INTRODUCTION

The present standard of care for treating major depressive disorder is to increase the concentration of monoamine neurotransmitters by inhibiting their reuptake with medications such as selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs). SSRIs are fully effective in only half of the depressed patients (Gaynes and Warden, 2009), however. In addition, the 3–8 week latency to achieve a therapeutic effect complicates optimization of medication and delays symptomatic relief. More effective and faster-acting therapies are clearly needed to reduce the socioeconomic burden of this debilitating condition.

The discovery that inhibitors of NMDA-type glutamate receptors (NMDARs), such as ketamine, exert a rapid antidepressant action (Trullas and Skolnick, 1990; Berman et al, 2000; Zarate et al, 2006) has triggered a reevaluation of the causes of depression and potential targets for antidepressants. Unfortunately, there are concerns that ketamine’s addictive and psychotomimetic properties will hinder it from reaching its potential to treat human depression (Machado-Vieira et al, 2009). Ketamine also produces a rapid antidepressant-like behavioral response in rodents subjected to chronic stress (Maeng et al, 2008; Li et al, 2010; 2011; Autry et al, 2011). This has provided critical mechanistic insights into how rapid antidepressant actions may be produced and raised hopes that new drugs can be developed that target the same effector mechanisms, without ketamine’s disadvantages.

A common element linking the therapeutic actions of antidepressants, including SSRIs (Cai et al, 2013), ketamine (Pittenger and Duman, 2008; Kavalali and Monteggia, 2012; Abdallah et al, 2014), and scopolamine (Voleti et al, 2013), is their shared effects on excitatory synapses in cortico-mesolimbic reward circuits (Thompson et al, 2015). Chronic stress produces depression-like changes in behavior, as well as deleterious effects on excitatory synaptic structure and function, in multiple brain regions that are associated with cognition, reward, and mood, including the hippocampus (McEwen, 2000; Kallarackal et al, 2013), prefrontal cortex (PFC) (Yuen et al, 2012; Pittenger and Duman, 2008), and nucleus accumbens (NAc) (Lim et al, 2012). Conversely, ketamine, serotonin, and SSRIs promote excitatory synaptic transmission and reverse the effects of chronic stress in these same areas (Pittenger and Duman, 2008; Li et al, 2010; Autry et al, 2011; Cai et al, 2013).

Although the mechanisms underlying ketamine’s rapid antidepressant action remain under active investigation, one hypothesis is that it suppresses NMDAR-mediated excitation of inhibitory interneurons (Moghaddam et al, 1997; Farber et al, 1998; Homayoun and Moghaddam, 2007; Abdallah et al, 2014; cf. however, Autry et al, 2011). This results in a mild disinhibition of the neuronal population. Presumably because of this increase in network activity, a brief period of ketamine administration triggers several activity-dependent processes, such as induction of long-term potentiation, increased expression of the immediate early gene ΔFosB, and altered activity of the mTOR and/or eEF2 signaling pathways. This ultimately leads to the rapid induction of synapse-related genes, increased synthesis of synaptic proteins (Li et al, 2010; Autry et al, 2011; Duman and Voleti, 2012), restoration of normal synaptic strength, and persistent amelioration of depressive signs and symptoms. These results suggest that it is ultimately the mild increase in activity in forebrain neural circuits that is the key mechanism via which ketamine exerts its rapid therapeutic antidepressant actions.

On the basis of this model, we sought to identify other compounds that could trigger mild increases in neuronal activity, like ketamine. Partial inverse agonists at the benzodiazepine binding site of γ-aminobutyric acid type-A receptors (GABAARs) are negative allosteric modulators that have been shown to promote coherent network activity (Hajós et al, 2004). GABAARs containing the α5-subunit are most strongly expressed by prefrontal cortical neurons and hippocampal pyramidal cells, offering the potential means for selectively targeting cortical inputs to mesolimbic circuits, thereby potentially minimizing psychotomimetic and sedative side effects. L-655,708 and MRK-016 are negative allosteric modulators of GABAARs with a 10–100 fold selectivity for GABAARs containing the α5-subunit (Atack et al, 2005; 2006; 2009). The α5-selective negative allosteric modulators are not epileptogenic, hallucinogenic, or anxiogenic in humans (Atack et al, 2009).

We hypothesized that L-655,708 and MRK-016 would exert a rapid antidepressant action via convergence on ketamine’s downstream effector mechanisms, and therefore predicted that a single treatment of these compounds would rapidly (<24 h) reverse chronic stress-induced changes in both hedonic behavior and excitatory synaptic transmission at the archetypical stress-sensitive synapse between temporoammonic (TA) afferents and the distal dendrites of CA1 pyramidal cells.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Chronic Restraint Stress (CRS)

Male Sprague Dawley rats (5–6 weeks-old; Harlan Laboratories) were placed in appropriately sized restraint tubes, highly restricting their movement for periods of 4 hours in a brightly lit lab, every day for 10 days during the light phase of their diurnal cycle (Lim et al, 2012).

Chronic Unpredictable Stress (CUS)

Male Sprague-Dawley rats (5–6 weeks-old; Harlan Laboratories) were randomly divided into control and CUS groups. Rats in the CUS group were individually housed. CUS animals were treated with two mild stressors every day for 5–6 weeks during their light phase (Willner et al, 1987). The stressors were randomly cycled to enhance unpredictability. The stressors were: forced swim, in which rats were placed in a basin containing cold water for 5 min; strobe lighting, in otherwise total darkness for 30 min; restraint, during which rats were put into appropriately sized restraint tubes for 30 min; exposure to white noise for 30 min; and food or water deprivation for 14 h spanning the dark phase.

Sucrose Preference Test (SPT)

Rats were given a choice between two bottles containing tap water or 1% sucrose solution (Rygula et al, 2006). Testing was conducted overnight (16 h), including the full duration of their dark phase. Animals were first trained with both bottles while group-housed. For subsequent tests, including during the baseline period, animals were individually housed. Sucrose preference was calculated as the percentage of the sucrose solution consumed to the total amount of fluid consumed. For determining the effects of L-655,708 and MRK-016, only unstressed rats that demonstrated a sucrose preference >65% at baseline were used.

Social Interaction Test (SIT)

As described previously (Zelena et al, 1999; Berton et al, 2006), rats were placed in a plastic enclosure (82 × 82 cm) with a 5 × 5 grid of squares (16.4 × 16.4 cm) visible underneath the clear plastic flooring. A small translucent perforated plastic box (20 × 16 cm) was placed against the center of one wall of the arena. During the testing conditions, white overhead fluorescent lighting was used to illuminate the arena. A video camera positioned 170 cm above the floor of the arena was used to track the movements of the rats during the experiment.

Each test consisted of a ‘target-absent’ trial followed by a ‘target-present’ trial. During testing, an individual test rat was placed in the center of the arena and, after 30 s of adaptation, the rat’s movements were recorded with the video camera for 2.5 min. After the ‘target-absent’ trial, the test rat was removed from the arena and placed back in its home cage. For the ‘target-present’ trial, a novel juvenile (3–6 weeks old; always younger than the rat being tested) rat was placed in the internal box at one end of the arena and, after 3 minutes, the test rat was returned to the center of the arena and filmed for 2.5 min. Both rats were removed and the testing arena and internal box were cleaned with 70% ethanol before the next rat was tested. No test rat ever encountered the same target rat more than once.

Results were quantified as the percentage of time spent in the ‘interaction zone’ (the five grid squares immediately surrounding the plastic cage) as measured for both the ‘target-absent’ and ‘target-present’ trials. These percentages were then used to calculate an interaction ratio, which is the percent time spent in the ‘interaction zone’ during the ‘target-present’ trial divided by that of the ‘target-absent’. For determining the effects of L-655,708 and MRK-016, only unstressed rats that demonstrated a social interaction ratio >100 at baseline were used.

Open Field

Open-field behavior was monitored in the 82 × 82 cm arena with the 5 × 5 grid of 16 outer squares and 9 inner squares placed in a dimly lit room during the rats’ light cycle. Rats were placed in one corner of the arena and filmed for post hoc analysis. The observer recorded the crossings into inner or outer squares within a 5-min test session. The data were analyzed for total number of crossings from one square to another inner or outer square, from which the total number of crossings into inner squares were calculated.

Acute Slice Electrophysiology

Standard methods were used to prepare 400-μm-thick transverse hippocampal slices. Dissection and recording saline contained: 124 mM NaCl, 3 mM KCl, 1.25 mM NaH2PO4, 1.5 mM MgSO4, 2.5 mM CaCl2, 26 mM NaHCO3, and 10 mM glucose, bubbled with 95% O2/5% CO2. Slices were then transferred to a submersion-type recording chamber and perfused at 0.5–2 ml/min at 20–22 °C. Picrotoxin (100 μM) and CGP52432 (2 μM) were included to block GABAA and GABAB receptors, and the CA3 region was removed with a scalpel. Because TA-CA1 synapses are electrotonically remote from CA1 cell somata, we used extracellular recording of local field excitatory postsynaptic potentials (fEPSPs). Recording pipettes (3–5 MΩ) contained extracellular saline and were placed in stratum lacunosum-moleculare (SLM) to record TA-CA1 responses. fEPSPs were amplified × 1000, filtered at 3 kHz, and digitized at 10 kHz. Concentric bipolar tungsten electrodes were placed >500 μm from the stimulating electrodes in SLM for TA afferents. Stimuli (100 μs) were delivered at 0.05 Hz.

As in our previous work (Kallarackal et al, 2013), Mg2+-free saline was used to produce a robust NMDAR-mediated component of the fEPSP for quantification of AMPAR-mediated synaptic excitation. 3–5 consecutive responses were averaged and fEPSP slope was calculated over a 1–3 ms window. For AMPAR-mediated responses, the window was fixed in the initial rising phase of the response, 2–5 ms after its initiation. For NMDA responses, DNQX (50 μM) was bath applied for 15 min and the slope was calculated over a 3–5 ms window in the rising phase of the response, 5–10 ms after its initiation.

Responses were elicited over a range of stimulation intensities before and after application of DNQX. We first compared responses across slices by normalizing them to the amplitude of the fiber volley. The linear portion of the relationship between response slope and FV amplitude was fit with a straight line and the slopes of the fitted lines were compared across conditions, as we have done previously (Kallarackal et al, 2013). We also normalized the slope of AMPAR-mediated responses to the slope of the NMDAR-mediated response, choosing traces that had a fiber volley closest to 0.2 mV in amplitude.

Western Blotting

SLM tissue punches (1 mm diameter) were dissected from area CA1 in hippocampal slices and pooled (3–4 punches/sample). Membranes were probed with antibodies directed against GluA1 (Millipore Bioscience Research Reagents) and β-actin (Cell Signaling Technology). Levels of proteins are expressed as the ratio of intensity normalized to β-actin intensity, as done previously (Kallarackal et al, 2013).

Drugs/Stats

L-655,708 was purchased from Tocris Bioscience (R&D Systems, Minneapolis, MN) and prepared at a concentration of 5 mM in 75% saline/25% DMSO. The dose used in this study (0.7 mg/kg i.p.) was taken from the study of Martin et al (2009), in which L-655,708 was shown to reverse the amnestic properties of the anesthetic etomidate; a positive sign of its efficacy in brain. This dose is sufficient to occupy ~70% of forebrain binding sites (Atack et al, 2005). MRK-016 (Tocris Bioscience) was dissolved in DMSO at 6 mg/ml.

All quantification and analyses of behavioral, electrophysiological, and western blotting results were performed with the experimenter masked to the condition of the animal, tissue, or protein sample. Data were first compared with an analysis of variance, followed by pairwise post hoc comparison tests using SPSS software.

RESULTS

Rapid Reversal of Stress-Induced Changes in Hedonic Behavior by L-655,708

Several hedonic behaviors in rodents are sensitive to chronic stress and are restored by chronic, but not acute, administration of SSRIs, providing one means to screen for potential antidepressant compounds. We first sought to determine whether L-655,708 would rapidly (<24 h) restore normal hedonic behavior following chronic restraint stress (Watanabe et al, 1992; Lim et al, 2012), consistent with the rapid antidepressant-like actions of ketamine (Li et al, 2011). We first made baseline measurements of social interaction and sucrose preference, and then subjected 5–6-week-old rats to 10 days of CRS. At the culmination of CRS treatment, rats were injected with either vehicle (75% saline, 25% DMSO; i.p.) or L-655,708 (0.7 mg/kg, i.p.). Social interaction and sucrose preference tests were then repeated 24 h post injection. Chronic restraint stress produced a decrease in social interaction ratio in vehicle-injected CRS rats compared with all other groups, whereas the social interaction ratio in the L-655,708-injected rats was not significantly different than baseline (F(1,12)=2.85, p=0.115; n=7 vehicle-injected, 7 L-655,708 injected; Figure 1a). Similarly, there was a significant decrease in sucrose preference in stressed rats that received a vehicle injection compared with their pre-stress baseline, whereas stressed rats administered L-655,708 showed levels of sucrose preference that were not different from their pre-stress baseline (F(1,11)=7.514, p=0.019; Figure 1b). These behavioral changes are consistent with previous descriptions of chronic SSRIs (Cai et al, 2013) and acute ketamine (Li et al, 2011).

Figure 1
figure 1

L-655,708 rapidly reverses loss of sucrose preference and social interaction behaviors after chronic restraint stress (CRS). Quantification of results from the (a) social interaction (SIT, left) and (b) sucrose preference tests (SPT, right) at two time points: before CRS and after 10 days of CRS. 24 h before the second round of tests, rats received an injection of either vehicle (black) or L-655,708 (gray). Mean social interaction ratios differed significantly following vehicle injection compared with all other groups (F(1,12)=2.85, p=0.115; n=7 CRS+vehicle rats, 7 CRS+L-655,708 rats). Mean sucrose preference differed significantly following vehicle injection compared with all other groups (2 × 2 repeated-measures ANOVA group–time interaction F(1,11)=7.514, p=0.019). *p<0.05 compared with pre-CUS baseline, Bonferroni post hoc test; n=7 CRS+vehicle rats, 6 CRS+L-655,708 rats). The dashed line in both graphs indicates the value at which there is no preference for social interaction or the sucrose solution in this and all graphs.

PowerPoint slide

To determine whether the rapid reversal of stress-induced changes in behavior by L-655,708 was specific to the CRS procedures, we repeated these experiments using another chronic psychological stress paradigm, chronic unpredictable stress (CUS, Willner et al, 1987). Social interaction tests were conducted at three sequential time points (Figure 2): before CUS (baseline), after 4 weeks of CUS and an injection of the vehicle solution 24 h earlier, and after an additional week of CUS and an injection of L-655,708 24 h earlier (0.7 mg/kg, i.p.). The mean social interaction ratio was significantly lower in rats subjected to CUS that were given a vehicle injection compared with their baseline values (F(2,34)=15, p<0.0001;vehicle-treated group p<0.01 vs baseline and L-655,708, Tukey’s; n=18 rats; Figure 2a), consistent with previous descriptions of stress-induced behavioral changes in mice (Berton et al, 2006). Twenty-four hours after a single injection of L-655,708, social interaction in 14 of 18 CUS rats were reversed to levels that were not significantly different than pre-stress baseline or responses in unstressed animals (Figure 2b, black symbols). The four CUS rats injected with L-655,708 that failed to display an increase in social interaction appeared largely resilient to the CUS procedures (Figure 2b, gray symbols). Unstressed rats displayed no significant difference in either sucrose preference or social interaction 24 h after injection with L-655,708 or saline (Figure 2c and d).

Figure 2
figure 2

L-655,708 rapidly reverses loss of social interaction behaviors after chronic unpredictable stress (CUS) but does not affect hedonic behavior in unstressed rats. Quantification of results from the social interaction test (SIT) at three time points: before CUS, after 4 weeks of CUS and a vehicle injection given 24 h earlier, and after an additional week of CUS and an injection of L-655,708 given 24 h earlier. (a) Mean social interaction ratio differed significantly in the CUS+vehicle group compared with all other groups (F(2,34)=15, p<0.0001; *p<0.05 vs other groups, Tukey’s test, n=18 rats). (b) Analysis of individual animals revealed that 14 of 18 animals displayed robust responses to CUS and L-655,708 (black). The four rats that failed to respond to L-655,708 appeared largely resilient to the effects of CUS (gray symbols). Unstressed controls were also given vehicle and L-655,708 injections at the same time points. Neither injection produced a significant change in social interaction (c) or sucrose preference (d).

PowerPoint slide

Similarly, a single injection of L-655,708 restored sucrose preference in rats subjected to CUS (F(2,8)=8.66, p=0.01, n=5 rats; Figure 3a). L-655,708 had no significant effect on the time rats spent in the interaction zone in the target-absent condition after either CUS (F(2,23)=1.484, p=0.248) or CRS (F(1,10)=0.971, p=0.348), and it had no significant effect in unstressed animals (Supplementary Figure). Similarly, L-655,708 had no effect on either the total number of line crossings (Figure 3b) or center line crossings (Figure 3c) in an open-field test 30 min after injection (n=6 rats each), indicating that it did not induce nonspecific effects on locomotor behavior or anxiety.

Figure 3
figure 3

L-655,708 rapidly reverses loss of sucrose preference after chronic unpredictable stress (CUS) but does not affect behavior in the open-field test. (a) Quantification of results from sucrose preference tests at three time points: before CUS, after 4 weeks of CUS and a vehicle injection given 24 h earlier, and after an additional week of CUS and an injection of L-655,708 given 24 h earlier. Mean sucrose preference differed significantly in the CUS+vehicle group compared with all other groups (F(2,8)=8.66, p=0.01, Tukey’s test, n=5 rats). *p<0.05 compared with pre-CUS baseline, Bonferroni post hoc test. In the open-field test, there was no significant difference between unstressed rats given an injection of vehicle or L-655,708 (n=6 rats each) 30 min earlier in either total number of line crossings (b), an indicator of locomotor activity, or in crossings to the center of the arena (c), an indicator of anxiety.

PowerPoint slide

We also tested the effects of a second, chemically distinct α5-preferring negative allosteric modulator, MRK-016, at a dose of 3 mg/kg. This dose should produce roughly 80% occupancy of α5-containing GABAARs (Atack et al, 2009). Within 24 h, a single injection of MRK-016 restored sucrose preference (F(3,33)=20.63, p<0.0001, n=12 rats; Figure 4a and b) and social interaction (F=(2,14)=11.84, p=0.000978, n=8 rats; Figure 4c) in rats subjected to CRS. Sucrose preference and social interaction ratios remained statistically indistinguishable from baseline values even 7 days after the injection of MRK-016, despite continued CRS.

Figure 4
figure 4

MRK-016 rapidly reverses loss of sucrose preference (SPT) and social interaction (SIT) behaviors after chronic restraint stress (CRS). (a) Quantification of results from one group of rats in the sucrose preference test at five time points: baseline, after 2 weeks of CRS, 24 h after an additional 3 days of CRS and 24 h after injection of vehicle, after one week of CRS and 24 h after an injection of MRK-016 (3 mg/kg), and in the same animals after an additional week of CRS and 7 days after the injection of MRK-016. Mean sucrose preference differed significantly following vehicle injection compared with all other groups (F(3,33)=20.63, p<0.0001, n=12 rats). *p<0.05 compared with pre-CUS baseline, Tukey’s post hoc test). (b) Results in individual animals for the full experiment. (c) Quantification of results from one group of rats in the social interaction test at four time points: baseline, after 2 weeks of CRS and 24 h after injection of vehicle, after 1 week of CRS and 24 h after an injection of MRK-016 (3 mg/kg), and in the same animals after an additional week of CRS and 7 days after the injection of MRK-016. Mean social interaction ratios differed significantly following vehicle injection compared with all other groups (F=(2,14)=11.84, p=0.0009, n=8 rats). *p<0.05 compared with pre-CUS baseline, Tukey’s post hoc test).

PowerPoint slide

Taken together, these data show that two α5-selective negative allosteric modulators reversed chronic stress-induced depressive-like behavioral changes in <24 h in a model-independent manner. The response of individual animals to chronic stress and the two compounds in both the sucrose preference and social interaction tests were highly correlated (r=0.69; t(25)=4.781, p<0.0001; Figure 5). Neither compound produced any detectable change in the behavior of unstressed animals in these tests.

Figure 5
figure 5

Correlation of changes in social interaction and sucrose preference behaviors after chronic stress and after administration of α5-preferring negative allosteric modulators. Changes from baseline responses in the social interaction and sucrose preference tests in response to either chronic restraint stress (CRS) or chronic unpredictable stress (CUS) in individual rats are shown in black. Changes from CRS/CUS responses in the social interaction and sucrose preference tests in response to injection of either L-655,708 or MRK-016 24 h earlier in individual rats are shown in gray. There was a strong correlation for both behavioral outcomes (linear fit in black; r=0.69; t(25)=4.781, p=0.0001; n=27).

PowerPoint slide

Rapid Reversal of Stress-Induced Weakening of Excitatory Synaptic Transmission by L-655,708

Chronic stress induces a reduction in excitatory synaptic transmission in many cortico-mesolimbic nuclei, including TA-CA1 synapses in the hippocampus (Kallarackal et al, 2013). Chronic, but not acute, administration of fluoxetine restores their strength with a time course that is comparable with behavioral restoration (Cai et al, 2013). We therefore examined TA-CA1 synaptic transmission to ask whether L-655,708 would reverse this stress-induced synaptic phenotype as rapidly as it reversed stress-induced behavioral changes.

We used extracellular electrophysiological recording of fEPSPs at TA-CA1 synapses in SLM of area CA1 in brain slices prepared from unstressed rats, rats subjected to 10 days of CRS and given a vehicle injection 24 h earlier, and rats subjected to 10 days of CRS and given an L-655,708 injection 24 h earlier (0.7 mg/kg, i.p.). These were the same rats used for the behavioral analyses in Figure 1. fEPSPs were recorded in ACSF lacking added Mg2+ so as to unblock NMDA receptors. We first quantified the slope of the AMPAR component of the fEPSP elicited over a range of stimulation intensities as a function of the amplitude of the fiber volley (FV), a measure of the number of synapses activated. We then applied DNQX to block AMPAR-mediated transmission and elicited a series of NMDAR-mediated responses over the same range of stimulation intensities, as in our previous study (Kallarackal et al, 2013).

We observed a decrease in the slope of the AMPAR-mediated component of the fEPSP across all stimulation intensities in slices from rats subjected to CRS and given a vehicle injection, compared with slices from unstressed rats and slices from rats subjected to CRS and given an L-655,708 injection (F(2,17)=3.675, p=0.047; n=5 unstressed, 8 CRS+vehicle, 7 CRS+L655,708; Figure 6a and b). There was no corresponding difference in the slope of the NMDA component of the fEPSP under these conditions (F(2,17)=0.549, p=0.588; Figure 3a and b). We also computed an AMPA:NMDA ratio for each slice using the slope of the response elicited when the FV was ~0.2 mV in amplitude before and after application of DNQX. The AMPA:NMDA ratio was significantly higher in slices from CRS rats injected with L-655,708 24 h earlier, compared with vehicle-treated animals subjected to CRS (p<0.05 LSD post hoc), and was not different than responses in tissue from unstressed rats (F(2,17)=4.345, p=0.03; Figure 6c).

Figure 6
figure 6

L-655,708 rapidly reverses stress-induced weakening of AMPAR-mediated synaptic transmission in TA-CA1 synapses of the rat hippocampus. (a) Representative traces showing the AMPAR- and NMDAR-mediated components of the fEPSP, recorded extracellularly in SLM of area CA1 in response to simulation of TA afferents in saline lacking added Mg2+. Traces are shown before (black) and after (gray) addition of 50 μM DNQX to block AMPARs. Traces were recorded from slices taken from unstressed controls (left) and rats subjected to 10 days of CRS then given an injection 24 h earlier of either vehicle solution (middle) or L-655,708 (right). (b) The mean slope of the relationship between AMPAR- (left) and NMDAR-mediated response (right) and FV amplitude over a range of stimulation intensities from the three groups of animals. The slope of the AMPAR-mediated responses was decreased significantly in slices from vehicle-treated CRS rats (red) compared with slices from unstressed (blue) or L-655,708-treated CRS rats (green) across the range of stimulation intensities (1-Way ANOVA, F(2,17)=3.675, p=0.047; n=5 unstressed, 8 CRS+vehicle, 7 CRS+L-655,708; *p<0.05, LSD post hoc test). There were no significant differences in NMDAR-mediated responses (1-Way analysis of variance (ANOVA), F(2,17)=0.549, p=0.588). (c) AMPA:NMDA ratios were computed from the initial slopes of the responses in each slice before and after application of DNQX (shown by dotted lines in a). AMPA:NMDA ratios were decreased significantly in slices from vehicle-treated CRS rats compared with slices from either unstressed or L-655,708-treated CRS rats (one-way ANOVA F(2,17)=4.345 p=0.03; n=5 unstressed, 8 CRS+vehicle, 7 CRS+L-655,708; *p<0.05 compared with unstressed and CRS+L-655,708, LSD post hoc test). A full color version of this figure is available at the Neuropsychopharmacology journal online.

PowerPoint slide

These results replicate our earlier observations that AMPAR-mediated signaling, but not NMDAR-mediated signaling, is impaired by chronic stress at TA-CA1 synapses and we now demonstrate that this impairment can be reversed rapidly by treatment with L-655,708. We conclude that the behavioral antidepressant efficacy of L-655,708 is associated with a restoration of excitatory neurotransmission at this stress-sensitive synapse.

Rapid Reversal of Stress-Induced Downregulation of GluA1 Expression by L-655,708

Restoration of stress-impaired AMPAR-mediated transmission produced by treatment with either chronic SSRIs (Kallarackal et al, 2013) or ketamine (Li et al, 2011) is associated with an increase in the expression of the GluA1 subunit of the AMPAR. We next asked whether L-655,708 would also restore GluA1 expression as rapidly as it reverses the stress-induced behavioral and synaptic phenotypes.

SLM tissue was harvested from slices taken from unstressed rats, rats subjected to 10 days of CRS and given a vehicle injection 24 h earlier, and rats subjected to 10 days of CRS and given an injection of L-655,708 24 h earlier (0.7 mg/kg, i.p.). These were the same rats used for Figure 1. As we have reported previously for CUS, rats subjected to CRS and injected with vehicle displayed a significantly lower level of GluA1 protein in SLM as compared with unstressed control rats (Kruskal–Wallis H test: χ2(2)=10.62, p=0.0049; n=6 unstressed rats, 6 CRS+vehicle, 8 CRS+L-655,708; Figure 7). In contrast, stressed rats injected with L-655,708 24 h earlier displayed levels of GluA1 in SLM that were not significantly different than levels in unstressed controls.

Figure 7
figure 7

L-655,708 rapidly reverses stress-induced decreases in GluA1 expression at TA-CA1 synapses of the rat hippocampus. (Upper panel) Representative western blots of GluA1 and β-actin protein levels in SLM samples from unstressed rats (left) and rats subjected to 10 days of CRS then given an injection 24 h earlier of either vehicle solution (middle) or L-655,708 (right). (Lower panel) GluA1 expression, normalized to β-actin levels, was decreased significantly in tissue from vehicle-treated CRS rats (red) compared to tissue from unstressed (blue) or L-655,708-treated CRS rats (green)(Kruskal–Wallis H test: χ2(2)=10.62, p=0.0049; n=6 unstressed, 6 CRS+vehicle, 8 CRS+L-655,708; *p<0.05 compared with unstressed and CRS+L-655,708, post hoc Mann–Whitney U-test). A full color version of this figure is available at the Neuropsychopharmacology journal online.

PowerPoint slide

These results demonstrate that the stress-induced decrease in GluA1 protein expression, which was correlated with both weakened excitatory synaptic transmission at TA-CA1 synapses and altered hedonic behaviors, was reversed rapidly by treatment with L-655,708. α5-subunit-selective negative allosteric modulators thus displaying antidepressant efficacy at behavioral, electrophysiological, and molecular end points.

DISCUSSION

We have discovered that two negative allosteric modulator of GABARs that act as a partial inverse agonists at the benzodiazepine binding site and are highly selective for receptors containing α5-subunits, L-655,708 and MRK-016, reverse the behavioral and synaptic phenotypes produced by two different chronic stress paradigms within 24 h of a single systemic administration. These effects persisted for several days after a single injection. At the concentrations used in our study, these compounds do have measurable binding to non-α5-subunits, thus we cannot formally exclude the possibility that their effects are mediated by GABAARs containing other alpha subunits.

The motivated, appetitive behaviors assayed in the social interaction and sucrose preference tests test the hedonic properties of two distinct stimuli, food, and sociality. In both the sucrose preference and social interaction tests, rats must respond in an active manner to display the high levels of sucrose preference and social interaction that are normally observed. Chronic stress is known to decrease the rewarding properties of a variety of natural and unnatural stimuli in rodents, including their high preference for palatable sucrose-containing solution (Pothion et al, 2004) and the naturally rewarding value of social interactions (Zelena et al, 1999; Berton et al, 2006). The loss of the rewarding properties of these stimuli is considered to be analogous to anhedonia, or the inability to experience reward, a core symptom of human depression. Importantly, normal behavior in both the sucrose preference test (Rygula et al, 2006) and social interaction test (Berton et al, 2006) is restored with chronic, but not acute, administration of SSRIs, thus providing a strong predictive indication of the potential human antidepressant efficacy of α5-selective negative allosteric modulators.

α5-Containing GABAA Receptors as an Antidepressant Drug Target

GABAA receptors are heteropentameric ion channels, most commonly containing α-, β-, and γ-subunits in a 2 : 2 : 1 stoichiometry (Sieghart and Sperk, 2002). The interface between a γ2-subunit and an α-subunit forms the binding site for benzodiazepines, a site of allosteric modulation that alters channel gating and GABAAR function. In addition to benzodiazepine agonists (positive modulators), there are inverse agonists that act at the same site to decrease GABAA receptor function (negative modulators). The molecular identity of the α-subunit in the benzodiazepine binding site determines its pharmacological profile. GABAARs containing α1-subunits mediate the sedative and amnestic effects of benzodiazepines preferentially, whereas GABAARs containing α2- and α3-subunits mediate their anxiolytic effects (Rudolph et al, 1999; Möhler et al, 2002). Partial inverse agonists with low affinity for this binding site offer the potential advantage of a wider therapeutic concentration range and a lower likelihood of producing negative side effects, such as anxiety or epileptiform discharge.

The α5-subunit mRNA is abundant in pyramidal cells in the hippocampus and deep layers of the neocortex, and α5-containing GABAARs are localized at synaptic and extrasynaptic sites in the dendrites (Fritschy and Möhler, 1995; Wainwright et al, 2000; Serwanski et al, 2006). α5-containing GABAARs mediate tonic (Caraiscos et al, 2004) and slow phasic (Prenosil et al, 2006; Zarnowska et al, 2009) inhibition and lower the excitability of pyramidal cells (Bonin et al, 2007), so negative allosteric modulators of α5-containing GABAARs should promote coherent activity (Hajós et al, 2004; Towers et al, 2004). Drugs targeting these receptors are thus an attractive means to selectively alter activity within cortico-mesolimbic circuits without altering activity in other circuits. Indeed, deletion of the α5-gene alters learning in hippocampal-dependent tasks but not hippocampal-independent ones (Collinson et al, 2002; Crestani et al, 2002). Interestingly, α5-subunits are upregulated in mice after chronic stress (Matsumoto et al, 2007), suggesting a potential role for the α5-subunit in the cognitive deficits associated with chronic stress and depression.

Mechanisms of L-655,708 Action

Our observation that L-655,708 rapidly restores pathologically weakened AMPAR-mediated excitatory transmission at TA-CA1 synapses of the hippocampus suggests a likely mechanism by which L-655,708 exerts its antidepressant efficacy in behavioral testing. SSRIs (Cai et al, 2013), ketamine (Autry et al, 2011; Li et al, 2010), and L-655,708 all enhance excitatory synaptic strength at multiple sites within the cortico-mesolimbic reward circuitry, thereby providing strong support for an association between the restoration of excitatory synaptic strength and the reversal of depressive behavioral phenotypes (Duman, 2014; Thompson et al, 2015). TA-CA1 synapses are not uniquely sensitive to stress or responsive to antidepressants, but they do serve as a convenient archetype of stress-induced changes that are likely occurring at many synapses in the cortico-mesolimbic reward circuitry (Lim et al, 2012; Yuen et al, 2012). The net effect of this weakening is likely to be a dysfunction in processing of rewarding stimuli, which may underlie common symptoms of depression, such as anhedonia (Nestler and Carlezon, 2006). Restoration of the strength of these synapses by effective antidepressants should restore normal responses to rewarding stimuli (Belujon and Grace, 2014).

Acute administration of ketamine improves mood and reduces depressive symptoms within 1–2 h in humans and these effects persist for up to 2 weeks. Similarly, ketamine restores sucrose preference and novelty suppressed feeding behaviors rapidly (24 h) in chronically stressed animals (Li et al, 2011). Ketamine’s therapeutic effects are induced during the brief period (~1–2 h) when it is present in the brain at sufficient concentrations to inhibit NMDARs, triggering relief of symptoms that persist for days after it is cleared from the body. During this induction phase, ketamine may preferentially reduce excitation of GABAergic inhibitory interneurons (Dwyer and Duman, 2013; Farber et al, 1998; Homayoun and Moghaddam, 2007), thereby producing a mild disinhibition of the neuronal population and increased activity in the hippocampus, entorhinal cortex, NAc, and PFC, as observed in rodents (eg, Kittelberger et al, 2012; Middleton et al, 2008; Hunt et al, 2011; Caixeta et al, 2013) and in humans (Cornwell et al, 2012; Driesen et al, 2013). This activity is accompanied by a neurochemically detectable surge of glutamate release in the PFC and NAc (Lorrain et al, 2003; Moghaddam et al, 1997; Razoux et al, 2007). The α5-selective negative allosteric modulators also produce mild disinhibition by impairing GABAAR channel function, and should therefore also trigger an increased coherence in oscillatory activity, as has been observed following injection of nonselective negative allosteric modulators (Hajós et al, 2004).

We suggest that the ability of both GABAAR negative allosteric modulators and ketamine to promote activity accounts for their shared antidepressant efficacy because such activity strengthens excitatory synapses via convergence onto common activity-dependent signaling pathways. Potential activity-dependent processes include an increase in BDNF signaling, activation of mTOR signaling, and protein synthesis (Li et al, 2010; 2011; Autry et al, 2011). An increase in coherent circuit activity is also likely to promote the induction of long-term potentiation (eg, Huerta and Lisman, 1993). All of these activity-dependent processes have been shown to strengthen excitatory synapses. Indeed, we have observed that L-655,708 administration triggers a rapid increase in GluA1 protein levels in the distal dendrites of CA1 pyramidal cells, coincident with an increase in AMPAR-mediated synaptic excitation. Interestingly, chronic, but not acute, administration of SSRIs also promotes GluA1 protein levels in this same region (Kallarackal et al, 2013), which is densely innervated with serotonergic terminals.

Therapeutic Potential

Because disinhibition promotes induction of long-term potentiation, the presumptive cellular basis of memory, negative allosteric modulators of GABAARs containing α5-subunits have been developed as cognitive enhancers (Ballard et al, 2009; Quirk et al, 1996; Atack et al, 2006; 2009). Unlike nonselective negative allosteric modulators, these compounds are not anxiogenic, hallucinogenic, or epileptogenic in humans (Atack et al, 2009).

Although SSRIs are reasonably safe and well tolerated, and are not addictive, they are effective in only a subset of patients and only after a delay of weeks to months. Ketamine, on the other hand, exerts a rapid antidepressant action in the majority of patients, but its therapeutic utility is extremely limited due to its dissociative and anesthetic properties, as well as its potential for abuse and overdose. L-655,708 and MRK-016 reversed stress-induced behavioral changes and restored AMPAR-mediated excitatory synaptic strength in two well-validated rodent models of antidepressant efficacy, like SSRIs and ketamine. Because these effects were induced within 24 h of a single systemic injection, we suggest that negative allosteric modulators acting on α5-subunit-containing GABAARs represent a novel, rapidly acting, effective, and clinically viable treatment for human depression.

FUNDING AND DISCLOSURE

This work was supported by grants R01 MH086828 (SMT), T32 GM008181 (AMVD), T32 NS063391 (MDK), and T32 NS007375 (TAL). The authors declare no conflict of interests.