Gamma band plasticity in sensory cortex is a signature of the strongest memory rather than memory of the training stimulus

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2013.05.001Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Gamma waves index coordinated neuronal activity, more effective than uncoordinated.

  • Tone plus stimulation of the nucleus basalis implanted associative memory in rats.

  • Post-training generalization gradients revealed individual differences in tone memory.

  • Gamma increase in primary auditory cortex was linked to the strongest tone in memory.

  • Thus, increased neural coordination may be a substrate for specific memory contents.

Abstract

Gamma oscillations (∼30–120 Hz) are considered to be a reflection of coordinated neuronal activity, linked to processes underlying synaptic integration and plasticity. Increases in gamma power within the cerebral cortex have been found during many cognitive processes such as attention, learning, memory and problem solving in both humans and animals. However, the specificity of gamma to the detailed contents of memory remains largely unknown. We investigated the relationship between learning-induced increased gamma power in the primary auditory cortex (A1) and the strength of memory for acoustic frequency. Adult male rats (n = 16) received three days (200 trials each) of pairing a tone (3.66 kHz) with stimulation of the nucleus basalis, which implanted a memory for acoustic frequency as assessed by associatively-induced disruption of ongoing behavior, viz., respiration. Post-training frequency generalization gradients (FGGs) revealed peaks at non-CS frequencies in 11/16 cases, likely reflecting normal variation in pre-training acoustic experiences. A stronger relationship was found between increased gamma power and the frequency with the strongest memory (peak of the difference between individual post- and pre-training FGGs) vs. behavioral responses to the CS training frequency. No such relationship was found for the theta/alpha band (4–15 Hz). These findings indicate that the strength of specific increased neuronal synchronization within primary sensory cortical fields can determine the specific contents of memory.

Introduction

A major advance in broadening our understanding of the neural substrates of learning and memory has involved a shift in emphasis from linear stimulus–response circuits to neural networks. Following Hebb’s insights (Hebb, 1949), it is now generally accepted that coordinated neuronal activity forms during learning to represent and store relevant information, serve cognition and ultimately behavioral action. Gamma frequency oscillations (∼30–120 Hz) are thought to reflect the synchronous activity of neurons both within and across cortical fields (Buzsáki & Wang, 2012). The timescale of gamma oscillations is appropriate for synaptic integration (Salinas and Sejnowski, 2000, Volgushev et al., 1998) and spike timing dependent plasticity (STDP) (Bi and Poo, 1998, Isaac et al., 2009, Wespatat et al., 2004). Particularly relevant to the domain of learning and memory, increased gamma activity has been linked to processes such as attention (Börgers, Epstein, & Kopell, 2008) and short-term memory (Lutzenberger et al., 2002, Pesaran et al., 2002). Moreover, the level of gamma activity at the time of encoding can predict the degree of later recall (Fell and Axmacher, 2011, Osipova et al., 2006, Sederberg et al., 2006, Sederberg et al., 2007). An increase in cortical gamma power also develops during simple associative auditory classical conditioning in humans (Miltner, Braun, Arnold, Witte, & Taub, 1999), underscoring its ubiquity and the potential applicability of appropriate animal models to mechanisms of human learning.

Animal models of associative learning have identified candidate neural substrates for the representation and storage of signal stimuli in the cerebral cortex. For example when a tone is paired with a reinforcer, receptive fields (RF) in the primary auditory cortex (A1) shift to emphasize the frequency of the conditioned stimulus (CS) (Bakin and Weinberger, 1990, Edeline and Weinberger, 1993, Gao and Suga, 2000, Kisley and Gerstein, 2001). Such representational plasticity has the main attributes of associative memory: associativity, specificity, consolidation and long-term retention (reviewed in Weinberger, 2007). Gamma activity may play a critical role in the development of cortically based associative learning. For example, an increase in gamma power within A1 predicts both specific CS-directed cortical plasticity and also behaviorally validated learning 24 h later, but only during initial acquisition, not during maintenance of the memory (Headley and Weinberger, 2011, Weinberger et al., 2006).

Heretofore, it has been assumed tacitly that enhanced gamma activity induced by a signal stimulus during learning reflects the increased salience or behavioral relevance of that stimulus, e.g., the CS in simple associative learning. However, there is an alternative possibility. It is well known that even when perceptual, acquisition and storage processes are functioning optimally, the content of the resultant memory can differ from the actual experience. Subjects trained identically do not all acquire the exact same content (Bieszczad and Weinberger, 2010b, Bieszczad and Weinberger, 2010a, Bieszczad and Weinberger, 2012, Ohl et al., 2001, Polley et al., 2006). Therefore, the greatest increase in gamma activity may actually reflect the stimulus that has gained the greatest strength through learning, rather than the training stimulus. The relative strength of memory for different stimuli along a sensory dimension cannot be determined during training, but rather depends on obtaining post-training stimulus generalization gradients (Bouton, 2007, Mostofsky, 1965). Discrimination learning (i.e., reinforced CS+ with non-reinforced CS−) is a well-documented example. The peak of the post-training generalization gradient is generally not at the CS+ but is displaced to a stimulus value that is farther away from the CS− (Purtle, 1973). Such “peak shift” has been thought to reflect the summation of an excitatory neural gradient centered on the CS+ and an inhibitory neural gradient centered on the CS− (Spence, 1937).

Recently, we found such shifted generalization peaks in simple associative conditioning, due to pre-training exposure to various tones that induced an inhibitory neural gradient in primary auditory cortex (A1) (Miasnikov & Weinberger, 2012). This disjunction between the training frequency and the peak of the generalization gradient provides a unique opportunity to determine whether enhanced gamma activity during learning is tied to the CS frequency or to the peak of the generalization gradient, i.e., to the tone that is most strongly represented in memory. If increased gamma activity reflects increased neural synchrony that is part of the substrate of auditory frequency memory, then the greatest increase in gamma should be tightly linked to the strongest memory, regardless of the training frequency. We report here the analysis of changes in gamma activity that had been recorded during the previous study. The same changes in gamma were analyzed two ways: based on the CS training frequency and based on the peak of the generalization gradient.

Section snippets

Materials and methods

As the present analysis concerns EEG activity obtained in our previous study (Miasnikov & Weinberger, 2012), the materials and methods are mainly the same and will be summarized briefly. All procedures were performed in accordance with the University of California, Irvine, Animal Research Committee and the NIH Animal Welfare guidelines. During training and testing, subjects were continuously monitored by video cameras.

Behavior

Pairing tone and NBstm produced some significant changes from the pre-training (Day 2) respiratory responses. Fig. 2A presents group pre- and post-training tone frequency generalization gradients (FGGs). Prior to training, responses were not uniform but were greatest at 8.22 kHz for the group. Regardless, as the tones used both pre- and post-training were identical, it is the difference between the pre- and post-FGGs that reveals the effects of training.

The post-training FGG did not differ from

Resume of gamma findings

This report focuses on the relationship between associatively induced gamma band activity in sensory cortex and behaviorally validated memory. The latter was implanted by pairing a tone with stimulation of the nucleus basalis. Previously, increased gamma activity had been identified as a neural signature of association, e.g., when a sensory stimulus develops the ability to predict a reinforcer (Gruber et al., 2001, Headley and Weinberger, 2011, McLin et al., 2003, Miltner et al., 1999). Such a

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to Steven Clifford (CED) for specialized software development, Gabriel K. Hui for assistance with the manuscript and Jacquie Weinberger for citation research. This study was funded by NIDCD/NIH DC-02938 to NMW.

References (71)

  • T. Gruber et al.

    Modulation of induced gamma band responses and phase synchrony in a paired associate learning task in the human EEG

    Neuroscience Letters

    (2001)
  • A.A. Miasnikov et al.

    Motivationally neutral stimulation of the nucleus basalis induces specific behavioral memory

    Neurobiology of Learning and Memory

    (2008)
  • A.A. Miasnikov et al.

    Rapid induction of specific associative behavioral memory by stimulation of the nucleus basalis in the rat

    Neurobiology of Learning and Memory

    (2006)
  • A.A. Miasnikov et al.

    Specific auditory memory induced by nucleus basalis stimulation depends on intrinsic acetylcholine

    Neurobiology of Learning and Memory

    (2008)
  • A.A. Miasnikov et al.

    Consolidation and long-term retention of an implanted behavioral memory

    Neurobiology of Learning and Memory

    (2011)
  • A.A. Miasnikov et al.

    Detection of an inhibitory cortical gradient underlying peak shift in learning: A neural basis for a false memory

    Neurobiology of Learning and Memory

    (2012)
  • T. Moriizumi et al.

    Separate neuronal populations of the rat globus pallidus projecting to the subthalamic nucleus, auditory cortex and pedunculopontine tegmental area

    Neuroscience

    (1992)
  • P.B. Sederberg et al.

    Oscillatory correlates of the primacy effect in episodic memory

    NeuroImage

    (2006)
  • M. Volgushev et al.

    Modification of discharge patterns of neocortical neurons by induced oscillations of the membrane potential

    Neuroscience

    (1998)
  • N.M. Weinberger

    The nucleus basalis and memory codes: Auditory cortical plasticity and the induction of specific, associative behavioral memory

    Neurobiology of Learning and Memory

    (2003)
  • N.M. Weinberger

    The medial geniculate, not the amygdala, as the root of auditory fear conditioning

    Hearing Research

    (2011)
  • N.M. Weinberger et al.

    The level of cholinergic nucleus basalis activation controls the specificity of auditory associative memory

    Neurobiology of Learning and Memory

    (2006)
  • N.M. Weinberger et al.

    Sensory memory consolidation observed: Increased specificity of detail over days

    Neurobiology of Learning and Memory

    (2009)
  • F.A. Wilson et al.

    Neuronal responses related to reinforcement in the primate basal forebrain

    Brain Research

    (1990)
  • M. Ainsworth et al.

    Dual γ rhythm generators control interlaminar synchrony in auditory cortex

    Journal of Neuroscience

    (2011)
  • J.S. Bakin et al.

    Induction of a physiological memory in the cerebral cortex by stimulation of the nucleus basalis

    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America

    (1996)
  • G.Q. Bi et al.

    Synaptic modifications in cultured hippocampal neurons: Dependence on spike timing, synaptic strength, and postsynaptic cell type

    Journal of Neuroscience

    (1998)
  • K.M. Bieszczad et al.

    Extinction reveals that primary sensory cortex predicts reinforcement outcome

    European Journal of Neuroscience

    (2012)
  • T.S. Bjordahl et al.

    Induction of long-term receptive field plasticity in the auditory cortex of the waking guinea pig by stimulation of the nucleus basalis

    Behavioral Neuroscience

    (1998)
  • C. Börgers et al.

    Gamma oscillations mediate stimulus competition and attentional selection in a cortical network model

    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America

    (2008)
  • M.E. Bouton

    Learning and behavior: A contemporary synthesis

    (2007)
  • G. Buzsáki et al.

    The origin of extracellular fields and currents—EEG, ECoG, LFP and spikes

    Nature Reviews Neuroscience

    (2012)
  • G. Buzsáki et al.

    Mechanisms of gamma oscillations

    Annual Review of Neuroscience

    (2012)
  • E.G. Cape et al.

    Effects of glutamate agonist versus procaine microinjections into the basal forebrain cholinergic cell area upon gamma and theta EEG activity and sleep−wake state

    European Journal of Neuroscience

    (2000)
  • G.G. Celesia et al.

    Acetylcholine released from cerebral cortex in relation to state of activation

    Neurology

    (1966)
  • Cited by (22)

    • Deciphering the code: Identifying true gamma neural oscillations

      2022, Experimental Neurology
      Citation Excerpt :

      In humans, gamma oscillatory activity has been shown to correlate with various functional processes, including sensory representation (Gray and Singer, 1989; Hanslmayr et al., 2007; Ribary et al., 1991), learning (Popescu et al., 2009), working memory (Howard et al., 2003; Roux et al., 2012), emotional processing (Güntekin and Başar, 2014; Headley and Paré, 2013), selective attention (Benchenane et al., 2011; Fell et al., 2003) and perception (Tallon-Baudry and Bertrand, 1999). While the precise mechanisms by which gamma oscillations are involved in such processes are not entirely clear, synaptic integration and spike timing dependent plasticity may play an important role (Weinberger et al., 2013). In light of such studies, gamma oscillations have emerged as a likely candidate for mediating neural integration and information processing related to sensory and cognitive processing.

    • Sensory cortical and subcortical auditory neurophysiological changes predict cue-specific extinction behavior enabled by the pharmacological inhibition of an epigenetic regulator during memory formation

      2021, Brain Research Bulletin
      Citation Excerpt :

      Frequency-specific extinction memory associated with signal-specific auditory cortical and subcortical plasticity replicates the pattern of plasticity associated with frequency-specific memory in a tone-reward training task (Bieszczad et al., 2015; Rotondo and Bieszczad, 2020). Signal-specific narrowing of auditory cortical tuning bandwidth is in agreement with studies demonstrating that plasticity in auditory cortical frequency representations reflects what is actually learned about sound features, rather than pure stimulus statistics of the acoustic experience (Polley et al., 2006; Weinberger et al., 2013). Further, changes in the ABR validate that signal-specific auditory subcortical plasticity is associated with frequency-specific memory.

    • CS-specific modifications of auditory evoked potentials in the behaviorally conditioned rat

      2017, Brain Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      Thus, a reduction in the P2 latency may represent a signature component of the enhanced cortical synchrony that often results from associative learning. Changes in cortical EEG spectral power following both visual and auditory conditioning are well substantiated (Jeschke et al., 2008; Chavez et al., 2013; Weinberger et al., 2013). Like AEP plasticity, gamma band activity changes also exhibit CS-specificity and fail to occur in unpaired control groups (Headley and Weinberger, 2011, 2013).

    • Learning strategy refinement reverses early sensory cortical map expansion but not behavior: Support for a theory of directed cortical substrates of learning and memory

      2015, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory
      Citation Excerpt :

      We introduce here aspects of CONCERTO, as the framework of a theory for brain, learning and memory. The complete CONCERTO model posits both system level and circuit–cell–molecule levels of explanation, aspects of which have been published elsewhere: e.g., neuromodulators/muscarinic receptors (Bieszczad et al., 2013; McLin et al., 2002; Miasnikov, Chen, & Weinberger, 2008; Weinberger et al., 2006; see also Edeline, 2012; Edeline, Manunta, & Hennevin, 2011), and neuronal synchrony and gamma band activity (Headley & Weinberger, 2011, 2013; Weinberger, Miasnikov, Bieszczad, & Chen, 2013). We are able to present here only major elements of its system level formulation.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text