Elsevier

Progress in Neurobiology

Volume 74, Issue 3, October 2004, Pages 127-166
Progress in Neurobiology

The supramammillary area: its organization, functions and relationship to the hippocampus

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2004.09.003Get rights and content

Abstract

The supramammillary area of the hypothalamus, although small in size, can have profound modulatory effects on the hippocampal formation and related temporal cortex. It can control hippocampal plasticity and also has recently been shown to contain cells that determine the frequency of hippocampal rhythmical slow activity (theta rhythm). We review here its organization and anatomical connections providing an atlas and a new nomenclature. We then review its functions particularly in relation to its links with the hippocampus. Much of its control of behaviour and its differential activation by specific classes of stimuli is consistent with a tight relationship with the hippocampus. However, its ascending connections involve not only caudal areas of the cortex with close links to the hippocampus but also reciprocal connections with more rostral areas such as the infralimbic and anterior cingulate cortices. These latter areas appear to be the most rostral part of a network that, via the medial septum, hippocampus and lateral septum, is topographically mapped into the hypothalamus. The supramammillary area is thus diffusely connected with areas that control emotion and cognition and receives more topographically specific return information from areas that control cognition while also receiving ascending information from brain stem areas involved in emotion. We suggest that it is a key part of a network that recursively transforms information to achieve integration of cognitive and emotional aspects of goal-directed behaviour.

Introduction

The supramammillary area (SuM) is a relatively thin, chemically distinct, layer of cells overlying the mammillary bodies in the hypothalamus. While relatively few in number, the cells of this area have recently become of interest as they appear to exert significant modulatory control over the hippocampus and, directly and via the hippocampus, substantial portions of the telencephalon. Thus, like the raphe serotonergic system and the coerulear noradrenergic system, SuM may have functional importance out of all proportion to its size. Its connection with the hippocampus is made early in primate development and it has been suggested that “the early ingrowth of the excitatory SuM–hippocampal system in human and non-human primates may contribute to the prenatal activity-dependent development of the hippocampal formation”, perhaps as a result of its control of hippocampal theta activity (Berger et al., 2001).

It contrasts with the mammillary bodies in being much more a source of input to archicortex (particularly hippocampus) than a receptor of output from it. It also contrasts, as far as can be told, with many other hypothalamic areas in being a specific controller of hippocampal theta activity. It is, then, located in an area of the brain usually associated with emotion and motivation and not only connects with areas of the brain thought to be involved in predominantly cognitive functions but controls a pattern of brain activity (theta) that has been postulated to be important for control of cognition and particularly memory. We argue, on several different grounds, therefore, that its modulation of telencephalic activity is fundamental to cognitive–emotional interactions.

This claim is subject to two caveats. First, we are claiming only a modulatory role for SuM. It is likely to pace information around circuits but not to encode and transform that information itself. It is likely to determine whether other inputs produce plasticity in target structures but not to induce plasticity solely through its own inputs. Second, as will become clear, it is likely to be only the first discovered of a number of (perhaps topographically organized) structures that function in this specific way.

To both make our claim, and flesh out our caveats, we analyse the anatomy, physiology and contributions to behavioural function of SuM. We focus, in particular, on the extent to which SuM may discharge behaviourally important functions through its afferent and efferent connections with the hippocampal formation. Our conclusions may seem to present SuM as fundamentally controlling hippocampal function—but not only must SuM be only one of several structures that control the hippocampus (each, we argue under different behavioural or cognitive circumstances) but also the hippocampus must be only one of many widely separated structures modulated by SuM. Some of the parallels we identify between SuM and hippocampal function are likely, therefore, to be functions that depend on the joint activity of many “limbic” structures.

Our specific link with the hippocampus is made by the fact that SuM has recently been shown to contain cells that can control plasticity in the hippocampus via monosynaptic input. It also contains other cells that control the frequency of the rhythmic phasic firing of hippocampal cells (theta activity) via a relay in the medial septum (MS). These two types of interaction with the hippocampus are functionally separate and anatomically distinct. But they are of such a kind (controlling general tone and controlling the timing of firing of large populations of cells) that it is likely that SuM neurones play a much more important modulatory role in hippocampal function than their paucity and distance from the hippocampus might suggest. SuM also has similar extensive connections with many other structures and may, then, similarly modulate many areas of the forebrain.

The hippocampus has been postulated to contribute to cognitive functions such as spatial mapping (O’Keefe and Nadel, 1978) and relational memory (Cohen and Eichenbaum, 1993) and emotional functions (Papez, 1937), particularly behavioural inhibition (Gray, 1982, Gray and McNaughton, 2000). All components of the hippocampal formation, including the entorhinal and posterior cingulate cortex as well as hippocampus proper (Gray and McNaughton, 2000, Leung and Borst, 1987), show theta activity. This can be viewed as regular phasic firing controlled by pacemaker impulses relayed to the hippocampal formation from the MS (Brucke et al., 1959, Gogolák et al., 1968, Petsche et al., 1962, Tömböl and Petsche, 1969). Thus, the time at which the population of cells in the hippocampus fires is strongly influenced by afferent theta activity that produces regular changes in membrane potential (Bland, 1986). This control could be viewed as septal input actively eliciting theta activity in the hippocampus. However, it is probably better viewed as a temporal gating of activity by phasic inhibitory input (Leung, 1998). Thus when cells are permitted to fire is determined largely by phasic release from inhibition (Smythe et al., 1992), but it is also influenced by other factors (O’Keefe and Recce, 1993, Bose and Recce, 2001). By contrast, which cells fire during the appointed time is determined by other specifically patterned inputs. Theta activity is likely to play a significant specific role in the cognitive and/or emotional functions of the hippocampal formation (O’Keefe and Nadel, 1978, Gray, 1982, Miller, 1991, Gray and McNaughton, 2000) as well, perhaps, as a more general role in the control of sensori-motor integration (Bland and Oddie, 2002, Oddie and Bland, 1998) or sensory inhibition (Sainsbury, 1998). The postulated power of the control of hippocampus (and other areas like entorhinal, posterior cingulate and possibly perirhinal cortex) by SuM rests in the possibility that a failure of appropriate timing of computations carried out between these structures could result in serious degradation or even total loss of the critical information (Miller, 1991).

The frequency of hippocampal theta activity is controlled in part by medially placed SuM neurons (Kirk, 1993, Kirk, 1997, Kirk, 1998, Kirk and McNaughton, 1991, Kirk and McNaughton, 1993). However, lateral rather than medial areas of SuM project preferentially to the MS (Vertes, 1992, Vertes and Kocsis, 1997, Vertes and McKenna, 2000). There is also monosynaptic input from more laterally placed SuM neurons (but not more medially placed ones) to the hippocampus. The ascending pathways that control theta and other hippocampal activity appear, then, to be topographically organized and to be both direct and relayed (as with the MS). The hippocampus, in turn, has topographically organised, relayed, projections back to the hypothalamus including SuM (Risold and Swanson, 1996). The details of the system, then, need to be understood if its function (and by implication that of much of the forebrain) is to be fully understood.

SuM, at least on occasion, controls the frequency of theta activity. Theta activity, in turn, is most associated with the hippocampus. SuM is, therefore, likely to be important for the behavioural functions of the hippocampus. As noted, different parts of SuM may also contribute differentially to hippocampal function. However, as detailed below, SuM also has extensive connections with many other areas. We would expect it (through similar computational mechanisms) to contribute to a variety of different functions. This paper, therefore, reviews the neuroanatomy, neurophysiology and molecular biology of SuM, and provides a preliminary assessment of the functions of SuM, the relation of these functions to its known parts, and the extent to which those functions are discharged by interaction with the hippocampus and particularly by the control of theta activity by SuM.

Abbreviations used in the remainder of the text are defined in Table 1 to allow ready reference.

Section snippets

The boundaries of the supramammillary area and its parts

There are small but important differences in the definition and dissection into parts of SuM by different researchers (Fig. 1). We will focus below on a concordance between the full-scale atlases of Paxinos and Watson (1998) and Swanson (1998).

According to the rat brain atlas of Paxinos and Watson (1998) (see Fig. 1), SuM is a small nucleus or set of nuclei, which is about 2000 μm × 500 μm × 500 μm, lying above the mammillary bodies and below the posterior hypothalamic area. The lateral area of the

What is theta activity?

Hippocampal “theta rhythm” (Jung and Kornmüller, 1938; Green and Arduini, 1954) is one aspect of hippocampal EEG, which in some species is within the human EEG theta range (4–8 Hz) but in the rat and some other species can span frequencies between 4 and 14 Hz. Hippocampal theta rhythm recorded extracellularly with gross electrodes results from sources and sinks associated with the synchronised oscillations of IPSPs or EPSPs, or perhaps both (Fujita and Sato, 1964; Nunez et al., 1987, Nunez et

Behaviour and c-fos activation of SuM

Activation, as shown by c-fos immunoreactive cell mapping, is found in SuM neurons in response to either electrical stimulation or microinfusion of the excitatory amino acid kainate or of the GABA antagonist SR-95531, applied to the medial hypothalamus—a defensive area (Silveira et al., 1995). Electrical stimulation of the dorsal CG, another defensive area, resulted in striking c-fos immunoreactivity in SuM (Sandner et al., 1992). SuM is also one area (others, Fig. 8, include the piriform and

SuM dysfunction and behaviour

The electrophysiological data (both from theta and from analysis of hippocampal evoked potentials) suggest links between both SuMp and SuMg and the modulation of hippocampal processing. SuMs projects to the entorhinal cortex which is one of the major inputs to the hippocampus proper. These data all suggest that lesions of SuM should affect some behaviours in the same way as hippocampal lesions.

Similarly, the c-fos data suggest that SuM is involved in emotional behaviour, particularly defensive

SuM, behavioural change and theta frequency

The neurophysiological data make it clear that SuM is not the only nucleus involved in the control of theta. With reticular stimulation even large lesions of SuM produce only a modest reduction in theta frequency (McNaughton et al., 1995). The behavioural data reinforce this picture with, in the DRL task, a lack of change in theta frequency immediately before rewarded responses and a reduction in theta frequency immediately before non-rewarded responses. Given that lever pressing is likely to

SuM—an interface between cognition and emotion?

All the above suggests that SuM may be an important cross-road linking higher cognitive and lower emotional structures—so mediating cognitive–emotional interactions.

Here, rostral forebrain–SuM–CG pathways may be of particular importance. SuM connects bidirectionally with a range of structures including LS, ventromedial prefrontal areas and the cingulate (Fig. 5). The LS plays a clear role in defensive behaviour (Risold and Swanson, 1997). Lesions of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex

References (272)

  • B.H. Bland et al.

    Anatomical, electrophysiological and pharmacological studies of ascending brainstem hippocampal synchronizing pathways

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (1998)
  • B.H. Bland et al.

    Intraseptal microinfusion of muscimol: effects on hippocampal formation theta field activity and phasic theta-ON cell discharges

    Exp. Neurol.

    (1996)
  • Z. Borhegyi et al.

    Dual projection from the medial septum to the supramammillary nucleus in the rat

    Brain Res. Bull.

    (1998)
  • J.G.G. Borst et al.

    Electrical activity of the cingulate cortex. II. Cholinergic modulation

    Brain Res.

    (1987)
  • W.G. Brake et al.

    Neonatal ventral hippocampal lesions attenuate the nucleus accumbens dopamine response to stress: an electrochemical study in the adult rat

    Brain Res.

    (1999)
  • G. Carre et al.

    Population spike facilitation in the dentate gyrus following glutamate to the lateral supramammillary nucleus

    Brain Res.

    (1991)
  • T. Chiba et al.

    Afferent and efferent connections of the medial preoptic area in the rat: a WGA-HRP study

    Brain Res. Bull.

    (1985)
  • J.J. Chrobak et al.

    AF64A (ethylcholine aziridinium ion), a cholinergic neurotoxin, selectively impairs working memory in a multiple component T-maze task

    Brain Res.

    (1987)
  • C. Destrade

    Two types of diencephalically driven RSA (theta) as a means of studying memory formation in mice

    Brain Res.

    (1982)
  • C. Destrade et al.

    Is a retrosplenial (cingulate) pathway involved in the mediation of high frequency hippocampal rhythmical slow activity (theta)?

    Brain Res.

    (1982)
  • B. Earley et al.

    Behavioural, biochemical and histological effects of trimethyltin (TMT) induced brain damage

    Neurochem. Int.

    (1992)
  • M. Fendt et al.

    The neuroanatomical and neurochemical basis of conditioned fear

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (1999)
  • C. Gall et al.

    Supramammillary afferents to guinea pig hippocampus contain substance P-like immunoreactivity

    Neurosci. Lett.

    (1984)
  • B. Givens et al.

    Bidirectional modulation of scopolamine-induced working memory impairments by muscarinic activation of the medial septal area

    Neurobiol. Learn. Mem.

    (1995)
  • G. Gogolák et al.

    Septum cell activity in the rabbit under reticular stimulation

    Brain Res.

    (1967)
  • G. Gogolák et al.

    The firing pattern of septal neurons and the form of the hippocampal theta wave

    Brain Res.

    (1968)
  • J.A. Gray

    Medial septal lesions, hippocampal theta rhythm and the control of vibrissal movement in the freely moving rat

    Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (1971)
  • J.A. Gray et al.

    Comparison between the behavioural effect of septal and hippocampal lesions: a review

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (1983)
  • C. Gundlah et al.

    Ovarian steroid effects on serotonin 1A, 2A and 2C receptor mRNA in macaque hypothalamus

    Mol. Brain Res.

    (1999)
  • W. Haefely

    Pharmacology of anxiety

    Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol.

    (1991)
  • C.W. Harley et al.

    Open field motor patterns and object marking, but not object sniffing, are altered by ibotenate lesions of the hippocampus

    Neurobiol. Learn. Mem.

    (1999)
  • C.A. Heidbreder et al.

    The medial prefrontal cortex in the rat: evidence for a dorso-ventral distinction based upon functional and anatomical characteristics

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (2003)
  • J.P. Herman et al.

    Ventral subiculum regulates hypothalamo-pituitary–adrenocortical and behavioural responses to cognitive stressors

    Neuroscience

    (1998)
  • S. Ikemoto et al.

    Rewarding injections of the cholinergic agonist carbachol into the ventral tegmental area induce locomotion and c-Fos expression in the retrosplenial area and supramammillary nucleus

    Brain Res.

    (2003)
  • T. Ino et al.

    The supramammillary region of the cat sends substance P-like immunoreactive axons to the hippocampal formation and the entorhinal cortex

    Neurosci. Lett.

    (1988)
  • D.T.D. James et al.

    Septal driving of hippocampal theta rhythm as a function of frequency in the free-moving male rat

    Neuroscience

    (1977)
  • L.E. Jarrard et al.

    The effects of selective hippocampal lesions on DRL behavior in rats

    Behav. Biol.

    (1977)
  • D.G. Amaral et al.

    Subcortical afferents to the hippocampal formation in the monkey

    J. Comp. Neurol.

    (1980)
  • D.G. Amaral et al.

    Anatomical organization of the primate amygdaloid complex

  • Assaf, S.Y., 1978. Electrical activity in the hippocampal formation of the rat: role of ascending monoamine systems....
  • S.Y. Assaf et al.

    Noradrenergic modulation of neuronal transmission between the entorhinal cortex and the dentate gyrus of the rat

    J. Physiol.

    (1979)
  • C.H.M. Beck et al.

    Conditioned fear-induced changes in behavior and in the expression of the immediate early gene c-fos: with and without diazepam pretreatment

    J. Neurosci.

    (1995)
  • B. Berger et al.

    Human and monkey fetal brain development of the supramammillary–hippocampal projections: a system involved in the regulation of theta activity

    J. Comp. Neurol.

    (2001)
  • D.C. Blanchard et al.

    Ethoexperimental approaches to the biology of emotion

    Annu. Rev. Psychol.

    (1988)
  • B.H. Bland et al.

    Discharge patterns of hippocampal theta-related cells in the caudal diencephalon of the urethan-anesthetized rat

    J. Neurophysiol.

    (1995)
  • B.H. Bland et al.

    Theta band oscillation and synchrony in the hippocampal formation and associated structures: the case for its role in sensorimotor integration

    Behav. Brain Res.

    (2002)
  • B.H. Bland et al.

    Extrinsic modulation of medial septal cell discharges by the ascending brainstem hippocampal synchronizing pathway

    Hippocampus

    (1994)
  • Z. Borhegyi et al.

    Distinct substance P- and calretinin-containing projections from the supramammillary area to the hippocampus in rats: a species difference between rats and monkeys

    Exp. Brain Res.

    (1997)
  • Z. Borhegyi et al.

    The supramammilary nucleus innervates cholinergic and gabaergic neurons in the medial septum-diagonal band of Broca complex

    Neuroscience

    (1998)
  • J. Born et al.

    Hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal activity during human sleep: a coordinating role for the limbic hippocampal system

    Exp. Clin. Endocrinol. Diab.

    (1998)
  • Cited by (144)

    • Seeking motivation and reward: Roles of dopamine, hippocampus, and supramammillo-septal pathway

      2022, Progress in Neurobiology
      Citation Excerpt :

      First, inactivation of regions rostral to the SuM modifies the amplitude but not the frequency of reticular-elicited HTO, while inactivation of regions caudal to the SuM affects both frequency and amplitude (Kirk and McNaughton, 1993). Second, the stimulation of SuMn drives HTO (Bland and Oddie, 1998; Kirk, 1998; Pan and McNaughton, 2004; Pedersen et al., 2017; Vertes and Kocsis, 1997), while the inhibition of SuMn decreases the frequency of HTO in freely moving rats (Pedersen et al., 2017; Saji et al., 2000). Moreover, lesions of SuMn produce behavioral deficits similar to those observed from hippocampal lesions (Pan and McNaughton, 2002).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text