Elsevier

NeuroImage

Volume 48, Issue 3, 15 November 2009, Pages 601-608
NeuroImage

Similar and dissociable mechanisms for attention to internal versus external information

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.07.002Get rights and content

Abstract

We compared two attentional executive processes: updating, which involved attending to a perceptually present stimulus, and refreshing, which involved attending to a mentally active representation of a stimulus no longer perceptually present. In separate blocks, participants either replaced a word being held in working memory with a different word (update), or they thought back to a just previously seen word that was no longer perceptually present (refresh). Bilateral areas of frontal cortex, supplementary motor area, and parietal cortex were similarly active for both updating and refreshing, suggesting that a common network of areas is recruited to bring information to the current focus of attention. In a direct comparison of update and refresh, regions more active for update than refresh included regions primarily in right frontal cortex, as well as bilateral posterior visual processing regions. Regions more active for refresh than update included regions primarily in left dorsolateral frontal and left temporal cortex and bilateral inferior frontal cortex. These findings help account for the similarity in areas activated across different cognitive tasks and may help specify the particular executive processes engaged in more complex tasks.

Introduction

A common fronto-parietal network of areas (lateral prefrontal cortex, parietal cortex and supplementary motor area) is active in many cognitive tasks (Cabeza and Nyberg, 2000, Duncan and Owen, 2000) including working memory (WM), visual attention (Kelley et al., 2007, Serences et al., 2005, Serences and Yantis, 2007, Marois et al., 2004), and encoding (Blumenfeld and Ranganath, 2007) and retrieval from long term memory (Cabeza et al., 2008). This similarity of activity is consistent with the idea that common component processes are recruited for diverse cognitive tasks (Duncan and Owen, 2000, Cabeza and Nyberg, 2000, Johnson and Hirst, 1993; see also Awh and Jonides, 2001, Awh et al., 2006). Characterizing these components more specifically is a major challenge for cognitive science.

Two component executive processes that might, in part, account for this common activation across experiments are updating (Roth et al., 2006) and refreshing (Johnson et al., 2005). Updating, the replacement of an item actively being maintained in working memory with a different item, produces activity in a network including left inferior frontal junction (IFJ, the junction of inferior frontal sulcus and inferior precentral sulcus), dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), supplementary motor area (SMA), and bilateral parietal cortex including intraparietal sulcus (IPS). This network becomes active during updating of different types of visual stimuli (Roth et al., 2006), updating from a sensory stimulus or an item from long term memory (Roth and Courtney, 2007), and when the rule operating on a stimulus is updated (Montojo and Courtney, 2008). Refreshing is the process of attending to information that is not perceptually present but is momentarily active from either recent perception or thought (e.g., thinking back to a just-seen stimulus no longer on the screen; Raye et al., 2002; Johnson et al., 2005). Refreshing produces activity in a network including DLPFC, anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), supramarginal gyrus (SMG), IPS, and middle temporal gyrus, for a variety of types of stimuli (Johnson et al., 2005, Raye et al., 2002, Raye et al., 2007).

Similarities in activation for updating and refreshing across experiments have been noted before (Courtney et al., 2007), but this is the first experiment to directly compare them. One possibility is that updating and refreshing are the same process investigated in the context of different experimental paradigms and under different names. If so, we would expect refreshing and updating to produce the same patterns of activation in the same participants in procedures using the same materials. On the other hand, updating has been proposed as a specific mechanism for changing the contents of what is being actively maintained in working memory by prioritizing new information that is concurrently perceived or retrieved. Refreshing has been proposed as a general mechanism for briefly bringing information that is currently active but not perceptually present to the foreground of attention. Update and refresh may involve different areas of cortex. For example, updating should show relatively greater activity in posterior visual processing regions as a new target is perceptually attended, and refreshing should show relatively greater activity in frontal regions (e.g., left DLPFC) associated with modulating representations of no longer present perceptual stimuli (M.R. Johnson et al., 2007).

Assuming that there is a distinction between the cognitive operations and neural activity involved in perceptual vs. reflective attention (e.g., Dobbins and Han, 2006, Gilbert et al., 2005, Johnson et al., 2005, Johnson and Johnson, in press), directly comparing the neural correlates of updating working memory from a perceptually present stimulus with refreshing an active representation of a stimulus that is no longer perceptually present should help clarify similarities and differences in neural activity observed in more complex cognitive tasks that recruit these processes.

Section snippets

Participants

Participants were 22 (12 females) healthy adult (M = 24 yrs, range 19–44) non-smokers, with no history of head injury, psychiatric illness, drug or alcohol abuse, and no current medications that would affect the function of the brain, heart or blood circulation. Participants were compensated and all gave written informed consent. The experiments were undertaken with the approval of Yale University School of Medicine Human Investigation Committee, and in compliance with national legislation and

Behavioral results

The percent correct (87.3%, standard error 2.5%) for the response to match to sample, and false alarm rate (0.17%, standard error 0.03%), in the update task indicated that participants effectively updated WM.

Overlap in executive function areas

Fig. 2 shows areas of activation associated with transient activity associated with update and refresh, sustained activity associated with working memory maintenance (identified with a sustained block regressor for the update blocks) and their overlap. As is clear from Fig. 2, update and

Discussion

This study investigated two types of executive function: attending to a perceptual stimulus in order to replace a representation in WM (update) and attending to an active representation that is no longer perceptually present (refresh). We identified areas that were commonly active across these two types of executive function and areas that differed between these two executive functions.

Conclusions

The current results indicate that it is possible to separate executive functioning into regions commonly active across different executive functions and regions more specific to particular functions such as attending to an incoming stimulus to replace a maintained stimulus (update) or attending to an already active representation (refresh). Updating and refreshing are two mechanisms for bringing information into the focus of conscious attention. We would expect the network of commonly active

Acknowledgments

This research was funded in part by the National Institutes of Health (NS051622 to RTC and AG09253 to MKJ). The authors wish to thank Karen Mitchell for comments on the manuscript and the staff of the Yale Magnetic Resonance Research Center, especially Hedy Sarofin, Karen Martin and Terry Hickey, for their help with fMRI data acquisition.

References (53)

  • MontojoC.A. et al.

    Differential neural activation for updating rule versus stimulus information in working memory

    Neuron

    (2008)
  • NybergL. et al.

    Common prefrontal activations during working memory, episodic memory, and semantic memory

    Neuropsychologia

    (2003)
  • PoldrackR.A.

    Can cognitive processes be inferred from neuroimaging data?

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2006)
  • RayeC.L. et al.

    Neuroimaging a single thought: dorsolateral PFC activity associated with refreshing just-activated information

    NeuroImage

    (2002)
  • RayeC.L. et al.

    Refreshing: a minimal executive function

    Cortex

    (2007)
  • RothJ.K. et al.

    Neural system for updating object working memory from different sources: sensory stimuli or long-term memory

    NeuroImage

    (2007)
  • VisscherK.M. et al.

    Mixed blocked/event-related designs separate transient and sustained activity in fMRI

    NeuroImage

    (2003)
  • YantisS. et al.

    Cortical mechanisms of space-based and object-based attentional control

    Curr. Opin. Neurobiol.

    (2003)
  • BlumenfeldR.S. et al.

    Prefrontal cortex and long-term memory encoding: an integrative review of findings from neuropsychology and neuroimaging

    Neuroscientist

    (2007)
  • CabezaR. et al.

    Imaging cognition II: an empirical review of 275 PET and fMRI studies

    J. Cogn. Neurosci.

    (2000)
  • CabezaR. et al.

    The parietal cortex and episodic memory: an attentional account

    Nat. Rev. Neurosci.

    (2008)
  • ChristoffK. et al.

    Evaluating self-generated information: anterior prefrontal contributions to human cognition

    Behav. Neurosci.

    (2003)
  • CourtneyS.M. et al.

    An area specialized for spatial working memory in human frontal cortex

    Science

    (1998)
  • CourtneyS.M. et al.

    A hierarchical biased-competition model of domain-dependent working memory maintenance and executive control

  • CoxR.W.

    AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages

    Comput. Biomed. Res.

    (1996)
  • DerrfussJ. et al.

    Involvement of the inferior frontal junction in cognitive control: meta-analyses of switching and Stroop studies

    Hum. Brain Mapp.

    (2005)
  • Cited by (28)

    • Deficient single item maintenance following intact updating in schizophrenia

      2018, Schizophrenia Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      There were no group differences for age, education, ethnicity, or estimated premorbid intelligence (Table 1). Serial order processing (Hill et al., 2011) and updating paradigms (Roth et al., 2009) were adapted to develop a measure of updating that minimized other working memory demands. As illustrated in Fig. 1, participants were presented a target location and instructed to update to a new location periodically.

    • Transcranial magnetic stimulation to dorsolateral prefrontal cortex affects conflict-induced behavioural adaptation in a Wisconsin Card Sorting Test analogue

      2017, Neuropsychologia
      Citation Excerpt :

      One hypothesis is that the cognitive system might ‘reset’ to an average level of control that might not be as ‘lax’ as it would normally be after low-conflict trials (thus speeding up LH responses) but not as high as it would normally be after high-conflict trials (thus slowing down HH responses). Alternatively, if dlPFC's role in these kinds of tasks is to refresh/retrieve task instructions and rules (Badre, 2008; Raye et al., 2002; Roth et al., 2009), we predict that LH trials should be most affected, becoming even slower, as these are the trials signaling the need to retrieve task instructions. 32 participants (15 male, mean age 24.18 years) took part in the experiment.

    • The Interplay between Auditory Attention and Working Memory

      2015, Mechanisms of Sensory Working Memory: Attention and Perfomance XXV
    • State-dependent architecture of thalamic reticular subnetworks

      2014, Cell
      Citation Excerpt :

      The involvement of TRN in cognitive function offers a unique perspective on connecting a number of concepts in neuroscience that had previously been studied separately. For example, whereas our study examined the participation of TRN microcircuits in sleep and attentional states separately, the subnetwork architecture of TRN may allow for flexible switching between processing of external input and internal constructs in cognitive tasks, facilitating selective thalamo-cortical network engagement (Roth et al., 2009). Interestingly, recent experiments in humans have shown that rapid changes in arousal measures, such as pupil diameter, predict successful performance on cognitive tasks requiring the use of external information to update internal beliefs (Nassar et al., 2012).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text