Elsevier

Neuropsychologia

Volume 45, Issue 7, 2007, Pages 1489-1500
Neuropsychologia

What are confabulators’ memories made of? A study of subjective and objective measures of recollection in confabulation

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.11.007Get rights and content

Abstract

Confabulating patients claim to remember events that had not actually happened, suggesting a vivid subjective experience of false memories. The present study was aimed at examining the nature of subjective experience of retrieval in confabulators and its relation to the objective ability to recollect qualitative aspects of the original episode. In Experiment 1, 5 confabulators, 7 non-confabulating patients, and 12 control subjects studied words under shallow and deep encoding conditions and underwent a Remember (R)/Know (K) recognition task [Tulving, E. (1985). Memory and consciousness. Canadian Psychology, 26, 1–12]. For recognized words, they additionally indicated two qualitative features of the encoding context. Whereas subjective (i.e. R responses) and objective (i.e. source) measures of recollection were associated in normal controls and non-confabulating patients, they were dissociated in confabulators. In Experiment 2, participants explained the content of their R responses. We found that confabulators’ recollections mainly included autobiographical information related to test items, but not to the study encounter. We conclude that remembering states in confabulators may be linked to a deficit in inhibiting irrelevant memories triggered by test items during retrieval attempts.

Introduction

Confabulating patients have memories for events that have not been actually experienced. These false memories, termed confabulations (see Gilboa & Moscovitch, 2002 for a recent review), may include as many contextual and perceptual details as do memories of true events (Johnson, O’Connor, & Cantor, 1997). Confabulation is generally considered an impairment of retrieval rather than encoding processes (Burgess & Shallice, 1996; Moscovitch, 1989; Moscovitch & Melo, 1997; Schacter, Norman, & Koutstaal, 1998), consistent with the fact that it may affect memories acquired before the occurrence of brain damage (when encoding operations were well-functioning) as much as those acquired subsequent to brain damage.

Of great interest is confabulators’ subjective experience of retrieval: patients may claim they can re-live these confabulatory reports, suggestive of experiences of vivid recollection. For example, Dalla Barba (1993) described a patient, MB, who confabulated in response to questions tapping personal episodic memory (Dalla Barba, 1993). When asked to classify his memories according to the remember (R)/know (K) distinction (Tulving, 1985), that is, to indicate whether he recollected some aspects of the original episode (i.e., R response) or whether the memory was merely familiar (i.e. K response), MB frequently attributed R judgements to his confabulatory reports.

Not only confabulators claim to re-live their false memories, but they apparently do. That is, there is evidence that some confabulating patients act upon their confabulations as if they were true memories (e.g., looking for the phone number of a non-existing friend; see Schnider, 2003), or even show emotional responses congruent with the content of a confabulated event (e.g., crying for the alleged death of a work colleague). This complete adherence to confabulations may be related to the way patients subjectively experience their memories, given that people base their decisions and actions upon their own experience (Bechara, Damasio, Tranel, & Damasio, 1997; Goel & Dolan, 2003; Koriat & Goldsmith, 1996). Thus, understanding the nature of the subjective experience associated to memory retrieval in confabulators may contribute to our understanding of the mechanisms responsible for confabulatory behaviour.

The goal of the present study was to characterize the subjective experience of remembering in confabulating compared to non-confabulating patients and control participants. We attempted to achieve this goal by investigating three issues. First, we investigated the extent to which subjective measures of recollection (i.e., R responses in the R/K paradigm) behave similarly to objective behavioural indicators of recollection (i.e., memory for details surrounding the encoding context). Second, we examined the extent to which subjective measures of recollection are affected by variables that are known to affect objective measures of recollection in these three groups of participants. Third, we sought to identify the type of information that triggers remembering states in confabulators compared to the other participant groups.

To date, there exists substantial agreement that recollection is the process responsible for the retrieval of contextual details surrounding the original episode (e.g., Atkinson & Juola, 1974; Jacoby, 1991, Mandler, 1980, Tulving, 1985, Yonelinas, 1994). Recollection is distinguished from familiarity, which is the process that reflects the global strength of the memory trace without additional qualitative information (Yonelinas, 1994). Individuals are thought to be able to report on their subjective experience of recollection and to differentiate it from the experience of familiarity (Gardiner, Ramponi, & Richardson-Klavehn, 1998; Tulving, 1985). These different subjective experiences of retrieval have been operationalized in the R/K paradigm (Tulving, 1985), in which participants are asked to indicate, for each recognized item, whether they recollect some aspects of its presentation (i.e. R response) or whether the item is merely familiar (i.e. K response). Numerous behavioural (Gardiner, 1988; Gardiner & Parkin, 1990; Rajaram, 1993) and neuroimaging studies (Eldridge, Knowlton, Furmanski, Bookheimer, & Engel, 2000; Wheeler & Buckner, 2004; Woodruff, Johnson, Uncapher, & Rugg, 2005) provided evidence consistent with the hypothesis that R and K responses reflect qualitatively distinct memory processes underlying recognition performance (Yonelinas, 1994; but see Dunn, 2004, Wixted and Stretch, 2004).

If the R/K paradigm captures the distinction between recollection and familiarity, one should find that when R responses are provided individuals are more likely to correctly retrieve features of the encoding context than when K responses are provided. In other words, subjective and objective measures of recollection should be specifically associated. In line with this notion, Dudukovic and Knowlton (2006) have recently demonstrated that normal subjects were able to identify contextual details (i.e. the ink color) for remembered items, while their source accuracy for familiar items was at chance levels (but see Hicks, Marsh, & Ritschel, 2002). Similarly, Perfect and colleagues demonstrated that normal subjects were more likely to identify the temporal (i.e. the order of occurrence in the original list) and the spatial context (i.e. the quadrant of the screen in which the item had been presented) for remembered compared to familiar items (Perfect, Mayes, Downes, & Van Eijk, 1996). Moreover, ERP studies showed that the LPC, a late positive component that is typically associated to R responses (Duzel, Yonelinas, Mangun, Heinze, & Tulving, 1997), was detected only when both the source of an information and its content were retrieved (Rugg, Henson, & Robb, 2003; Senkfor & Van Petten, 1998).

Subjective estimates of recollection have also been compared to those obtained with the process dissociation procedure (Jacoby, 1991), in which recollection is measured as the ability to retrieve a specific aspect of the study event (e.g., when or where an item was presented) and to use this as a basis for controlled responding. The two procedures led to almost identical estimates of recollection, which were also similarly affected by encoding manipulations (Yonelinas, 2001). These results suggest that phenomenological experiences of recognition memory, that is R and K responses, function similarly to objective measures of recollection and familiarity in normal subjects.

Of importance, subjective and objective estimates of recollection also converge in some memory-impaired populations. Compared to normal controls, amnesic patients typically show a reduction of R responses in recognition tasks (Dalla Barba, 1997, Knowlton and Squire, 1995; Schacter, Verfaellie, & Pradere, 1996), and are also markedly impaired in objective measures of recollection, such as those derived from the process dissociation procedure (Verfaellie & Treadwell, 1993; Verfaellie, 1994), or the ROC analysis (Yonelinas, Kroll, Dobbins, Lazzara, & Knight, 1998; Yonelinas et al., 2002). Recently, however, dissociations between subjective and objective indicators of recollection have been observed. Duarte and colleagues found that patients with focal lesions in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) are impaired in recollecting the context in which items had been encountered, yet show seemingly unaltered remembering states (Duarte, Ranganath, & Knight, 2005).

The question of interest for the current research concerns the extent to which subjective and objective measures converge in confabulating patients. On the one hand, the frequent creation of false memories and attributions of true memories to wrong contexts are clear indicators of impaired (objective) recollection. On the other hand, the vividness with which these faulty memories appear to be experienced suggests that these patients may still have remembering states. It is possible, then, that the subjective experience of remembering and the objective ability to recollect qualitative features of the study context are not as tied in confabulating patients as they are in normal subjects. We have previously argued that whereas in normal subjects remembering states are thought to reflect the retrieval of contextual features of the learning episode, in confabulators remembering states may be triggered by vivid thoughts related to test items but independent of the context in which these items were originally encountered (Ciaramelli, Ghetti, Frattarelli, & Làdavas, 2006). If this is the case, the association between subjective and objective measures of recollection in confabulating patients should be lower than that observed in non-confabulating patients and normal controls. The question of whether or not subjective and objective measures of recollection converge in confabulating patients motivated Experiment 1.

One additional way to gain insight on the behaviour of subjective measures of recollection in confabulators compared to other participant groups is the investigation of how subjective and objective measures are affected by experimental manipulations that typically result in changes in recollection. The literature is rich of examples of such conditions (see Yonelinas, 2002 for a review). For example, the study of experimental manipulations at time of encoding has demonstrated that processing the meaning of the stimulus versus its perceptual features increases recollection more than familiarity (e.g., Gregg & Gardiner, 1994; Yonelinas, 2002). Further, divided versus full attention reduces recollection more than familiarity (e.g., Jacoby & Kelley, 1991; Yonelinas, 2001). Similar dissociations are also found following retrieval manipulations (e.g., Dodson & Johnson, 1996; Yonelinas & Jacoby, 1994).

If subjective measures of recollection rely on different processes than objective measures of recollection in confabulating patients compared to other groups, then conditions that support recollection should differentially affect subjective and objective measures in confabulating patients, but not in other groups. This question was also addressed in Experiment 1.

As a final issue, we sought to begin to shed light into the cause of the dissociation between objective and subjective measures of recollection in confabulation. As we described earlier, a dissociation between subjective and objective measures of recollection has been recently reported in DLPFC patients (Duarte et al., 2005). In this study, however, little emphasis was placed on the neuropsychological mechanism responsible for the dissociation. In this respect, a case-study by Curran and colleagues is quite revealing (Curran, Schacter, Norman, & Galluccio, 1997). They described a patient with a right DLPFC lesion, BG, whose R responses predominantly included associations made to the test words (e.g., for the word DISEASE: “I remember this word because one of the reasons I’m going to the dentist this afternoon is some gum disease”; Curran et al., 1997, p. 1041), rather than contextual aspects of the learning episode. These results suggest that BG's subjective experience of retrieval did not necessarily reflect any re-access to the study phase. We note, however, that BG (Curran et al., 1997), as well as patients studied by Duarte and colleagues (Duarte et al., 2005), did not suffer from confabulation, thus the mechanism underlying the dissociation between objective and subjective recollection in these patients may differ from the one operating in confabulating patients.

Our hypothesis is that confabulating patients still experience remembering states in the face of clearly impaired retrieval processes because remembering states in these patients reflect something different from the accessibility of information about the encoding context. As we discussed earlier, the experience of remembering in confabulators might be triggered by an excessive processing of context-irrelevant information related to test items during retrieval (Ciaramelli, Ghetti, Frattarelli et al., 2006). To test this hypothesis, we conducted Experiment 2, in which we asked participants to describe the contents of their R responses. Whereas R responses would be associated with the description of contextual features of the learning episode in other participant groups, such responses would be likely associated with extra-list contexts in confabulating patients (Gardiner et al., 1998).

Section snippets

Experiment 1

In this experiment, we investigated the relation between subjective and objective measures of recollection in confabulators, non-confabulating patients, and normal controls. In addition, we manipulated a variable known to affect recollection to a greater degree than familiarity, i.e. depth of processing. Thus, we had participants study words both in a shallow- and in a deep-encoding condition. Participants were tested with a typical R/K paradigm and for each recognized word they were also asked

Experiment 2

Participants were tested on a R/K recognition task and asked to characterize their R and K responses. In other words, they were not only asked to provide a R/K judgment, but also describe the content of their memories motivating R and K judgments (see also Norman & Schacter, 1997; Piolino et al., 2003). Based on the results of Experiment 1, we predicted that normal subjects and non-confabulating amnesics’ reports for R responses would involve contextual details pertaining to the presentation of

General discussion

Confabulators often claim to vividly remember events that did not actually happen (Dalla Barba, 1993; Moscovitch, 1989). In the present study, we were interested in characterizing the subjective experience of retrieval in these patients and its relation to the objective ability to recollect accurately qualitative aspects of the original episode. To do so, we investigated the extent to which subjective and objective measures of recollection behave similarly, and are affected by variables that

Acknowledgements

We thank Cristina Di Stefano, Giulia Giovagnoli, and Moreno Barbiani for rating participants’ transcripts, and Patrick Davidson and the reviewers for their comments on earlier versions of this manuscript.

References (81)

  • M.D. Kopelman et al.

    Temporal and spatial context memory in patients with focal frontal, temporal lobe, and diencephalic lesions

    Neuropsychologia

    (1997)
  • M. Moscovitch et al.

    Strategic retrieval and the frontal lobes: Evidence from confabulation and amnesia

    Neuropsychologia

    (1997)
  • M.D. Rugg et al.

    Neural correlates of retrieval processing in the prefrontal cortex during recognition and exclusion tasks

    Neuropsychologia

    (2003)
  • D.L. Schacter et al.

    The neuropsychology of memory illusions: False recall and recognition in amnesics patients

    Journal of Memory and Language

    (1996)
  • M.E. Wheeler et al.

    Functional-anatomic correlates of remembering and knowing

    Neuroimage

    (2004)
  • C.C. Woodruff et al.

    Content-specificity of the neural correlates of recollection

    Neuropsychologia

    (2005)
  • A.P. Yonelinas

    The nature of recollection and familiarity: A review of 30 years of research

    Journal of Memory and Language

    (2002)
  • A.P. Yonelinas et al.

    Noncriterial recollection: Familiarity as automatic, irrelevant recollection

    Consciousness and Cognition

    (1996)
  • D.M. Amodio et al.

    Meeting of minds: The medial prefrontal cortex and social cognition

    Nature Neuroscience Reviews

    (2006)
  • R.C. Atkinson et al.

    Search and decision processes in recognition memory

  • S. Baldassi et al.

    Visual clutter causes high-magnitude errors

    PLoS Biology

    (2006)
  • L. Barca et al.

    Word naming times and norms for age of acquisition, frequency, familiarity, imageability, concreteness, and other variables for Italian nouns

    Giornale Italiano di Psicologia

    (2001)
  • A. Bechara et al.

    Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy

    Science

    (1997)
  • O. Box et al.

    The evolution of spontaneous confabulation, delusion, misidentification and a related delusion in a case of severe head injury

    Neurocase

    (1999)
  • P.V. Burgess et al.

    Confabulation and the control of recollection

    Memory

    (1996)
  • H. Buschke et al.

    Evaluating storage, retention and retrieval in disordered memory and learning

    Neurology

    (1974)
  • E. Ciaramelli et al.

    Divided attention during retrieval suppresses false recognition in confabulation

  • M.A. Conway

    Sensory-perceptual episodic memory and its context: Autobiographical memory

    Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences

    (2001)
  • M.A. Conway et al.

    Motivated confabulation

    Neurocase

    (1996)
  • W.C. Culbertson et al.

    Tower of London

    (2001)
  • G. Dalla Barba

    Confabulation: Knowledge and recollective experience

    Cognitive Neuropsychology

    (1993)
  • G. Dalla Barba

    Confabulation: Remembering “another” past

  • G. Dalla Barba

    Recognition memory and recollective experience in Alzheimer's disease

    Memory

    (1997)
  • G. Dalla Barba et al.

    Confabulation: Remembering “another” past, planning “another” future

    Neurocase

    (1997)
  • C.S. Dodson et al.

    Some problems with the process-dissociation approach to memory

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: General

    (1996)
  • W. Donaldson

    The role of decision processes in remembering and knowing

    Memory & Cognition

    (1996)
  • A. Duarte et al.

    Effects of unilateral prefrontal lesions on familiarity, recollection, and source memory

    Journal of Neurosciences

    (2005)
  • J.C. Dunn

    Remember–Know: A matter of confidence

    Psychological Reviews

    (2004)
  • E. Duzel et al.

    Event-related brain potential correlates of two states of conscious awareness in memory

    Proceedings of the National Academy of Science

    (1997)
  • L.L. Eldridge et al.

    Remembering episodes: A selective role for the hippocampus during retrieval

    Nature Neuroscience

    (2000)
  • Cited by (0)

    View full text