Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-42gr6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T19:19:01.829Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The positive and negative symptoms of schizophrenia reflect impairments in the perception and initiation of action

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 July 2009

C. D. Frith*
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, Clinical Research Centre, Northwick Park Hospital, Harrow
*
1Address for correspondence: Dr C. D. Frith, Division of Psychiatry, CRC Northwick Park Hospital, Watford Road, Harrow, HA1 3UJ.

Synopsis

The CNS maintains a fundamental distinction between actions elicited by external stimuli and actions elicited by internal goals (acts of will). As a result the intact organism can monitor centrally three aspects of its own actions: (1) the action appropriate to current external stimulation (stimulus intention or meaning); (2) the action appropriate to current goals (willed intention); and (3) the action which was actually selected (corollary discharge). In Type I (acute) schizophrenic patients, intentions of will lead to actions, but these willed intentions are not monitored correctly. This apparent discrepancy between will and action gives rise to experiential (1st rank) positive symptoms (e.g. delusions of control and passivity). In Type II (chronic) patients, intentions of will are no longer properly formed and so actions are rarely elicited via this route. This gives rise to behavioural negative signs (e.g. poverty of speech).

The behaviour of Type II schizophrenics has surface similarities to that shown by patients with Parkinson's disease and patients with frontal lobe lesions in that all three types of patient show a relative deficit of actions elicited by willed intentions. Dopamine blocking drugs reduce positive symptoms in Type I patients precisely because they induce Parkinsonism, i.e. reduce the likelihood of actions being initiated by willed intentions. This in turn reduces the likelihood that actions will occur for which the patient had no awareness of his intention to act.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1987

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Allen, H. A. (1983). Do positive and negative symptom subtypes of schizophrenia show qualitative differences in language production? Psychological Medicine 13, 787797.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allen, H. A. & Frith, C. D. (1983). Selective retrieval and free emission of category exemplars in schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychology 74, 481490.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andreasen, N. (1979). Thought, language and communication disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry 36, 13151330.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andreasen, N. C., Olsen, S. A., Dennert, J. W & Smith, M. R. (1982). Ventricular enlargement in schizophrenia: relationship to positive and negative symptoms. American Journal of Psychiatry 139, 297302.Google ScholarPubMed
Angel, R. W. (1976). Efference copy in the control of movement. Neurology 26, 11641168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Angrist, B., Rotrosen, J. & Gershon, S. (1980). Positive and negative symptoms in schizophrenia - differential response to amphetamine and neuroleptics. Psychopharmacology 72, 1719.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bleuler, E. (1911). Dementia Praecox oder Gruppe der Schizophrenien. Deuticke: Wien.Google Scholar
Bowen, F. P., Kamienny, R. E., Burns, M. M. & Yahr, M. D. (1975). Parkinsonism: effects of levodopa treatment on concept formation. Neurology 25, 701704.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brindley, G. & Merton, P A. (1960). The absence of position sense in the human eye. Journal of Physiology London, 153, 127130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, R., Colter, N., Corsellis, J. A. N., Crow, T. J., Frith, C. D., Jagoe, R., Johnstone, E. C. & Marsh, L. (1986). Post-mortem evidence for structural brain changes in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry (in the press).Google Scholar
Callaway, E. & Naghdi, S. (1981). An information processing model of schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 39, 339347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, M. D. (1976). Sensitivity to syntactic structure: good versus poor premorbid schizophrenics. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 85, 4150.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, B. D. (1978). Referent communication disturbances in schizophrenia. Language and Cognition in Schizophrenia, (ed. Schwartz, S.), pp. 134. Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ.Google Scholar
Connell, P. H. (1958). Amphetamine Psychosis. Institute of Psychiatry Maudsley Monographs No. 5. Chapman and Hall: London.Google Scholar
Cromwell, R. J. (1968). Stimulus redundancy and schizophrenia. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 146, 360375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crow, T. J. (1980 a). Molecular pathology of schizophrenia. More than one dimension of Pathology? British Medical Journal 280, 6668.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crow, T. J. (1980 b). Drug treatment of schizophrenia and its relationship to disturbances of dopaminergic transmission. In The Biochemistry of Psychiatric Disturbances (ed. Curzon, G.), pp. 7388. Wiley: Chichester.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1969). Content and Consciousness. Routledge and Regan Paul: London.Google Scholar
Denny-Brown, D. (1968). Clinical symptomatology of diseases of the basal ganglia. In Handbook of Clinical Neurology. Volume 6, (ed. Vinken, P. J. and Bruyn, G. W.), p. 136. North Holland: Amsterdam.Google Scholar
Dickinson, A. (1980). Contemporary Animal Learning Theory. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge.Google Scholar
Done, D. J. & Frith, C. D. (1984 a). The effect of context during word perception in schizophrenic patients. Brain and Language 23, 318336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Done, D. J. & Frith, C. D. (1984 b). Automatic and strategic control of eye movements in schizophrenia. In Theoretical and Applied Aspects of Eye Movement Research. (ed. Gale, A. G. and Johnson, F.), pp. 481487. Elsevier: Amsterdam.Google Scholar
Feighner, J. P., Robins, E., Guze, S. B., Woodruff, R. A., Winokur, G. & Munoz, R. (1972). Diagnostic criteria for use in psychiatric research. Archives of General Psychiatry 26, 5763.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feinberg, I. (1978). Efference copy and corollary discharge: implications for thinking and its disorders. Schizophrenia Bulletin 4, 636640.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flowers, K. (1978). Lack of prediction in the motor behaviour of Parkinsonism. Brain 101, 3552.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flowers, K. A. (1982). Frontal lobe signs as a component of Parkinsonism. Behaviour and Brain Research 5, 100101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frith, C. D. (1979). Consciousness, information processing and schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry 134, 225235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frith, C. D. & Done, D. J. (1983). Stereotyped responding by schizophrenic patients in a two-choice guessing task. Psychological Medicine 13, 779786.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frith, C. D. & Done, D. J. (1987). Routes to action in reaction time tasks. Psychological Research (in the press).Google Scholar
Gerver, D. (1967). Linguistic rules and the perception and recall of speech by schizophrenic patients. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology 6, 204211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldberg, G. (1985). Supplementary motor area structure and function: review and hypotheses. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8, 567616.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, G., Mayer, N. H. & Toglia, J. U. (1981). Medial frontal cortical infarction and the alien hand sign. Archives of Neurology 38, 683686.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, P. & Preston, M. (1981). Reinforcement of vocal correlates of auditory hallucinations by auditory feedback. British Journal of Psychiatry 139, 204208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Griffith, J. D., Cavanagh, J., Held, J. & Oates, J. A. (1972). Dextroamphetamine: evaluation of psychomimetic properties in man. Archives of General Psychiatry 26, 97100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Helmholtz, H. von. (1866). Handbuch der Physiologischen Optik. Voss: Leipzig. English translation from Edition 3 (1925). A Treatise on Physiological Optics (1963).(ed. Southall, J. P. C.) Vol. 3. Dover: New York.Google Scholar
Hemsley, D. R. (1977). What have cognitive deficits to do with schizophrenic symptoms? British Journal of Psychiatry 130, 167173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hirsch, S. R. & Leff, J. P. (1975). Abnormalities in Parents of Schizophrenics. Institute of Psychiatry Maudsley Monographs No. 22. Oxford University Press: Oxford.Google Scholar
Holzman, P. S., Levy, D. L. & Proctor, L. R. (1976). Smooth pursuit eye movements, attention and schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 33, 14151420.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hornykiewicz, O. (1973). Dopamine in the basal ganglia. Its role and therapeutic implications. British Medical Bulletin 29, 172178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnstone, E. C., Crow, T J., Frith, C. D., Carney, M. P. W. & Price, J. S. (1978). The mechanism of the antipsychotic effect in the treatment of acute schizophrenia. Lancet 1, 848851.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kolb, B. & Wishaw, I. Q. (1983). The performance of schizophrenic patients on tests sensitive to left or right frontal, temporal or parietal function in neurological patients. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 171, 435443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kornhuber, H. H. (1974). Cerebral cortex, cerebellum and basal ganglia: an introduction to their motor functions. In The Neurosciences: 3rd study programme (ed. Schmitt, E. D. and Warden, F. G.), pp. 267280. MIT Press: Cambridge, Mass.Google Scholar
Kraepelin, E. (1913). Psychiatrie, Vol 3, Part 2, (eighth edn) Translated 1919 as: Dementia Praecox and Paraphrenia. Living-stone: Edinburgh.Google Scholar
Laszlo, J. I. & Ward, G R. (1978). Vision, proprioception and corollary discharge in a movement recall task. Acta Psychologica 42, 477493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lees, A. J. & Smith, E (1983). Cognitive deficits in the early stages of Parkinson's disease. Brain 106, 257270.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leff, J. P. (1978) Social and psychological causes of the acute attack. In Schizophrenia: Towards a New Synthesis (ed. Wing, J. K.), pp. 139165. Academic Press London.Google Scholar
Leff, J. P. (1981). Psychiatry Around the Globe: A Transcultural View. Marcel Dekker: New York.Google Scholar
Levin, S. (1984). Frontal lobe dysfunctions in schizophrenia-I. Eye movement impairments. Journal of Psychiatric Research 18, 2755.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lhermitte, F. (1983). ‘Utilization behaviour’ and its relation to lesions of the frontal lobes. Brain 106, 237255.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Libet, B. (1985). Unconscious cerebral initiative and the role of conscious will in voluntary action. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8, 529566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liddle, P. F. & Crow, T. J. (1984). Age disorientation in chronic schizophrenia is associated with global intellectual impairment. British Journal of Psychiatry 144, 193199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luria, A. R. (1983). The Working Brain, pp. 197210. Basic Books: New York.Google Scholar
MacKay, D. M. (1978). The dynamics of perception. In Cerebral Correlates of Conscious Experience (ed. Buser, P. A. and Rougeul-Buser, A.), pp. 5368. North-Holland: Amsterdam.Google Scholar
MacKay, D. M. & Mittelstaedt, H. (1974). Visual stability and motor control (reafference revisited). In Cybernetics and Bionics(Proc. 5th Congr. Deutsche Gesellschaft für Kybernetik), pp. 7180. Oldenbourg: Munich.Google Scholar
Malenka, R. C., Angel, R. W.Hampton, B. & Berger, P. A. (1982). Impaired central error-correcting behaviour in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 39, 101107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, J. P. (1967). The Basal Ganglia and Posture, pp. 3335. Pitman Medical: London.Google Scholar
Megaw, E. D. (1972). Directional errors and their correction in a discrete tracking task. Ergonomics 15, 633643.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mellor, C. S. (1970). First rank symptoms of schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry 117, 1523.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milner, B. (1963). Effects of different brain lesions on card sorting. Archives of Neurology 9, 90100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morel-Maroger, A. (1978). Effets positifs de la l-DOPA sur une symptomatologie ‘marginale’ observée chez les parkinsoniens. L'Encephale IV, 223231.Google Scholar
Nuechterlein, K. H. (1977). Reaction time and attention in schizophrenia: a critical evaluation of the data and theories. Schizophrenia Bulletin 3, 373428.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Orzack, M. H. & Kornetsky, C. (1966). Attention dysfunction in chronic schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 14, 323326.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Owens, D. G. C. & Johnstone, E. C. (1980). The disabilities of chronic schizophrenia – their nature and the factors contributing to their development. British Journal of Psychiatry 136, 384395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Owens, D G. C, Johnstone, E. C & Frith, C. D. (1982). Spontaneous involuntary disorders of movement in neuroleptic treated and untreated chronic schizophrenics - prevalence, severity and distribution. Archives of General Psychiatry 39, 452461.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Payne, R. W., Caird, W. K. & Laverty, S. G. (1964). Overinclusive thinking and delusions in schizophrenic patients. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 68, 562566.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 32, 325.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rabbitt, P. M. A. (1966). Error-correction time without external signals. Nature 212, 438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rabbitt, P. M. A. & Vyas, S. (1981). Processing a display even after you make a response to it. How perceptual errors can be corrected. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 33 A, 223239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reason, J. T. (1979). Actions not as planned: the price of automatisation. In Aspects of Consciousness, Vol. I Psychological Issues (ed. Underwood, G. & Stevens, R.), pp. 6790. Academic Press: London.Google Scholar
Ridley, R. M. & Baker, H. F. (1983). Is there a relationship between social isolation, cognitive inflexibility, and behavioural stereotypy? An analysis of the effects of amphetamine in the marmoset. In Ethnopharmacology: Primate Models of Neuropsychiatric Disorders. (ed. Miczek, K. A.), pp. 101135. Alan R. Liss: New York.Google Scholar
Rochester, S. R., Harris, J. & Seeman, M. V. (1973). Sentence processing in schizophrenic listeners. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 82, 350356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosvold, H. C. (1972). The frontal lobe system: cortical-subcortical interrelationships. Acta Neurobiologica Experimentica 32, 125140.Google ScholarPubMed
Rosvold, H. E., Mirsky, A. F., Sarason, J., Bransome, E. D. & Beck, L. H. (1956). A continuous performance test of brain damage. Journal of Consulting Psychology 20, 343350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schell, G. R. & Strick, P. L. (1984). The origin of thalamic input to the arcuate premotor and supplementary motor areas. Journal of Neuroscience 4, 539560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Searle, J. (1983). Intentionality. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Searle, J. (1984). The freedom of the will. Reith Lecture 6, The Listener 12 13th, pp. 1012.Google Scholar
Seidman, L. J. (1983). Schizophrenia and brain dysfunction: an integration of recent neurodiagnostic findings. Psychological Bulletin 94, 195238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shakow, D. (1963). Psychological deficit in schizophrenia. Behavioural Science 8, 275305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slopsema, J. S., van der Gugten, J. & de Bruin, J. P. C. (1982). Regional concentrations of noradrenaline and dopamine in the frontal cortex of the rat. Brain Research 250, 197200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sperry, R. W. (1950). Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visual inversion. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 43, 482–189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, M. A. & Abrams, R. (1984). Cognitive impairment in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry 14, 196201.Google Scholar
Teuber, H.-L. (1964). The riddle of frontal lobe function in man. In The Frontal Granular Cortex and Behaviour, (ed. Warren, J. M. and Akert, K.), pp. 410444. McGraw-Hill: New York.Google Scholar
Von Holst, E. & Mittelstaedt, H. (1950). Das Reafferenzprinzip (Wechselwirkungen zwischen Zentralnervensystem und Peripherie). Naturwissenschaften 37, 464476.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wing, A. & Miller, E. (1984). Basal ganglia lesions and psychological analysis of voluntary movement control. CIBA Symposium 107.Google Scholar
Wing, J. K., Cooper, J. E. and Sartorius, N. (1974). The Description and Classification of Psychiatric Symptoms. An Instruction Manual for the PSE and CATEGO System. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge.Google Scholar