Note
Supramodal language comprehension: Role of the left temporal lobe for listening and reading

We are deeply indebted to Prof. Detlef B. Linke and would like to dedicate this work to his memory.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.02.008Get rights and content

Abstract

In this fMRI study, we aimed at identifying the cortical areas engaged in supramodal processing of language comprehension. BOLD changes were recorded in 19 healthy right-handed subjects reading or listening to a story. During the visual control tasks the volunteers attended to a series of continuous letterstrings or a fixation cross, while during the acoustic control tasks either a reversed text or white noise were presented. The conjunction of the visual and acoustic story processing yielded left-dominant activations which in comparison to language-like stimuli focused to the left middle temporal gyrus as well as to the supramarginal gyrus. We conclude that the core structure representing supramodal language comprehension is the left temporal lobe at both banks of the superior temporal sulcus.

Introduction

Since the postmortem studies on aphasic patients by Broca and Wernicke, the left inferior frontal and parieto-temporal cortex have been implicated as critical cortical areas for language production and language comprehension (Broca, 1861, Graves, 1997, Lichtheim, 1885, Wernicke, 1874). The advent of functional neuroimaging methods has provided major modifications and refinements to this “Wernicke–Lichtheim model”. Furthermore, these methods yielded evidence for the representation of differentiated linguistic sub-functions in areas of the frontal and temporal lobes. However, with regard to the inferior frontal cortex, functional ascriptions of linguistic functions appear to be inconsistent in terms of function and exact localization (e.g., Dronkers, Wilkins, Van Valin, Redfern, & Jaeger, 2004; Grodzinsky, 2000, Muller, 2000; Muller & Basho, 2004). In addition, non-language functions were observed to map to the inferior frontal cortex including music perception (Maess, Koelsch, Gunter, & Friederici, 2001; Patel, 2003), the performance, imitation and observation of movements (Binkofski et al., 2000, Buccino et al., 2001; Carr, Iacoboni, Dubeau, Mazziotta, & Lenzi, 2003; Hamzei et al., 2003, Iacoboni et al., 1999; Leslie, Johnson-Frey, & Grafton, 2004; Rizzolatti & Arbib, 1998; Rizzolatti, Fogassi, & Gallese, 2001), working memory processes (Smith & Jonides, 1999), and the sequential processing of sounds (Gelfand & Bookheimer, 2003).

In the temporal lobe, lesion studies in primates and in humans, cortical mapping and functional imaging revealed areas related to processing of acoustic stimuli (Griffiths, Warren, Scott, Nelken, & King, 2004; Nelken, 2004; Ojemann, Schoenfield-McNeill, & Corina, 2002; Scott, 2005). Moreover, functional imaging studies underlined the role of temporal association areas in the perception of auditory verbal stimuli (Binder et al., 1999, Binder et al., 2000, Démonet et al., 1992; Démonet, Price, Wise, & Frackowiak, 1994; Narain et al., 2003, Perani et al., 1996, Price et al., 1996; Scott, Blank, Rosen, & Wise, 2000). That corresponds to the finding that the superior temporal sulcus accommodates specific representations for the processing of human voices (Belin, Zatorre, Lafaille, Ahad, & Pike, 2000). However, Hickok and co-workers presented first evidence that the planum temporale – comprising the perisylvian cortex in the occipital vicinity of the primary auditory area – seems to represent phonematic aspects of speech production as well (Hickok, 2001, Hickok et al., 2000). Thus, it was suggested that the planum temporale serves as an “auditory-to-meaning interface” for the construction of acoustically represented speech (Hickok & Poeppel, 2000).

The circuit involved in the comprehension of visually presented language stimuli is known to consist of widespread temporal and frontal regions (Crozier et al., 1999, Jessen et al., 1999, Joubert et al., 2004; Keller, Carpenter, & Just, 2001). With regard to the inferior frontal cortex, recent reviews hypothesized that this region comprises a higher-order interface rather than being responsible for distinct language-related functions (Bookheimer, 2002; Kaan & Swaab, 2002; Muller & Basho, 2004). In contrast, other authors tried to demonstrate separable functional, anatomical sub-divisions, or both (Fiebach, Schlesewsky, Lohmann, von Cramon, & Friederici, 2005; Friederici, Ruschemeyer, Hahne, & Fiebach, 2003; Poldrack et al., 1999; Suzuki & Sakai, 2003; Vigneau et al., 2006). With respect to the temporal lobe, previous studies demonstrated that it is involved in lexico-semantic as well as syntactical processing of written language comprehension (Binder et al., 2000; Mummery, Shallice, & Price, 1999; Rossell, Price, & Nobre, 2003; Vandenberghe, Price, Wise, Josephs, & Frackowiak, 1996).

Taken together, studies dealing with representations of either auditory or visual language comprehension suggest an involvement of lateral temporal and inferior frontal cortex. In addition, neurons in the surrounding area of the superior temporal sulcus have been shown to account for verbal memory functions independent of the input modality (Ojemann et al., 2002). From comparative anatomical studies it is known that the primate's temporal lobe receives input from auditory as well as visual primary and association cortices (Belin, Fecteau, & Bedard, 2004; Seltzer & Pandya, 1989).

Furthermore, there are a few recent neuroimaging studies on supramodal language comprehension. Like in the fMRI experiment we present in this article, Spitsyna and co-workers focused on the convergence of processing of spoken and written language in a PET study. They report distinct local representations of across-modality responses for intelligible language in both anterior and posterior cortical regions of the left temporal lobe (Spitsyna, Warren, Scott, Turkheimer, & Wise, 2006). Previously, an fMRI study was conducted to investigate the influence of sentence complexity and input modality on language comprehension (Constable et al., 2004). In accordance with the PET study's results, modality-independent language comprehension appeared to be strongly left-lateralized. But the shared circuit of the visual and acoustic domain did not map exclusively to the temporal lobe and its posterior vicinity as in the PET study. In addition, Constable and co-workers describe more extensive perisylvian regions including the inferior frontal gyrus.

Comparable to the PET study mentioned before, we used fMRI to examine whether there is an actual intersection for receptive language processing of both input modalities in the human brain. To test for the existence of such modality-independent brain regions, we combined visually and acoustically presented verbal stimuli in one paradigm to map the supramodal language processing area(s). According to the studies described above, we hypothesize this intersection to be found in the left temporal lobe.

Section snippets

Subjects

Twenty healthy subjects (10 females) were studied. All subjects had German as their first language and spoke English fluently. Further fluent languages were French (10 of 19), Spanish (8 of 19). They were students at the University in Bonn. Mean age was 26 years (20–29 years; SD = 2). They had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. None of them had a history of neurological or psychiatric disorder. Right-handedness was verified using the Edingburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971) translated

Visual task

When contrasted with the baseline condition (fixation cross), reading of the continuous text yielded widespread activation clusters in fronto-temporal regions bilaterally with a clear left-dominance (Table 1). In the left hemisphere, it encompassed both banks of the superior temporal gyrus in its whole rostro-occipital extension (Brodmann area [BA] 21, 38). Left inferior frontal gyrus (BA 44, 45, 47) and adjacent premotor cortex were involved (BA 6; in the vicinity of the supplementary motor

Supramodal processing

The representation of modality-independent language processing was present in the left inferior frontal and lateral temporal cortex. Language as well as language-like stimuli evoked responses in two clusters in the inferior frontal gyrus (BA 44/45 and BA 47) and extensively in the middle temporal gyrus (BA 38, 21 and adjacent 22). The core structure of modality-independent language processing, as revealed by contrasting comprehensible with unintelligible stimuli, focused on the left middle

Acknowledgement

We thank Prof. R.J. Seitz for helpful comments on the manuscript.

References (92)

  • A.L. Giraud et al.

    The contribution of visual areas to speech comprehension: A PET study in cochlear implants patients and normal-hearing subjects

    Neuropsychologia

    (2002)
  • T.D. Griffiths et al.

    Cortical processing of complex sound: A way forward?

    Trends Neurosci.

    (2004)
  • F. Hamzei et al.

    The human action recognition system and its relationship to Broca's area: An fMRI study

    Neuroimage

    (2003)
  • G. Hickok et al.

    A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of the role of left posterior superior temporal gyrus in speech production: Implications for the explanation of conduction aphasia

    Neurosci. Lett.

    (2000)
  • G. Hickok et al.

    Towards a functional neuroanatomy of speech perception

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2000)
  • G. Hickok et al.

    Dorsal and ventral streams: A framework for understanding aspects of the functional anatomy of language

    Cognition

    (2004)
  • F. Jessen et al.

    Activation of human language processing brain regions after the presentation of random letter strings demonstrated with event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging

    Neurosci. Lett.

    (1999)
  • S. Joubert et al.

    Neural correlates of lexical and sublexical processes in reading

    Brain Lang.

    (2004)
  • E. Kaan et al.

    The brain circuitry of syntactic comprehension

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2002)
  • M.P. Kirschen et al.

    Load- and practice-dependent increases in cerebro-cerebellar activation in verbal working memory: An fMRI study

    Neuroimage

    (2005)
  • S.A. Kotz et al.

    On the lateralization of emotional prosody: An event-related functional MR investigation

    Brain Lang.

    (2003)
  • K.R. Leslie et al.

    Functional imaging of face and hand imitation: Towards a motor theory of empathy

    Neuroimage

    (2004)
  • K. Mathiak et al.

    Discrimination of temporal information at the cerebellum: Functional magnetic resonance imaging of nonverbal auditory memory

    Neuroimage

    (2004)
  • R.A. Muller et al.

    Are nonlinguistic functions in “Broca's area” prerequisites for language acquisition? FMRI findings from an ontogenetic viewpoint

    Brain Lang.

    (2004)
  • C.J. Mummery et al.

    Dual-process model in semantic priming: A functional imaging perspective

    Neuroimage

    (1999)
  • I. Nelken

    Processing of complex stimuli and natural scenes in the auditory cortex

    Curr. Opin. Neurobiol.

    (2004)
  • T. Nichols et al.

    Valid conjunction inference with the minimum statistic

    Neuroimage

    (2005)
  • R.C. Oldfield

    The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory

    Neuropsychologia

    (1971)
  • R.A. Poldrack et al.

    Functional specialization for semantic and phonological processing in the left inferior prefrontal cortex

    Neuroimage

    (1999)
  • C.J. Price et al.

    Cognitive conjunction: A new approach to brain activation experiments.

    Neuroimage

    (1997)
  • A.M. Rapp et al.

    Neural correlates of metaphor processing

    Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res.

    (2004)
  • G. Rizzolatti et al.

    Language within our grasp

    Trends Neurosci.

    (1998)
  • S.L. Rossell et al.

    The anatomy and time course of semantic priming investigated by fMRI and ERPs

    Neuropsychologia

    (2003)
  • L. Scheef et al.

    Is the new selective Wernicke paradigm really selective for Wernicke's area?

    Neuroimage

    (2000)
  • S.K. Scott

    Auditory processing—Speech, space and auditory objects

    Curr. Opin. Neurobiol.

    (2005)
  • S.K. Scott et al.

    The neuroanatomical and functional organization of speech perception

    Trends Neurosci.

    (2003)
  • N. Tzourio-Mazoyer et al.

    Interindividual variability in the hemispheric organization for speech

    Neuroimage

    (2004)
  • M. Vigneau et al.

    Meta-analyzing left hemisphere language areas: Phonology, semantics, and sentence processing

    Neuroimage

    (2006)
  • R.J. Wise et al.

    Noun imageability and the temporal lobes

    Neuropsychologia

    (2000)
  • M. Ashtari et al.

    Left middle temporal gyrus activation during a phonemic discrimination task

    Neuroreport

    (2004)
  • P. Belin et al.

    ‘What’, ‘where’ and ‘how’ in auditory cortex

    Nat. Neurosci.

    (2000)
  • P. Belin et al.

    Voice-selective areas in human auditory cortex

    Nature

    (2000)
  • M.L. Berthier

    Transcortical aphasias

    (1999)
  • J.R. Binder et al.

    Conceptual processing during the conscious resting state. A functional MRI study

    J. Cogn. Neurosci.

    (1999)
  • J.R. Binder et al.

    Human temporal lobe activation by speech and nonspeech sounds

    Cereb. Cortex

    (2000)
  • F. Binkofski et al.

    Broca's region subserves imagery of motion: A combined cytoarchitectonic and fMRI study

    Hum. Brain Mapp.

    (2000)
  • Cited by (54)

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text