The basolateral amygdala modulates specific sensory memory representations in the cerebral cortex

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2008.10.010Get rights and content

Abstract

Stress hormones released by an experience can modulate memory strength via the basolateral amygdala, which in turn acts on sites of memory storage such as the cerebral cortex [McGaugh, J. L. (2004). The amygdala modulates the consolidation of memories of emotionally arousing experiences. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 1–28]. Stimuli that acquire behavioral importance gain increased representation in the cortex. For example, learning shifts the tuning of neurons in the primary auditory cortex (A1) to the frequency of a conditioned stimulus (CS), and the greater the level of CS importance, the larger the area of representational gain [Weinberger, N. M. (2007). Associative representational plasticity in the auditory cortex: A synthesis of two disciplines. Learning & Memory, 14(1–2), 1–16]. The two lines of research suggest that BLA strengthening of memory might be accomplished in part by increasing the representation of an environmental stimulus. The present study investigated whether stimulation of the BLA can affect cortical memory representations. In male Sprague–Dawley rats studied under urethane general anesthesia, frequency receptive fields were obtained from A1 before and up to 75 min after the pairing of a tone with BLA stimulation (BLAstm: 100 trials, 400 ms, 100 Hz, 400 μA [±16.54]). Tone started before and continued after BLAstm. Group BLA/1.0 (n = 16) had a 1 s CS–BLAstm interval while Group BLA/1.6 (n = 5) has a 1.6 s interval. The BLA/1.0 group did develop specific tuning shifts toward and to the CS, which could change frequency tuning by as much as two octaves. Moreover, its shifts increased over time and were enduring, lasting 75 min. However, group BLA/1.6 did not develop tuning shifts, indicating that precise CS–BLAstm timing is important in the anesthetized animal. Further, training in the BLA/1.0 paradigm but stimulating outside of the BLA did not produce tuning shifts. These findings demonstrate that the BLA is capable of exerting highly specific, enduring, learning-related modifications of stimulus representation in the cerebral cortex. These findings suggest that the ability of the BLA to alter specific cortical representations may underlie, at least in part, the modulatory influence of BLA activity on strengthening long-term memory.

Introduction

It is well known that memories for emotional events are more robust than memories for neutral events (McGaugh, 2004). For example, stimuli that are rated as emotionally salient are better remembered than stimuli that are rated as neutral (Bradley et al., 1992, Cahill et al., 1994, Kleinsmith et al., 1963, Sharot and Yonelinas, 2008). Many studies in both the human and animal literatures have established that post-training increases of stress can also enhance memory consolidation (Cahill and Alkire, 2003, Cahill et al., 2003, Hui et al., 2006, Kim et al., 2001, McCarty and Gold, 1981, Shors, 2001). Also, post-training systemic administration of stress hormones, such as corticosterone and epinephrine, is sufficient to enhance memory consolidation (Gold and van Buskirk, 1975, Gold et al., 1975, Hui et al., 2004, Kety, 1972, McGaugh and Roozendaal, 2002, Ogren et al., 1980, Ridley et al., 1981, Roozendaal et al., 1996).

Many lines of evidence have implicated the basolateral amygdala (BLA) as a substrate for stress-related modulation of memory. For example, post-training stimulation of the BLA is sufficient to induce enhanced memory consolidation for the training experience (Bergado et al., 2006, Gold et al., 1975). Further, post-training administration of stress hormones into the BLA enhances memory consolidation in a variety of tasks (Fekete et al., 1985, Gallagher and Kapp, 1981, Gallagher et al., 1977, Hui et al., 2004, Hui et al., 2006, Introini-Collison et al., 1989, Liang et al., 1986, Roozendaal, Castello, et al., 2008, Yang et al., 2006). Third, human imaging studies have also linked enhanced memory consolidation to amygdala activation. For example, PET and fMRI studies have shown that the level of activation in the BLA at the time of experiencing either negative or positive emotional experiences predicts the strength of memory at the time of recall (Cahill et al., 1996, Canli et al., 2000, Hamann et al., 1999, Hamann et al., 2002).

Animal studies have also linked physiological activation of the BLA to the modulation of emotional memory. Thus, emotional arousal in animals produces prolonged (hours) increased neuronal firing in the BLA (Pelletier, Likhtik, Filali, & Paré, 2005). Moreover, direct application of corticosterone to the BLA increases the excitability of its principal neurons (Duvarci & Paré, 2007). Direct drug infusion in the BLA can also enhance emotional memory (Fekete et al., 1985, Liang and Lee, 1988, Liang et al., 1990, Roozendaal and McGaugh, 1997, Roozendaal, Schelling, et al., 2008, Yang et al., 2006).

The enhancement of memory consolidation due to emotional learning and the application of stress hormones to the BLA are dependent on the activation of β-adrenergic receptors within this structure (LaLumiere et al., 2004, McGaugh, 1989, Quirarte et al., 1997, Roozendaal et al., 2002). Lesions of the BLA or infusions of β-adrenergic receptor antagonists into the BLA block enhancement of memory consolidation (Roozendaal and McGaugh, 1996, Roozendaal, Okuda, et al., 2006, Roozendaal, Hui, et al., 2006). However, the memory enhancing effects of stress, β-adrenergic receptor activation and other modulating treatments are not due to memory enhancement within the BLA itself. For example, lesions of the stria terminalis, efferents of BLA principle neurons, block the enhancement of memory consolidation due to the application of glucocorticoid receptor agonists to the BLA (Roozendaal and McGaugh, 1996, Setlow et al., 2000). Studies have identified the efferent sites of BLA modulatory effects, including the hippocampus, caudate, and cerebral cortex (McGaugh, 2002, Packard et al., 1994, Roozendaal and McGaugh, 1996).

BLA stimulation enhances cortical activation (Dringenberg et al., 2001, Dringenberg and Vanderwolf, 1996). Previous studies have also found evidence for other BLA interactions with cortical function. For example, stimulation of the BLA enhances cortical LTP (Dringenberg, Kuo, & Tomaszek, 2004). However, the means by which the BLA modulates memory in target structures, such as the cortex, are unknown. To address this issue, it would be helpful to identify a memory trace and then observe its modulation by the BLA. Such a candidate for a memory trace is available in the form of CS-specific associatively induced receptive field plasticity in the primary auditory cortex (A1) (Weinberger, 2004).

Classical and instrumental conditioning specifically modify the representations of sounds in A1 that acquire behavioral significance. Such learning causes the frequency receptive fields of cortical neurons to shift toward and to the frequency of the conditioned stimulus (Bakin and Weinberger, 1990, Gao and Suga, 1998, Kisley and Gerstein, 2001). These tuning shifts produce an increase in the number of neurons that become best tuned to the CS frequency, thus increasing the area of representation of the CS frequency. The magnitude of gain in the CS representation is an increasing function of the level of behavioral importance of the CS (Rutkowski and Weinberger, 2005, Weinberger, 2007). Thus, the amount of representational expansion may serve as a memory code for the learned behavioral significance of stimuli (Weinberger, 2001, Weinberger, 2003).

Associative representational plasticity has the major attributes of memory itself and therefore, may constitute a neural memory trace. It is associative (requires pairing), highly specific (responses to frequencies a small fraction of an octave away are attenuated) (Bakin et al., 1992, Bakin and Weinberger, 1990), discriminative (shifts to a CS+, loses response to a CS−) (Edeline & Weinberger, 1993), develops rapidly (within 5 trials) (Edeline, Pham, & Weinberger, 1993), consolidates (becomes stronger over days without further training) (Galván & Weinberger, 2002) and is enduring (tracked as long as 8 weeks) (Weinberger, Javid, & Lepan, 1993). In toto, these attributes of associative representational plasticity satisfy the criteria for constituting a neural memory trace (Weinberger, 2004).

As the BLA modulates memories, including those stored at least in part in the cerebral cortex, and as associatively induced CS-specific tuning shifts may constitute cortical representations of memory, then the BLA may modulate memories by promoting specific tuning shifts in the cortex. The goal of this study was to conduct an initial test of this hypothesis. A tone was paired with stimulation of the BLA to determine if it is capable of producing CS-specific tuning shifts similar to those that develop during behavioral learning.

Section snippets

Subjects

Subjects were 21 male Sprague–Dawley rats (320–500 g, Charles River Laboratories, Wilmington, MA) individually housed in a vivarium (maintained at 22 °C, 12/12 h light-dark cycle, on at 7:15 a.m.) with ad libitum access to food and water. All procedures were performed in accordance with the University of California Irvine Animal Research Committee and the NIH Animal Welfare guidelines.

Surgical procedures and electrophysiology

The entirety of the experiment was conducted with the rats under general anesthesia (urethane, 1.6 g/kg, i.p.).

Location of stimulating electrodes

Fig. 3 summarizes the placements of all stimulating electrodes. Of the 16 animals that were trained in the BLA/1.0 protocol, 11 had electrode placements within the BLA. The five animals with placements outside of the BLA failed to exhibit EEG desynchronization and served as a nonBLA stimulation control group, referred to hereafter as the nonBLA/1.0 group. All five of the animals trained in the BLA/1.6 protocol had placements within the BLA.

Effects of BLA stimulation on frequency tuning

Stimulation of the BLA did induce frequency tuning

Summary and validity of findings

The results demonstrate that electrical stimulation of the BLA is capable of producing shifts of frequency tuning in the primary auditory cortex. Moreover, this receptive field plasticity is directed toward the tonal frequency with which it was paired (the “CS”). Furthermore, these induced specific tuning shifts are long-lasting (retained for 75 min, the longest period tested) and their magnitude increases over time in the absence of further pairing (i.e., they consolidate). These attributes of

Acknowledgments

We thank Drew B. Headley, Gabriel K. Hui and Jacquie Weinberger for assistance. This study was funded by the NIDCD Grant #DC-05592 (N.M.W.), NIMH Grant #MH-12526 (J.L.M.) and the APA/DPN fellowship #5-T32-MH-18882 (C.M.C.).

References (101)

  • P.E. Gold et al.

    Facilitation of time-dependent memory processes with posttrial epinephrine injections

    Behavioral Biology

    (1975)
  • P.E. Gold et al.

    Effects of hormones on time-dependent memory storage processes

    Progress in Brain Research

    (1975)
  • T. Hatfield et al.

    Norepinephrine infused into the basolateral amygdala posttraining enhances retention in a spatial water maze task

    Neurobiology of Learning and Memory

    (1999)
  • G.K. Hui et al.

    Memory enhancement of classical fear conditioning by post-training injections of corticosterone in rats

    Neurobiology of Learning and Memory

    (2004)
  • I.R. Hui et al.

    Posttraining handling facilitates memory for auditory-cue fear conditioning in rats

    Neurobiology of Learning and Memory

    (2006)
  • I.B. Introini-Collison et al.

    Memory enhancement with intra-amygdala post-training naloxone is blocked by concurrent administration of propranolol

    Brain Research

    (1989)
  • K.C. Liang et al.

    Modulating effects of posttraining epinephrine on memory: Involvement of the amygdala noradrenergic system

    Brain Research

    (1986)
  • K.C. Liang et al.

    Involvement of amygdala pathways in the influence of post-training intra-amygdala norepinephrine and peripheral epinephrine on memory storage

    Brain Research

    (1990)
  • Z.L. et al.

    Human auditory primary and association cortex have differing lifetimes for activation traces

    Brain Research

    (1992)
  • R. McCarty et al.

    Plasma catecholamines: Effects of footshock level and hormonal modulators of memory storage

    Hormones and Behavior

    (1981)
  • J.L. McGaugh

    Memory consolidation and the amygdala: A systems perspective

    Trends in Neurosciences

    (2002)
  • J.L. McGaugh et al.

    Role of adrenal stress hormones in forming lasting memories in the brain

    Current Opinion in Neurobiology

    (2002)
  • A.A. Miasnikov et al.

    Motivationally neutral stimulation of the nucleus basalis induces specific behavioral memory

    Neurobiology of Learning and Memory

    (2008)
  • A.A. Miasnikov et al.

    Rapid induction of specific associative behavioral memory by stimulation of the nucleus basalis in the rat

    Neurobiology of Learning and Memory

    (2006)
  • A.A. Miasnikov et al.

    Specific auditory memory induced by nucleus basalis stimulation depends on intrinsic acetylcholine

    Neurobiology of Learning and Memory

    (2008)
  • S.O. Ogren et al.

    Evidence for a role of the locus coeruleus noradrenaline system in learning

    Neuroscience Letters

    (1980)
  • D.P. Phillips et al.

    Central auditory onset responses, and temporal asymmetries in auditory perception

    Hearing Research

    (2002)
  • G.L. Quirarte et al.

    Norepinephrine release in the amygdala in response to footshock and opioid peptidergic drugs

    Brain Research

    (1998)
  • R.M. Ridley et al.

    A new approach to the role of noradrenaline in learning: Problem-solving in the marmoset after alpha-noradrenergic receptor blockade

    Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior

    (1981)
  • B. Roozendaal et al.

    Noradrenergic activation of the basolateral amygdala modulates consolidation of object recognition memory

    Neurobiology of Learning and Memory

    (2008)
  • B. Roozendaal et al.

    Basolateral amygdala noradrenergic activity mediates corticosterone-induced enhancement of auditory fear conditioning

    Neurobiology of Learning and Memory

    (2006)
  • B. Roozendaal et al.

    The memory-modulatory effects of glucocorticoids depend on an intact stria terminalis

    Brain Research

    (1996)
  • B. Roozendaal et al.

    Glucocorticoid receptor agonist and antagonist administration into the basolateral but not central amygdala modulates memory storage

    Neurobiology of Learning and Memory

    (1997)
  • R.G. Rutkowski et al.

    Characterisation of multiple physiological fields within the anatomical core of rat auditory cortex

    Hearing Research

    (2003)
  • T. Sharot et al.

    Differential time-dependent effects of emotion on recollective experience and memory for contextual information

    Cognition

    (2008)
  • T.J. Shors

    Acute stress rapidly and persistently enhances memory formation in the male rat

    Neurobiology of Learning and Memory

    (2001)
  • J.F. Smiley et al.

    Monoaminergic–cholinergic interactions in the primate basal forebrain

    Neuroscience

    (1999)
  • N.M. Weinberger

    The nucleus basalis and memory codes: Auditory cortical plasticity and the induction of specific, associative behavioral memory

    Neurobiology of Learning and Memory

    (2003)
  • N.M. Weinberger et al.

    The level of cholinergic nucleus basalis activation controls the specificity of auditory associative memory

    Neurobiology of Learning and Memory

    (2006)
  • J.S. Bakin et al.

    Induction of a physiological memory in the cerebral cortex by stimulation of the nucleus basalis

    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America

    (1996)
  • S. Bao et al.

    Cortical remodelling induced by activity of ventral tegmental dopamine neurons

    Nature

    (2001)
  • J.A. Bergado et al.

    Stimulation of the basolateral amygdala improves the acquisition of a motor skill

    Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience

    (2006)
  • D.J. Berlau et al.

    Basolateral amygdala lesions do not prevent memory of context-footshock training

    Learning & Memory

    (2003)
  • T.S. Bjordahl et al.

    Induction of long-term receptive field plasticity in the auditory cortex of the waking guinea pig by stimulation of the nucleus basalis

    Behavioral Neuroscience

    (1998)
  • M.M. Bradley et al.

    Remembering pictures: Pleasure and arousal in memory

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition

    (1992)
  • G. Buzsaki et al.

    Nucleus basalis and thalamic control of neocortical activity in the freely moving rat

    Journal of Neuroscience

    (1988)
  • L. Cahill et al.

    Enhanced human memory consolidation with post-learning stress: Interaction with the degree of arousal at encoding

    Learning & Memory

    (2003)
  • L. Cahill et al.

    Amygdala activity at encoding correlated with long-term, free recall of emotional information

    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America

    (1996)
  • L. Cahill et al.

    Beta-adrenergic activation and memory for emotional events

    Nature

    (1994)
  • T. Canli et al.

    Event-related activation in the human amygdala associates with later memory for individual emotional experience

    Journal of Neuroscience

    (2000)
  • Cited by (80)

    • The auditory cortex and the emotional valence of sounds

      2019, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
    • Inactivation of the interpositus nucleus blocks the acquisition of conditioned responses and timing changes in conditioning-specific reflex modification of the rabbit eyeblink response

      2018, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory
      Citation Excerpt :

      These findings therefore suggest plasticity in the CE is not required for amygdalar modulation of CRs or CRM. Alternatively, other amygdalar nuclei that output to the CE may be involved, such as the lateral or basolateral nuclei, which have been implicated as sites of acquisition-related plasticity that can initiate plasticity in other brain areas involved in maintenance of those learned responses (Chau and Galvez, 2012; Chavez, McGaugh, & Weinberger, 2009; Johansen, Cain, Ostroff, & LeDoux, 2011; Maren, Yap, & Goosens, 2001; McIntyre, Power, Roozendaal, & McGaugh, 2003; Poremba and Gabriel, 1999). Other candidate sites for associative modification of the UR are cerebral cortical areas such as the motor cortex (MC) or medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC).

    • Memory Modulation

      2017, Learning and Memory: A Comprehensive Reference
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text