2000). The main function of the subvocal rehearsal process is to actively refresh the information items in the phonological store. However, only a few studies tackled the issue of the cerebellar function in WM directly (Chen and Desmond, 2005b, Chen and Desmond, 2005a, Desmond et al., 1997, Desmond et al., 2005, Hokkanen et al., 2006, Mathiak et al., 2004). Desmond and Fiez postulate a more prominent role of the cerebellum in WM when general task demands and/or memory load increase, respectively (Desmond and Fiez, 1998). However, studies specifically focusing on WM and the cerebellum have tested cerebellar function primarily within verbal WM (Chen and Desmond, 2005aChen and Desmond, 2005b, Chiricozzi et al., 2008, Desmond et al., 1997Desmond et al., 2005, Fiez, 2001a, Paulesu et al., 1993). Consequently, the results have been discussed in the context of the phonological loop and its subsystems, leading to the conclusion that parts of the cerebellum subserve the rehearsal system, while others belong to the phonological store (Chiricozzi et al., 2008, Desmond et al., 1997). In addition, by applying exclusively visual verbal WM, the Desmond group revealed a right-sided laterality effect with the involvement of lobules VI and crus1 in subvocal rehearsal processes, while right lobules VIIb and VIII were found to contribute to the phonological store (Chen and Desmond, 2005aChen and Desmond, 2005b, Desmond et al., 1997, 2003Desmond et al., 2005, Kirschen et al., 2005). However, the work of Kirschen et al. also presented evidence of an activation of the contralateral lobules as well, despite the fact that these left-sided contributions were less extended and less significant. Studies in lesioned patients failed to replicate a clear-cut right-sided lateralization of WM functions in the cerebellum. Concerning the visuospatial sketchpad, a study has yet to address the question of a cerebellar contribution to this WM subsystem directly but data derived from patients with unilateral left-sided cerebellar damage point towards a higher impairment in a visuospatial task compared to a right-sided lesion within the cerebellum (Hokkanen et al., 2006). Overall, the issue of cerebellar laterality in WM requires further evaluation.

In addition to verbal WM, other modalities employing object, spatial or abstract shape stimuli also induce cerebellar activations (Courtney et al., 1996, Hautzel et al., 2002, Nystrom et al., 2000, Pessoa and Ungerleider, 2004). This could be indicative of a more general role of the cerebellum within WM beyond its function in subordinate routines closely related to motor processes like inner speech. Evidence of extended cerebellar contributions to WM can be found in studies by Mathiak et al. testing WM of time (Mathiak et al., 2004) and by Gottwald et al. investigating attention and other central executive functions in patients with cerebellar lesions (Gottwald et al., 2003, 2004). Moreover, Appollonio et al. interpreted WM impairments after cerebellar damage as secondary to an impairment of executive functions because after controlling for executive measures, memory scores were no longer different to those of healthy controls (Appollonio et al., 1993). Finally, the cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome (CCAS) summarizes an association of lesions to the posterior cerebellar lobe with deficits of executive functions (planning, set shifting, verbal fluency, abstract reasoning, and working memory) (Schmahmann and Caplan, 2006, Schmahmann and Sherman, 1998).

Given such divergent evidence concerning the role of the cerebellum, the aim of the fMRI study presented here was twofold: (1) to directly address the question of whether the cerebellum processes the predominantly language-related aspects of WM or whether it is also involved in higher-order cognitive WM processes, and (2) to evaluate laterality effects of the cerebellum at the hemisphere and the lobular levels for both the verbal and abstract WM separately.

In order to maximize the differences of speech input, we performed a direct intra-individual comparison of verbal WM and abstract shape WM using non-nameable polygons as stimuli for the latter constructed according to method 1 described by Attneave and Arnoult (1956). Furthermore, to ensure adequate comparability between modalities and underlying processes and particularly to stress the central executive subfunctions of time coding und updating, an n-back WM task was chosen instead of a delayed match-to-sample paradigm (Peters et al., 2005, Smith and Jonides, 1997, Wager and Smith, 2003). If the cerebellum is predominantly involved in subvocal rehearsal processes, we hypothesized an intense activation of neocerebellar structures during the verbal WM task but not while performing the abstract WM paradigm. In addition, predominantly right-sided cerebellar activations should be evident as a result of the crossed cerebello-cortical diaschisis (Botez-Marquard et al., 1994, Broich et al., 1987, Pantano et al., 1986). If the abstract WM recruits cerebellar regions in the context of the visuospatial sketchpad in analogy to verbal WM and the phonological loop, then a left-sided lateralization in the abstract WM activation patterns should result. On the other hand, if higher cognitive functions summarized in the central executive are the major cause of cerebellar activation, both tasks should reveal comparable signal increases in the cerebellum. From the neocortical perspective, both WM paradigms have been proven to activate the central executive core areas in prefrontal and parietal cortices bilaterally and to the same extent (Hautzel et al., 2002). If this alternative hypothesis of cerebellar involvement beyond the subordinate WM slave systems and particularly beyond the phonological loop holds true, then the resulting neuronal activations should be symmetrically distributed over both cerebellar hemispheres.

Section snippets

Subjects

Seventeen right-handed healthy male subjects (mean age 25.7 +/− 3.8 years) participated in this fMRI study after giving written informed consent. The study was approved by the local ethics committee. Part of the data was published in an earlier paper (Hautzel et al., 2002). In this re-evaluation of the data, the focus was laid specifically on the analysis of the contribution of the cerebellum to the different working memory tasks.

Working memory tasks

For stimulus presentation and response data collection via button

Behavioral data

The results of the behavioral data are given in Fig. 2. Accuracy and reaction times did not significantly differ between verbal WM and abstract WM (accuracy: 89.4 ± 8.0 % vs. 86.7 ± 7.8 %, p > 0.05; reaction times: 709 ± 142 ms vs. 757 ± 118 ms, p > 0.05). However, accuracy was significantly higher in the reference conditions: verbal WM performance: 2-back 89.4 ± 8.0 % vs. 0-back 98.8 ± 1.2 % (p < 0.001); abstract WM performance: 2-back 86.7 ± 7.8 % vs. 0-back 98.6 ± 2.4 % (p < 0.001). Accordingly, reaction times were

Discussion

The present study revealed an almost identical cerebellar activation pattern when comparing percent change of BOLD signal in a verbal WM task with an abstract WM task on a lobule by lobule basis and keeping the cognitive demands of the tasks at the same level. In addition, no laterality effects at the hemispherical or the lobular level were found in both WM modalities.

Since previous work on the cerebellum and WM interpreted results mainly as contributions of the subordinate slave systems, the

References (102)

  • DesmondJ.E. et al.

    Increased frontocerebellar activation in alcoholics during verbal working memory: an fMRI study

    Neuroimage

    (2003)
  • DimitrovaA. et al.

    Probabilistic 3D MRI atlas of the human cerebellar dentate/interposed nuclei

    Neuroimage

    (2006)
  • DoyonJ. et al.

    Role of the striatum, cerebellum and frontal lobes in the automatization of a repeated visuomotor sequence of movements

    Neuropsychologia

    (1998)
  • FiezJ.A.

    Neuroimaging studies of speech an overview of techniques and methodological approaches

    J. Commun. Disord.

    (2001)
  • GottwaldB. et al.

    Does the cerebellum contribute to specific aspects of attention?

    Neuropsychologia

    (2003)
  • GruberO. et al.

    The functional neuroanatomy of human working memory revisited. Evidence from 3-T fMRI studies using classical domain-specific interference tasks

    Neuroimage

    (2003)
  • HarringtonD.L. et al.

    Neural representation of interval encoding and decision making

    Brain Res.Cogn. Brain Res.

    (2004)
  • HautzelH. et al.

    Topographic segregation and convergence of verbal, object, shape and spatial working memory in humans

    Neurosci. Lett.

    (2002)
  • HayterA.L. et al.

    Cerebellar contributions to working memory

    Neuroimage

    (2007)
  • JustusT. et al.

    Reduced phonological similarity effects in patients with damage to the cerebellum

    Brain Lang.

    (2005)
  • KirschenM.P. et al.

    Load- and practice-dependent increases in cerebro-cerebellar activation in verbal working memory: an fMRI study

    Neuroimage

    (2005)
  • KrauseB.J. et al.

    Learning related interactions among neuronal systems involved in memory processes

    J. Physiol. Paris

    (2006)
  • LeungH.C. et al.

    Load response functions in the human spatial working memory circuit during location memory updating

    Neuroimage.

    (2007)
  • LoganG.

    Executive control of thought

    Acta. Psychologica.

    (1985)
  • MaldjianJ.A. et al.

    An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fMRI data sets

    Neuroimage

    (2003)
  • MaldjianJ.A. et al.

    Precentral gyrus discrepancy in electronic versions of the Talairach atlas

    Neuroimage

    (2004)
  • MarklundP. et al.

    Unity and diversity of tonic and phasic executive control components in episodic and working memory

    Neuroimage

    (2007)
  • MathiakK. et al.

    Discrimination of temporal information at the cerebellum: functional magnetic resonance imaging of nonverbal auditory memory

    Neuroimage

    (2004)
  • MottaghyF.M.

    Interfering with working memory in humans

    Neuroscience

    (2006)
  • NystromL.E. et al.

    Working memory for letters, shapes, and locations: fMRI evidence against stimulus-based regional organization in human prefrontal cortex

    Neuroimage

    (2000)
  • PetersJ. et al.

    Visuo-verbal interactions in working memory: evidence from event-related potentials

    Brain Res.Cogn. Brain Res.

    (2005)
  • RepovsG. et al.

    The multi-component model of working memory: explorations in experimental cognitive psychology

    Neuroscience

    (2006)
  • RypmaB. et al.

    Load-dependent roles of frontal brain regions in the maintenance of working memory

    Neuroimage

    (1999)
  • SchallU. et al.

    Functional brain maps of Tower of London performance: a positron emission tomography and functional magnetic resonance imaging study

    Neuroimage

    (2003)
  • SchirmerA.

    Timing speech: a review of lesion and neuroimaging findings

    Brain. Res. Cogn. Brain Res.

    (2004)
  • SchmahmannJ.D. et al.

    The cerebrocerebellar system

    Int. Rev. Neurobiol.

    (1997)
  • SchmahmannJ.D. et al.

    Three-dimensional MRI atlas of the human cerebellum in proportional stereotaxic space

    Neuroimage

    (1999)
  • SmithE.E. et al.

    Working memory: a view from neuroimaging

    Cognit.Psychol.

    (1997)
  • AckermannH. et al.

    The contribution of the cerebellum to speech production and speech perception: clinical and functional imaging data

    Cerebellum

    (2007)
  • AllenG.I. et al.

    Cerebrocerebellar communication systems

    Physiol. Rev.

    (1974)
  • AllenG. et al.

    Attentional activation of the cerebellum independent of motor involvement

    Science

    (1997)
  • AppollonioI.M. et al.

    Memory in patients with cerebellar degeneration

    Neurology

    (1993)
  • AttneaveF. et al.

    The quantitative study of shape and pattern perception

    Psychol. Bull.

    (1956)
  • AwhE. et al.

    Dissociation of storage and rehearsal in verbal working memory: Evidence from PET

    Psychol. Sci.

    (1996)
  • BaddeleyA.D.

    Working Memory

    (1986)
  • BellebaumC. et al.

    Cerebellar involvement in executive control

    Cerebellum

    (2007)
  • Ben-YehudahG. et al.

    Cerebellar contributions to verbal working memory: beyond cognitive theory

    Cerebellum

    (2007)
  • BotezM.I. et al.

    Role of the cerebellum in complex human behavior

    Ital. J. Neurol. Sci.

    (1989)
  • Botez-MarquardT. et al.

    Neuropsychological functioning in unilateral cerebellar damage

    Can. J. Neurol. Sci.

    (1994)
  • BreukelaarJ.W. et al.

    Effects of lesions to the cerebellar vermis and hemispheres on timing and counting in rats

    Behav. Neurosci.

    (1999)
  • Cited by (66)

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text