Introduction

The perception of time stands as one of the primary functions of conscious behavior. Temporal perception underlies a host of cognitive and perceptual functions, and is required for predicting and anticipating upcoming sensory events and planning action. However, the neural mechanisms underlying time perception are still being uncovered. To that end, recent research in this area has begun to increase its focus on the role of endogenous oscillations in temporal perception and action1. Yet, the role of any particular neural oscillation in temporal perception remains to be determined. Indeed, a cross-study survey of neural oscillations in timing reveals that no single frequency band is dominant for temporal processing1. This finding suggests that distinct frequency bands are context specific to the timing task in which they are employed, but additionally raises the possibility that different frequency bands have different roles in the estimation of a temporal interval. Recent work favors a supramodal role for beta oscillations (~20 Hz) in the production and perception of temporal intervals. However, the majority of the evidence in favor of this beta-timing hypothesis is circumstantial, and notable challenges to the hypothesis exist.

The importance of neural oscillations for timing cannot be overstated. Neural oscillations were proposed as a driving factor for time perception sixty years ago2, yet failures to replicate these findings led to a reduction of oscillatory studies of timing in the literature, with the majority of studies instead focusing on broadband electrophysiological signatures of timing. One extensively studied phenomenon is the contingent negative variation (CNV), a slow negative potential at frontocentral electrodes that has been associated with expectation3. Studies of the CNV have demonstrated that the amplitude of this response correlates with the subjective perception of temporal duration, suggesting the CNV serves as an index of time for the brain4. Further, the supplementary motor area (SMA), a region highly implicated in studies of time perception5, has been suggested to be the source of the CNV6. However, challenges to this interpretation of the CNV exist, driven by a failure to replicate the correlation7; while these failures are troubling, the majority of CNV studies do reveal a close connection between CNV amplitude and subjectively perceived time.

To further investigate the involvement of oscillations in time perception, Kononowicz and van Rijn8 re-analyzed an earlier study of the CNV, which had found no association between amplitude and duration9, by applying a time/frequency analysis of EEG responses. Here, the authors found that the power of beta oscillations exclusively tracked the subjective length of duration. Notably, this task required subjects to produce a temporal interval (2.5 s) by indicating via a button press when they believed the interval to have elapsed. Prior work has demonstrated beta oscillations during temporal preparation and prediction, in which subjects must anticipate the predictable onset of a stimulus10,11,12,13,14,15. Further, beta oscillations have also been demonstrated during the perception of rhythmic cues, in which beta power and phase track the repeated occurrence of stimuli. Recent neural recordings in non-human primates have also demonstrated beta-oscillations in the basal ganglia associated with motor timing paradigms associated with self-generated rhythmic cues16. Notably, beta oscillations exhibit a supramodal property for timing, with similar modulations observed for auditory and visual stimuli1.

Despite the evidence cited above, challenges to the claim that beta oscillations index temporal processing remain. First, all of the above studies entail strong motor components, in which the interval must either be demarcated with a motor response, or the predictable onset of a target stimulus instructs subjects to make a speeded response. As beta oscillations have commonly been associated with motor functions17, the modulation of beta power in these studies may reflect an upcoming motor response, rather than an actual signal of timing. An exception to the above are studies of rhythmic stimuli11,18 in which subjects passively listen to a beat; however, modulation of beta power in these studies may still be driven primarily by cortical and sub-cortical motor areas, as the mere presence of a rhythm is sufficient to activate these areas. Second, the majority of beta studies have employed sub-second temporal intervals. Meijer, te Woerd, and Praamstra19 recently demonstrated that, when the anticipatory interval is in the supra-second range, peaks in beta resynchronization occur at the same point in time, between 800–1000 ms, regardless of how long the anticipatory interval lasts. Yet, this study primarily demonstrates a potential dissociation between motor preparatory processes and cannot rule out the possibility that differences observed between interval duration conditions do not reflect timing computations. Further, the study by Kononowicz and van Rijn8 included supra-second intervals (~2.5 s), suggesting the difference may have relied on the implicit/explicit nature of the timing task20.

Only one study of which we are aware has addressed the beta timing hypothesis in a non-motor paradigm. Kulashekhar and colleagues21 tested subjects with MEG on a temporal discrimination task with supra-second durations, using the well-known time/color paradigm of Coull and colleagues22 in which subjects view a visual stimulus rapidly flickering between shades of red and blue, and must either compare the duration or overall color of the stimulus. Here, the authors demonstrated that duration comparisons engendered significantly greater beta power amplitudes in a parieto-frontal network – including the SMA – during the encoding of stimulus duration. Further, higher beta amplitudes were associated with accurate vs. inaccurate choices, suggesting strong behavioral relevance.

Beyond beta oscillations, as noted above, other frequency bands have been associated with timing functions1. The second most associated frequency band to beta oscillations are slower, alpha oscillations. Indeed, alpha oscillations were the first to be associated with time perception2; their association with timing measures could not be replicated, however23. More recently, alpha oscillations have been observed in visual temporal prediction studies10,24,25. For non-motor timing paradigms, alpha oscillations have also been observed21,26. In the study by Kulashekhar and colleagues, alpha oscillations were stronger during the retrieval of duration information from memory, and were similarly associated with accurate as compared to inaccurate decisions, suggesting that alpha power in this task may reflect decision making for temporal duration. Further, recent work has suggested that alpha frequency serves as an index of the “sample rate” for the visual system, such that fluctuations in peak alpha frequency may be associated with changes in perceived duration27,28.

While the highlights above demonstrate the association of beta and alpha oscillations with time perception and temporal prediction, none of the aforementioned studies have demonstrated that beta oscillations are intrinsically associated with timing. That is, the involvement of either of these frequency bands may be epiphenomenal and not reflect actual timing computations. For this, interventional methods are necessary to perturb a particular frequency band and observe if temporal perception is commensurately affected. Furthermore, the particular way in which timing may be changed as a result of this perturbation can be informative regarding the precise mechanism that has been affected. Time perception may be fractionated into distinct cognitive or perceptual states, such that the perception of time must entail encoding, storage, retrieval and comparison processes29,30; different oscillatory frequencies may similarly be associated with these different stages21.

To address the above issues and directly test the beta-timing hypothesis, we tested subjects on a sub-second visual temporal bisection task, in which subjects must categorize stimulus durations into “long” and “short” categories31. To directly test the contribution of beta oscillations, we applied transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS), a type of noninvasive electrical stimulation in which the frequency can be specified, at both beta (20 Hz) and alpha (10 Hz) frequencies in separate sessions. Studies of tACS have demonstrated the efficacy of 1 milliamp stimulation at entraining neural populations to a specified frequency and increasing the power at that band (e.g.32). Further, as few studies have investigated the role of beta oscillations during non-motor time perception tasks, we re-analyzed two previously collected EEG datasets33,34 recorded from tasks similar to the present one. In one33, subjects were tested on an auditory sub-second temporal bisection task of the exact same design as the one used in the present study. This study was originally an investigation of the CNV signal and its role in memory for temporal intervals, demonstrating that the amplitude of the CNV signal on a trial-by-trial basis reflected both the duration of the present trial and changes in the average perceived duration across trials. In the other34, subjects performed a visual sub-second temporal discrimination task, while repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) was applied to the right supramarginal gyrus (SMG) of the parietal cortex, or to a control site in the midline at the junction of the occipital and parietal cortices, prior to the onset of the standard stimulus (600 ms). This study also focused on the CNV, demonstrating that stimulation of the SMG led to an increase in amplitude during the presentation of the standard stimulus, but with no change during the comparison stimulus. Further, the increase in CNV amplitude paralleled a shift in perception, such that subjects perceived the standard stimulus as longer in duration, with the size of both effects correlating between subjects. Accordingly, in re-analyzing these two datasets, we hoped to identify whether beta oscillations were implicated in time perception, as previously demonstrated by other studies. Further, the re-analysis allowed us to investigate timing in two non-motor paradigms, such that the report of stimulus duration was not dependent on the timing of the motor response.

Lastly, in order to identify precisely which aspect of time perception may have been influenced by the application of tACS, we decomposed choice and reaction time data from the temporal bisection task with a drift diffusion model (DDM) of decision making35,36. Recent work has demonstrated that temporal perception may be constructed as a variant of the DDM, known as the Time-Adaptive Opponent Process Drift Diffusion Model (TopDDM)37, in which opponent populations of neurons provide a bound for temporal accumulation. An extension of this model to decision making in 2AFC designs38 has demonstrated that the standard DDM, in which decision evidence accumulates to one of two boundaries at a particular rate, can well describe performance on the temporal bisection task. As such, particular elements of the DDM can be tied to distinct processing stages in time perception. We here applied a hierarchical DDM (HDDM)36, in which Bayesian posterior estimates of model parameters are determined via Markov Chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) sampling, and the individual subject estimates are constrained, hierarchically, by the mean group estimate. This procedure has been successful at describing performance on temporal bisection38 with findings predicted by TopDDM39.

Results

EEG Re-analyses

The majority of research demonstrating an effect of beta frequency power on temporal processing have utilized motor-based timing paradigms, wherein the motor response determines the interval being measured or entrained. To begin examining the impact of distinct frequency bands in timing, we first re-analyzed a prior dataset33 using an auditory temporal bisection task with sub-second stimuli, log-spaced between 300 and 900 ms. In this study, a difference in the amplitude of stimulus-locked event-related responses (ERPs), specifically in the CNV at frontocentral electrodes, maximal over electrode FCz. The presentation order of stimuli in this study was additionally first-order counterbalanced, such that every possible transition from one stimulus duration to another occurred an equal number of times; this allowed for the examination of so-called direct and carryover effects in these data40. Behaviorally, this study demonstrated a carryover effect, wherein the bisection point (BP) of the psychophysical function shifted, depending on the duration of the stimulus presented on the previous trial; longer prior trial durations were associated with a rightward shift in the psychophysical function, characterized by fewer classifications of the present trial duration as “long”. The CNV response similarly showed a shift in amplitude with the prior trial duration, with higher amplitudes for longer prior trial durations. This finding was interpreted as evidence for the CNV signal incorporating the previous trial duration, and serving as a memory standard by which the present trial duration was judged. When spectral power was examined for each duration tested in our stimulus set, stimulus-locked activity exhibited a stereotyped pattern of ERS at lower, alpha frequency between 200 and 400 ms post-onset, followed by an ERD of higher, beta activity between 400 and 800 ms post-onset (Supplementary Fig. 1). These windows correspond to the P300 and CNV components observed in the broadband data of the original study. Notably, for the direct effect of duration, no differences were detected at any time/frequency point between any of the tested durations, indicating that spectral power did not differ by the duration presented on each trial (Supplementary Fig. 1). However, instead, we observed a significant cluster in the carryover analysis, by collapsing across all of the present trial durations and only observing how the data changed based on the prior trial duration. Here, we observed a positive cluster that peaked at ~600 ms between 17 and 23 Hz, within the beta frequency band in a frontocentral cluster. This cluster indicated that the spectral power increased as the prior trial duration increased (Fig. 1B).

Figure 1
figure 1

EEG Re-analysis of Wiener and Thompson33. (A) Time/frequency plot of slope values for a linear regression of power with the stimulus duration (electrode FCz). Notably, no effect was found for the present trial duration (Direct Effect), only the previous trial (Carryover Effect). A significant cluster within the beta frequency range (17–23 Hz) was observed, peaking at approximately 600 ms. (B) Scalp distribution of mean slope values from within the white box inset, demonstrating a frontocentral peak at FCz. Significant electrodes are highlighted in green (cluster corrected p < 0.05). (C) Time-course of mean beta power in the 17–23 Hz range for both direct (bottom) and carryover (top) effects, demonstrating the staggering of late beta power with duration for the prior but not present trial duration.

The finding that beta power was modulated by the prior, but not present trial duration, follows the CNV findings of the original study. In this work, the CNV also covaried linearly with the prior trial duration, with a higher amplitude for longer prior trial durations. In that study, this shift in the CNV was tied to fluctuations in the categorical boundary for classifying durations as long or short; a higher CNV amplitude for longer prior durations was associated with an increased probability of subsequently classifying the present duration as short. This finding was interpreted as the categorical boundary being shifted towards the value of the prior trial duration31. Yet, the CNV signal here was also modulated by the present trial duration. The present findings with beta suggest that beta power is associated with the current value of the categorical boundary, or bisection point.

To further examine time/frequency effects, we next re-analyzed data from a simultaneous TMS-EEG study while subjects performed a visual temporal discrimination task34. In this second re-analysis, we compared spectral changes resulting from stimulation of either the rSMG or midline occipital cortex (Mid-Occ) prior to the onset of a 600 ms standard stimulus, which subjects were required to encode and subsequently judge against a comparison stimulus. In our original study, we found that stimulation of the rSMG, but not Mid-Occ, increased the CNV amplitude, and that the size of this effect correlated with an increased likelihood to judge the comparison stimulus as shorter than the standard; in other words, the perceived duration of the standard stimulus increased. When comparing rSMG and Mid-Occ stimulation conditions, we observed an increase in spectral power for a cluster spanning 300 to 500 ms post stimulus onset between 25 and 27 Hz, within the high beta frequency range (Fig. 2). The window of this effect again corresponds to the CNV signal observed in the original study, and follows from the above re-analysis that an increase in beta frequency power follows an increase in judging the stimulus as long.

Figure 2
figure 2

EEG Re-analysis of Wiener, et al.34. (A) Time/frequency representation of the difference between rSMG and Mid-Occ stimulation revealed an increase in high beta power following rSMG stimulation (electrode Cz; highlighted regions are significant at p < 0.05, uncorrected for visualization purposes). (B) Scalp distribution of beta power differences from within the white box at inset, representing the cluster-corrected region of signifiance. (C) The time-course of mean beta power from electrode Cz in the high beta range.

Altogether, our findings suggest that the power of beta frequency oscillations contributes to the value of a remembered duration. In the re-analysis of Wiener and Thompson’s33 data, beta covariance with the prior trial duration suggests that this signal indexes the criterion duration being used for comparison to the present trial duration. The re-analysis of Wiener and colleagues’34 data, in turn, suggests that beta power is modulated by rSMG stimulation while subjects are encoding a duration into memory that will subsequently be compared to another duration. In either case, we note that higher beta power is associated with longer remembered durations.

tACS – Simulation

We analyzed simulation data of the electrical field induced by tACS at our electrode locations (FC1 & FC2). Using a finite element head model of a canonical brain, we observed a maximal normalized electrical field at the location of the SMA (Fig. 3C). The effect covered both hemispheres, suggesting the effect of stimulation did not result from a lateralized impact.

Figure 3
figure 3

(A) Task design for visual temporal bisection. Subjects viewed a fixation point for 500 ms, and where then presented with a visual stimulus (Gaussian luminance blur) for one of seven possible durations, logarithmically-spaced between 300 and 900 ms. At offset, subjects were required to classify the presented stimulus into “long” or “short” duration categories. (B) Experimental setup. All subjects started each session by performing a baseline version of the temporal bisection task. Following this, tACS electrodes were administered on the subject and stimulation was initiated; all subjects performed a baseline questionnaire at this time. In the stimulation run, subjects again performed the bisection task, while concurrently receiving stimulation (10 Hz or 20 Hz, separate days). Following stimulation, subjects again filled out a questionnaire. (C) At top, the electrode montage used for tACS, with electrodes placed over FC1 and FC2 in the international 10–20 system. At bottom, the results of electric field modeling for our chosen montage, demonstrating a maximal electric field generation within the supplementary motor area.

tACS – Behavioral Analysis

We next applied tACS at beta and alpha frequency bands in a new group of subjects (n = 19) during a visual temporal bisection task. All subjects performed the task well, with no difference between stimulation conditions on the ability to perform the task (Supplementary Fig. 2); a multivariate ANOVA of post-stimulation questionnaires did not reveal any significant differences between alpha and beta tACS on any of our measures, including tingling sensations and phosphenes (all p > 0.05; supplementary material). Here, psychometric and chronometric curves exhibited well-described effects31,33, with an increasing probability of classifying a stimulus as long with increasing duration, and a corresponding decrease in reaction times for longer stimuli (Fig. 4). Notably, the bisection point (BP) for subjects in our sample was located above values typically found41, at a value above the arithmetic mean of the stimulus set [mean BP: 0.59 ± 0.027]; no difference between baseline BPs were observed [Wilcoxon Signed Rank Test: two-tailed z = −1.127, p = 0.278, based on 10,000 Monte Carlo simulations]. For chronometric data, a repeated measures ANOVA with frequency (alpha,beta), stimulation (baseline, concurrent), and duration (seven, log-spaced intervals) as within-subject factors revealed a significant main effect of stimulation [F(1,18) = 21.447, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.54], but no interaction with the frequency of stimulation [F(1,18) = 0.267, p = 0.612]. This effect was characterized by faster reaction times for both stimulation frequencies, with no difference in the size of the increase. For psychometric data, we conducted separate repeated measures ANCOVAs on BP and coefficient of variation (CV) values, including frequency and stimulation as within-subjects factors, as well as mean trial count as a covariate (supplementary materials). For BP values, we detected no main effect of frequency [F(1,17) = 0.992, p = 0.333] or stimulation [F(1,17) = 0.535, p = 0.475], but did observe a significant interaction [F(1,17) = 6.527, p = 0.021, η2p = 0.277]. No significant effects were observed for a similar analysis of CV values (all p > 0.05). Post-hoc Signed Rank Tests revealed that beta, but not alpha, stimulation led to a significant decrease in the BP value [alpha: z = −1.408, p = 0.165; beta: z = −2.857, p = 0.003, Cohen’s d = −0.463], indicating a greater propensity to classify duration stimuli as “long” when receiving beta stimulation compared to baseline. We additionally noted differences in the baseline BP between Alpha and Beta tACS sessions, with subjects exhibiting a slightly higher mean BP in the Beta tACS session than the Alpha tACS session [0.606 vs 0.574]. However, a Signed Rank test comparing these values revealed no significant difference between them [z = −1.127, p = 0.26], and also a significant correlation between these values [r = 0.617, p = 0.005]. We note that our previous reports have also exhibited differences in baseline between sessions on different days34,42, an effect that is likely common across different timing measurements43. To ensure our results were not driven by these differences, we re-ran our ANCOVA, including session order as an additional covariate. Our site by stimulation effect remained significant [F(1,16) = 6.193, p = 0.024], and no interaction with session order was found for any effect (all p > 0.05).

Figure 4
figure 4

Results of tACS on temporal bisection. Top and middle graphs feature psychometric data, with the average proportion at which each interval was classified as “long”. Top graphs display the mean and spread of bisection points; inner bars and outer bars represent 95% confidence intervals and the standard deviation, respectively. bottom graphs feature chronometric functions, with the average reaction time for each interval, regardless of choice. Gumbel distribution curves for psychometric data are fit to the average proportions for visualization purposes only. Individual open data points on top panels represent the bisection point for all subjects. Stimulation was found to reduce reaction time for both frequencies, whereas beta stimulation alone significantly shifted the bisection point leftward, characterized by a greater propensity to classify stimuli as “long”. Error bars represent +/− standard error.

Altogether, the behavioral data indicated a decrease in the BP value for beta stimulation, with no change from alpha stimulation. This effect was also independent of any changes in the CV value across stimulation conditions. Further, we observed no differences in any measures of carryover from the previous trial, either decisional or perceptual31, indicating that the effect of stimulation on the BP was not the result of a sequential effect of the prior trial duration or decision (all p > 0.05; Supplementary Fig. 3).

Drift Diffusion Modeling

In order to further isolate the cause of the shift in BP for beta stimulation, we decomposed choice and reaction time data with a drift diffusion model36,38; Fig. 5. Model fits demonstrated that the best-fitting or “winning” model for our design was one in which frequency, stimulation, and duration were all included (Supplementary Fig. 4). This improvement in model fit was consistent for hierarchical, non-hierarchical, and regression methods. Analysis of the Gelman-Rubin statistic for multiple chains revealed a value of 1.0016 ± 0.0061 (SD), indicating good chain stability. Visual inspection of model chains confirmed this finding, by demonstrating symmetrical traces and distributions, along with low autocorrelation levels. Finally, we performed a posterior predictive check of the winning model44 by simulating 500 datasets using the mean posterior estimates for each node. Comparison of these datasets with the original data demonstrated that the model was able to reproduce the pattern observed in the actual dataset (Supplementary Fig. 5)

Figure 5
figure 5

Example of the TopDDM model for temporal bisection (cf.37). On a given trial, at the onset of a to-be-timed interval, a first-stage drift process is initiated that accumulates at a particular rate (A) until duration offset occurs. At offset, a second-stage is initiated, with the ending accumulated time estimate of the first-stage serving as the starting point (z). From here, a decision variable accumulates towards one of two decision boundaries (a) at a particular rate (v). The direction of the drift depends on where the starting point lies relative to a categorical boundary that is used for classifying stimuli. In the example above, the same interval may be categorized as short or long, depending on the first-stage drift rate.

We initially attempted in our model to replicate findings from37, in their original formulation of the TopDDM model, as applied to temporal bisection. This model predicted four distinct patterns for a, v, t, and z values across duration (see Methods). Observation of the model fit values demonstrated the expected patterns for all four parameters (Fig. 6). Specifically, we observed that threshold (a) values decreased for duration values closer to the middle of the stimulus set, and increased for extreme duration values. Drift (v) values were observed as negative for durations lower than the mean of the stimulus set, and positive for values above it, increasing linearly. Non-decision time (t) values decreased linearly for longer duration values. Finally, we predicted that the starting point (z) parameter values would increase linearly with duration. While a repeated measures ANOVA of starting point values did indicate a linear contrast with duration [F(1,18) = 83.701, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.823], we note that starting point values monotonically increased only until the middle of the stimulus set (0.52 s), before decreasing with longer durations. This pattern, while not predicted, does match with earlier electrophysiological findings from the temporal bisection task, which indicate that subjects accumulate information only until the categorical boundary (bisection point) is reached33.

Figure 6
figure 6

Fits of model parameters for the hierarchical drift diffusion model. Insets represent model parameters collapsed across duration for each stimulation condition. Shaded regions and error bars represent standard deviation across subjects. Model parameters confirm earlier findings37 and show that stimulation decreases the threshold and increases the drift rate at both frequency ranges, but exclusively increases the starting point for beta stimulation. No impact on non-decision time was found.

Further comparison between stimulation conditions revealed a number of distinct findings for alpha and beta stimulation. First, we observed that the threshold parameter decreased for both alpha and beta stimulation [F(1,18) = 10.563, p = 0.004, η2p = 0.37]; yet, while this decrease was noticeably larger for alpha stimulation, no interaction between these effects was observed [F(1,18) = 2.441, p = 0.136]. For the drift parameter, a main effect of stimulation was again observed, with the drift rate overall shifting upwards (closer to the “long” duration boundary) [F(1,18) = 47.174, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.724], but again with no interaction between stimulation frequencies [F(1,18) = 0.556, p = 0.465]. For non-decision time, no main effect of stimulation was observed [F(1,18) = 0.193, p = 0.66] nor an interaction with frequency [F(1,18) = 0.012, p = 0.914]. Finally, for the starting point parameter, no main effect of stimulation was observed [F(1,18) = 0.929, p = 0.348]; however, an interaction between stimulation and frequency was found [F(1,18) = 12.612, p = 0.002, η2p = 0.412], with the starting point increasing closer to the long duration boundary for beta [Wilcoxon z = −2.857, p = 0.002, Cohen’s d = −0.46], but not alpha [Wilcoxon z = −1.368, p = 0.182] stimulation (for additional validation of this effect, please see supplementary materials).

Discussion

Beta oscillations have emerged in the past few years as a candidate mechanism for coordinating timing functions in the human brain. These findings suggest that beta oscillations have a supramodal property, in that they cover timing and predictive abilities regardless of the task context or modality of timed stimuli. In order to determine what role, if any, beta plays, we conducted two re-analyses of previously-collected EEG data, followed by a novel tACS study that employed stimulation at two distinct frequency bands (beta and alpha) while subjects performed a temporal bisection task. The result of these analyses point to a specific role for beta oscillations in timing.

In re-examining previously collected EEG data from an auditory temporal bisection task33, we first observed a curious finding: although oscillatory power, spanning a wide range that encompassed the beta band was modulated by the choice on each trial, with relatively lower beta power (desynchronization) prior to classifying a stimulus as “long” in duration, this effect was not parametrically modulated by the duration presented on each trial. Instead, beta power covaried linearly with the duration presented on the previous trial, with higher beta power for longer durations. One possible explanation for this finding is that beta power indexed the duration to which each presented value was being compared. That is, beta power serves as an index of the memory standard by which durations are categorized. Consider, the memory standard, as measured by the bisection point of the observed psychometric function, fluctuates on a trial-by-trial basis, and gravitates towards whatever duration was presented on the prior trial31,45. As such, beta power may reflect here a shift in the value of the memory standard over time. From a Bayesian perspective, this may be interpreted as an iterative updating of the prior probability distribution. Consistent with this view, recent evidence suggests that beta oscillations may track the trial-by-trial weight of evidence for making decisions46.

In our second re-analysis, we examined the effects of rTMS on the rSMG of the parietal lobe while subjects performed a visual temporal discrimination task. In this task, the temporal referent that subjects must use for comparison is presented on every trial. Stimulation applied immediately prior to the onset of this standard stimulus was found to shift comparison responses in a manner to suggest that subjects encoded the standard stimulus as longer in duration than it actually was33. The original analysis also found that the frontocentral CNV signal during the encoding of the standard stimulus increased with stimulation. Similarly, we observed an increase in high beta power during the encoding of stimulus duration. The direction of this effect is consistent with what we observed in our previous re-analysis – increased beta power was associated with a longer remembered duration. We reconcile this finding with the re-analysis of the previous study by noting that, here, subjects were encoding the standard stimulus duration into memory. As such, the observed increase in beta power may reflect a longer encoded duration. Similar findings have been observed in the interval timing literature in animals, where pharmacological manipulation alters remembered duration via a hypothetical memory translation constant47, invoked as a part of scalar timing theory29. Altogether, both re-analyses established a supramodal role for beta oscillations in time perception, that may be linked to the retention of a memory standard for duration, rather than the perceived duration itself.

One noteworthy difference between these two re-analyses is that the peak of beta changes occurred at different frequencies. In the first re-analysis, beta activity significantly changed in the 17–23 Hz range, whereas in the second re-analysis it was between 25–27 Hz. While the second study range is in the so-called “upper” beta range, there is currently no consensus on the functional difference between these ranges48; as such, any distinction made here between these two ranges is merely speculative. However, some work suggests that the difference between these ranges is determined by both dopamine tone49 and task context50. As such, the fact that each re-analysis came from a different timing task (auditory temporal bisection vs visual temporal discrimination) may have contributed to the difference.

While the above re-analyses are suggestive of the involvement of beta oscillations in perceptual timing, they do not reveal a mediating link between beta power and timing functions. Indeed, we included these re-analyses as a means solely to establish and suggest what role, if any, beta oscillations may have for timing. To establish this, we applied tACS at two distinct frequencies during a visual temporal bisection task. Here, we first observed that stimulation at both alpha and beta frequencies led to a decrease in reaction time functions, such that subjects were faster at making responses when receiving stimulation compared to when not. Although it may be tempting to state that stimulation made subjects faster, it should be noted that the pre-post design of our study leaves open the possibility that subjects were faster due to practice with the task; stimulation always occurred after the baseline task (Fig. 1), and so subjects may have improved simply by familiarity with the task demands. A second observation was that the bisection point (BP) significantly shifted leftward for beta, but not alpha stimulation. In this case, subjects became more likely to classify stimuli as “long” in duration. Notably, this effect was independent of any carryover effect from the previous trial or decisional bias from the previous choice31.

In order to further disentangle the possible bases for this effect, we decomposed choice and reaction time data with a drift diffusion model inspired by recent work (TopDDM)37. Across several methods of fitting the data to individual subjects, three findings were apparent. First, the threshold boundary, which the decision variable must cross in order for a response choice to be made, was lower for both alpha and beta stimulation regimes, as compared to baseline, indicating that subjects were less cautious, requiring less evidence, before making a choice51. Second, the drift parameter was significantly higher overall for both alpha and beta stimulation. In this case, a “higher” drift parameter indicates that the decision variable drifted more quickly towards the long duration boundary than the short duration one. This is due to the way in which we parameterized drift: negative drift values indicate an accumulation towards the short duration boundary, whereas positive values indicate accumulation to the long duration boundary (cf. 38). Finally, the starting point parameter moved closer to the long duration boundary, but only for beta, and not alpha stimulation. For many formulations of the drift diffusion model, a change in the starting point indicates a shift in bias, such that subjects favor one response over another and so require less evidence before committing to a decision in that direction. Here, a shift in the starting point carries a different interpretation; specifically, the starting point is meant to reflect the value of the first-stage timing process A at stimulus offset (Fig. 2). A higher starting point thus reflects a larger subjective value of time. We note that it is unlikely that the effects observed were due to a simple shift in bias; if that had occurred, we would likely also have seen a subsequent impact on the carryover effect of the previous decision31, which was not observed.

Functional consequence of beta and alpha manipulation

The results of the drift diffusion modeling suggest that beta oscillations exclusively shift the starting point of the decision process in temporal bisection, an effect that may be tied to a change in the first-stage temporal accumulator value. Yet, we note that a change in this effect does not match well with the observed findings. At first glance, an increase in the temporal readout value used for measuring subjective time would be expected to make subjects classify stimuli as long more often. However, the temporal bisection task requires subjects to continuously compare each duration to a running average of the durations they previously experienced. As such, every longer-perceived duration would in turn go into the memory used for judgment on the next trial. As such, subjects would gradually shift to using a new memory standard for the comparison of durations, and any difference from the shift in readout would disappear, consistent with so-called “clock pattern” effects observed for pharmacological agents affecting the dopamine system47. Further, the results of our EEG re-analyses did not suggest a role of beta oscillations in subjective time readout, as evidenced by a lack of effect for beta power while subjects measured stimuli of different lengths. Instead, these findings suggest a role for beta in the memory process, specifically for the encoding of durations into memory. Here, the effect of stimulation would be to shift the boundary used for comparing durations, rather than the perception of any individual duration itself. The effect would be comparable to a “memory pattern” effect, in which the psychophysical function shifts away from baseline and remains separate, as accurately perceived intervals are subsequently entered into memory as a different value. To disentangle which pattern exists in the data, we conducted a subsequent sliding-window analysis of the data. Here, we used a sliding window of 131 trials – the minimum needed for a sufficient trial count for each duration to accurately fit the data – and continuously analyzed the bisection point over the course of the stimulation session. The results are presented in Fig. 7, and show that, contrary to a clock pattern, the effect was maintained throughout the session, consistent with a memory pattern.

Figure 7
figure 7

Sliding window analysis of within-session effects. A sliding window of 131 trials was ran across choice data for each session and the bisection point was calculated within each window as described in the methods. This window was chosen as the minimum size needed for each of the seven durations to have the minimum number of trials needed to fit a psychometric function (n = 8). Data displayed above are smoothed by a 10-point moving average for the visualization of within-session trends. At right, frequency distributions of the bisection point values for each trace. The overall finding of above is that the shift in the bisection point values for beta stimulation are largely consistent across the session, rather than larger at one point in time over another. Shaded regions represent +/− standard error.

One noteworthy feature of the memory pattern observed is that stimulation induced a change that was consistent across the stimulation session. This finding aligns with our null result from the carryover analysis (supplementary materials), in which beta tACS did not induce any change in perceptual carryover from the previous trial. This change would be expected if beta stimulation induced a local instability in the memory standard, whereas our sliding window analysis suggests instead that a global shift in the average value of the memory standard occurred, with local fluctuations, based on preceding trial history, were preserved.

To reconcile the memory pattern we observed with the drift diffusion model findings, we suggest that the starting point, rather than representing a subjective time estimate at offset, instead reflects in-part the location of the categorical boundary. Under this framework, if the categorical boundary is shifted lower, then the distribution of intervals in the stimulus set becomes uneven, with more long intervals than short ones. As such, the starting point will shift towards the long duration boundary because long durations are more likely to occur. Further testing will be necessary to see if this bears out in behavioral data, by testing subjects with different distributions of intervals that are unevenly skewed towards the longest or shortest durations in the stimulus set, a difference that has been shown to reliably shift the bisection point45.

Anatomical locus of beta stimulation

In the present study, tACS was administered frontocentrally, over electrodes FC1 and FC2. Electric field modeling in a simulated subject suggests the impact of this montage is maximal within the confines of the supplementary motor area (SMA). We chose this site on the basis of functional evidence suggesting the SMA serves as part of a core network mediating time perception5. Indeed, numerous studies now demonstrate SMA activity while subjects are engaged in a timing task52. With regard to the precise function of this region, current debate exists regarding its role in timing22,53. One particular theory is that the SMA reflects a temporal accumulator process, used for measuring elapsed durations6. In this case, the SMA would correspond to the first-stage drift process of TopDDM. Yet, as observed, the shift in the bisection point function for beta stimulation is unlikely to be driven by a clock pattern effect. A second theory is that SMA activity reflects the memory criterion for making temporal decisions26,33. Given the pattern of results described above, we suggest this to be the more likely function of the SMA for timing.

Outside of the timing literature, beta oscillations have previously been associated with the SMA with a variety of functions. Indeed, beta-band oscillations have traditionally been associated with motor functions17, and although the present study required a speeded response, beta activity covaried with the duration presented on the previous trial, not the present one, suggesting an independence from motor processes. Neural oscillations have been suggested as an organizational framework for neuronal networks, whereby activity may be synchronized across neural ensembles54. In this regard, beta oscillations may be less well understood than oscillations in other frequency bands46. However, recent research has implicated frontocentral beta oscillations in numerous non-motor functions, such as perceptual decision-making55 and local visual feature processing56. Yet, this does not preclude the involvement of the motor system in non-motor functions. Indeed, the SMA and basal ganglia, likely generators of the CNV and beta oscillations observed in the present study10,16,57, have been implicated in temporal perception, regardless of movement requirements5. Importantly, here we distinguish the beta oscillations observed from other beta oscillations in the brain. That is, given the anatomical locus of our effect was confined to the SMA, we likely impacted beta oscillations within the cortico-striato-thalamic network. In contrast, beta oscillations have also been observed at occipital and parietal sites, with beta power in this region suggested to underlie visual excitability25. We suggest it unlikely that our stimulation paradigm impacted visual experience directly, as subjects did not report a higher number of phosphenes or other visual phenomenon when receiving beta stimulation over alpha stimulation.

Another noteworthy aspect of our study is that stimulation effects of the SMA on timing are rare58. Indeed, the SMA has been stimulated in numerous paradigms with transcranial magnetic stimulation, but only two studies that we are aware of have demonstrated an effect59,60. Both studies employed continuous theta burst stimulation, a method that has been suggested to induce a stronger and longer-lasting effect than conventional, repetitive TMS paradigms. However, in both cases, the effects observed were either weak59 or indistinguishable from stimulation at other sites60. One study that we are aware of has stimulated the SMA with tES61. Here, the authors found that transcranial random noise stimulation (tRNS), a method in which the stimulation frequency varies randomly between 0.1 and 640 Hz, had no impact on timing when administered over the SMA. Given that our findings point to a specific role of beta frequency stimulation, one possible explanation for these discrepancies is that SMA effects are frequency dependent. As such, stimulation of this region is more likely to lead to an effect if the paradigm includes a dominant frequency within the beta band.

Physiological locus of beta stimulation

The results of our re-analysis of two EEG datasets, as well as our stimulation findings, provide an overall role for beta oscillations in temporal memory. Moreover, these findings suggest a linear relationship between memory and beta power, with relative increases in beta power associated with longer retained duration memories. This finding is consistent with previous work, demonstrating a relationship between beta power and timing1. The closest analogue to our findings is a re-analysis by Kononowicz and van Rijn8 in which higher beta power was associated with a longer produced duration. In this study, subjects were required to press a button when a prescribed duration had elapsed (2.5-seconds). Here, according to scalar timing theory, a produced interval will be randomly longer if either the temporal accumulation rate is high, or if the retrieved memory used for a criterion is long. Our findings suggest it is the latter case that explains the association between high beta power and longer productions.

With regards to the physiological basis of our effect, we note that beta oscillations, particular in the motor circuit, are highly associated with dopamine and cholinergic function62,63. Both neurotransmitter systems have been implicated in timing functions64. Indeed, dopamine and acetylcholine manipulation in animals are capable of inducing clock and memory patterns in timing behavior, respectively. One particular impact of beta stimulation may have been to alter the dynamic balance between these two neurotransmitter systems, thus inducing the change in performance we observed. Whether or not beta oscillations observed with EEG or impacted with tACS change, depending on the dopamine state65 or effective level of acetylcholine66, remains to be seen.

Alpha oscillations and time perception

The lack of a consistent effect for alpha stimulation also presents an interesting finding. Although we did not find any effect in the alpha range in our EEG re-analyses, alpha oscillations have previously been associated with time estimation, particularly for visual stimuli1. Notably, although no significant effects were observed in mean differences across subjects, a closer examination of the distribution of performance with alpha stimulation reveals an interesting finding. Specifically, during alpha stimulation, we observed that the bisection point values became more heterogeneous between subjects as compared to prior to stimulation. This observation was confirmed with a simple two-sample F test comparing the variances of stimulation and pre-stimulation conditions, revealing that the distribution of bisection points became more variable across subjects during stimulation [F(1,18) = 4.273, p = 0.0035]; no difference in variances was observed for beta stimulation [F(1,18) = 1.608, p = 0.3221]. This finding suggests that alpha stimulation may have had some impact, but one that was inconsistent between subjects.

One possible explanation for this effect is the finding that alpha stimulation can demonstrably impact individual alpha frequency (IAF)28. More specifically, IAF is thought to reflect the intrinsic resting “peak” of alpha oscillations in an individual subject. Recent work has found that IAF also correlates with perceived visual timing27,67, and that manipulation of this rate can reliably shift the perceived timing of visual events28. However, much of this work relies on using a baseline measure of IAF in each subject, and then stimulating each individual at that rate68. In our case, all subjects were stimulated at the same rate (10 Hz), and so some subjects may have had their IAF speed up and other slow down. If the speed of intrinsic alpha oscillations determines in part the timing of visual stimuli, it’s possible that alpha stimulation would induce a stable shift in BP across subjects. Notably, this shift would occur in the same direction as that observed for beta tACS, with a leftward shift in the psychometric function and a lower BP. However, if alpha oscillations mediate the timing of visual stimuli, then it is possible that this effect would be akin to speeding up the internal clock, and so would induce a clock pattern in the BP, one that is initially high at the start of the session, but then gradually normalizes over the course of the session as subjects adjust to the new clock speed. However, without titrating stimulation frequency to IAF, this effect would wash out across subjects, potentially leading to the increase in between-subject variability we observed. By using a baseline measure of IAF, this effect could be explored with further tACS.

Limitations and Future Directions

In the present study, we began from a place of evaluating what evidence, if any, exists for beta oscillations and time estimation. Specifically, we wanted to see if beta power was tied to perceptual timing, in which a motor component was not required to demarcate the intervals. Although previous work suggests a strong overlap between perceptual and motor timing tasks5 beta activity observed during motor timing may be incidental, rather than essential. For this, our re-analyses of two previous datasets were conducted because neither required a timed motor response. Yet, we note here that the purpose of these re-analyses was not to fully characterize oscillatory functions in timing, as neither original experiment was designed to look at oscillatory effects, and so are less than optimal for reaching strong conclusions. We also here distinguish our re-analyses from that done by Kononowicz and van Rijn8, which more thoroughly re-examined CNV activity for oscillatory effects. Nevertheless, our re-analyses provide converging evidence across paradigms for a supramodal role for beta power in time estimation, which further studies will need to disentangle.

Another limitation of the present study is that our results only apply to perceptual timing. It remains possible that beta tACS may also lead to disruptions on other tasks requiring judgment between other magnitude stimuli (i.e. pitch, numerosity, size), or they may be specific to time judgments. Yet, we note here that our results need not apply only to time; indeed, recent work has begun to show that beta oscillations are evoked during a variety of perceptual decision making tasks69,70,71. One possibility, then, is that beta oscillations serve to index a remembered standard not just for time, but for a variety of perceptual phenomena72. Future studies with similar paradigms will be necessary to disentangle the role of beta oscillations across different perceptual stimuli, to determine if the supramodal property of beta oscillations suggested here extends to a wider taxon.

An additional limitation is that our study only included stimulation of a single site. Although the choice of this site location was driven by previous evidence suggesting the SMA as a supramodal target for time estimation and beta oscillations, our findings do not disentangle the specificity of the frequency effect at this site. Indeed, in our previous TMS-EEG study, used here for a re-analysis, it is worth noting that the parietal cortex was stimulated at 10 Hz, which nevertheless evoked a 20 Hz response at central electrodes. One possibility, then, is that the effect of stimulation is dependent on intrinsic frequency of the region being stimulated. Indeed, evidence suggests that distinct cortical regions exhibit dominant frequency bands73. Disentangling the responsiveness of a region from the impact of stimulation frequency will thus require a more nuanced approach, in which stimulation sites and frequencies are distinguished.

Our findings also present another curiosity – if higher beta power is associated with longer duration memories, then in order to explain our results, tACS must have reduced beta power. tACS is an effective means for entraining oscillations at a particular frequency band, but whether the effects raised or lowered power in the current study cannot be demonstrated. Yet, we note that the impact of tACS on oscillatory power is highly state-dependent32,74,75. Feurra and colleagues76 demonstrated that the effect of beta tACS was dependent on the motor imagery a subject was engaged with at the time of stimulation. Further recent work has shown that the effects of tACS on beta activity are highly contingent on the baseline state76. In our study, we suggest that stimulation effects likely interacted with the active necessity of the subject to maintain vigilance. However, the only way to truly demonstrate whether tACS raises or lowers beta power in our design would be to collect simultaneous EEG during the tACS task. Although simultaneous tACS-EEG acquisition is feasible32, the purpose of our study was to demonstrate the effectiveness of tACS in manipulating time perception. A next step, then, is to measure the EEG response both before and after stimulation; further, due to the short-lasting effects of tACS stimulation77, future work can attempt to determine the temporal specificity of the effect by manipulating when within a given trial stimulation takes place.

Conclusions

The results of our two re-analyses and subsequent tACS study together provide a fundamental linkage between beta oscillations and time perception. These findings suggest that beta power contributes to the duration of an interval in memory, which impacts how a subject will judge future intervals. Further, our results are unlikely to be due to alternative effects on motor preparation or decision bias, and instead reflect an intrinsic function of beta oscillations. These findings may be connected to future work on neural oscillations and brain stimulation in time estimation. Further, it leaves open the possibility of other frequency bands not explored here, such as delta and theta frequency, or high gamma frequency, each of which may be involved in their own functions for timing.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

20 healthy, right-handed subjects (12 male; Mean age: 24 ± 4 years) participated in the tACS experiment. One subject was removed due to an error in data collection where the same stimulation frequency was administered twice, resulting in an n of 19. All subjects were tested at the University of Pennsylvania, and drawn from the general population of the area via flyers. Exclusion criteria were (1) history of active neurological or psychiatric disease, (2) history of seizures or unexplained consciousness, (3) history of brain surgery, craniotomy, or other breach of the skull, (4) current consumption of anti-convulsant, anti-psychotic, or sedative/hypnotic medications, (5) participation in other brain stimulation experiments on the same day, (6) prior adverse reaction to non-invasive brain stimulation, (6), metallic hardware in or on the head that could not be removed (excluding dental work), and (7) pregnancy. All subjects gave their informed consent as approved by the University of Pennsylvania Institutional Review Board, and all methods were performed in accordance with University guidelines and regulations. A neurologist was on-call during each session.

Task Design

All subjects performed a visual temporal bisection task, using the ‘partition method’78. Task design was identical to our earlier reports using this task31,33. In the temporal bisection task, subjects must classify a stimulus, presented for a specific duration, into either “long” or “short” duration categories (Fig. 1A). On a given trial, following a 500 ms fixation point, subjects were presented with a Gaussian luminance blur, presented at 100% contrast against a grey background with a FWHM of 2 cm. The visual stimulus could persist for one of seven logarithmically spaced durations between 300 and 900 ms. Following each presentation, subjects were required to classify the stimulus with a button press. Subjects were specifically instructed to classify each stimulus duration by comparing it to the average of all previously presented stimuli. To assist in the classification for the first few trials, each session began with three stimulus presentations at the geometric mean of the stimulus set (520 ms). Following the offset of the stimulus, subjects were instructed to provide a response as quickly, but as accurately, as possible. Similar to our previous reports, the order of stimulus presentation was determined by a path-guided de Bruijn sequence79. Briefly, the de Bruijn sequence allowed us to present all stimulus durations in a sequence that was first-order counterbalanced, such that every possible transition from one duration to another occurred an equal number of times. This allowed us to accurately measure the influence of carryover effects from the previous trial. An additional label for null (empty) trials was added to the sequence, so as to include trial transitions where no stimulus was presented; on null trials, subjects viewed a blank screen for 550 ms, followed by the appearance of the fixation point for the next trial. The resulting trial matrix consisted of 64 possible trial types and a sequence of 448 trials (excluding nulls). Each duration in the total sequence was presented 64 times. The time to run through a single sequence was ~15 minutes.

tACS

The study was a 2-session single-blind experiment, with 2 days between sessions and verum stimulation at either 10 Hz or 20 Hz applied during each session (Fig. 1B). At each session, participants first completed the bisection task with no stimulation. Positions of FC1 and FC2 were then determined using the 10–20 system. These electrodes were chosen to maximize bilateral stimulation of the SMA (see simulation details below). 5 × 5 cm rubber electrodes placed inside sponges soaked in a 0.9% sterile saline solution (MOLTOX) were placed over FC1 and FC2 and held in place with a rubber headband. Impedance was reduced if the safety criteria of the tACS device (Neuroconn Magstim DC-Stimulator) were not met by soaking hair in saline and tightening the headband to improve electrode contact with the scalp. tACS was delivered at 1.5 mA peak to peak and at either 10 Hz or 20 Hz. Participants completed the bisection task once again while they were receiving stimulation, beginning five minutes after the initiation of stimulation. Participants were stimulated for a total of 20 minutes. The tACS frequency of the first session was pseudo-randomized and counterbalanced.

Subjects completed a post-study questionnaire after each session to assess side effects (tingling, itching sensation, burning sensation, pain, fatigue, nervousness, headache, difficulty concentrating, mood change, change in visual perception, visual sensation, or other effects) during and after tACS, using a visual analog scale to rate each category from 0 to 10. After the second session, they were also asked whether they believed they were receiving verum or sham stimulation during each session.

Simulation

To determine the likely physiological source of stimulation, we simulated the effect of our electrode montage using the SimNIBS 2.0 Toolbox (http://simnibs.de/)80. As in our main experiment, the placement of two electrodes (5 × 5 cm) located at FC1 and FC2, were simulated with a current of 1.5 mA. The normalized electrical field was simulated via a realistic finite element head model81. As both frequencies used the same stimulation amplitude, only one simulation was run82.

Behavioral Analysis

Behavioral analysis of the temporal bisection task follows earlier reports31,33. Choice and reaction time data were first calculated for each subject for each of the seven tested durations in our stimulus set. Trials were then filtered to only include those where the RT was between 100 and 1000 ms, to eliminate premature and late responses (see Supplementary Fig. 1 for average # trials removed). The justification for this cutoff was to ensure our ability to measure carryover effects from the previous trial31. Psychometric and chronometric curves were then generated for each participant. Psychometric curves were generated by plotting the proportion of long response choices for each of the seven tested durations; these points were then fitted by a cumulative Gumbel distribution using the psignifit version 3.0 software package (see http://psignifit.sourceforge.net/) for Python83. The Gumbel distribution was chosen to reflect the log-spacing of the duration stimuli, as well as the uncertainty associated with longer stimulus durations84. Upper and lower thresholds, the approximate points at which the subject is 25% or 75% likely to judge the stimulus as long, were calculated using the bias-corrected bootstrap method implemented by psignifit, based on 1999 simulations. The results of this analysis yielded the bisection point (BP; the time value at which subjects were equally likely to judge the stimulus as long or short), the difference limen (DL; the difference between the upper [75%] and lower [25%] threshold values divided in half), and the coefficient of variation (CV; DL/BP). The BP thus reflects the subjective midpoint of the range of tested durations, while the CV reflects the normalized variability of measurements. Chronometric curves were constructed by plotting the RT for each of the seven possible durations.

The use of a de Bruijn sequence in our study, as in our prior work31,33 allows us to investigate the potential impact of carryover effects. Briefly, psychometric and chronometric curves were generated as described above for each of the seven possible prior durations that could have occurred on the previous trial. A positive carryover effect is characterized by a contrast of the present duration away from the duration presented on the previous trial. As such, the BP is expected to shift leftward for trials on which the previous trial was long, and rightward for trials on which the previous trial was short. This can be characterized by the slope of a linear regression of BP values across all seven prior durations. The change in the BP is expected to reflect fluctuations in the mean remembered duration, or criterion value for classifying durations into long and short categories. Additionally, bisection choice data also exhibits a gravitation of the current decision towards the decision made on the previous trial. As such, the BP is shifted to the left if the prior trial’s response was “long” and to the right if the previous trial’s response was “short”, regardless of the duration presented on that trial. This change can be characterized by the difference in BP values between data sorted on the basis of the prior trial response. Both effects were investigated in the present study.

Drift Diffusion Modeling

In order to further parse the source of tACS effects on behavior, choice and reaction time data were decomposed with a drift diffusion model (DDM). In this case, we sought to implement the TopDDM, as described by38. In the original formulation of the TopDDM37, the model assumes that populations of neurons with Poisson firing rates serve as Clock Pulse Generators. Spikes from these generators are integrated by readout neurons over time, while simultaneously inhibited by interneurons that are also triggered by the generators. This process serves to allow for linear accumulation in the integrator neurons, and can be mathematically approximated as a stochastic differential equation \(\,dx=A\cdot dt+m\cdot \sqrt{A}\cdot dB\). The TopDDM thus assumes that, during a timed interval, accumulator neurons integrate the opponent processes, which serves as an elapsed measurement of time for a particular accumulation rate (A). This measurement elapses until the accumulated output crosses a fixed threshold. Notably, TopDDM assumes that, once subjects learn the duration to be timed, the accumulation rate adapts, via a learning rule, such that the drift is higher for shorter durations and lower for longer durations. In the extension to temporal bisection38, the model assumes that once the presented stimulus extinguishes, a second-stage drift diffusion process (v) is triggered that accumulates towards one of two decision boundaries (a). In an elegant extension, the starting point and direction of this second, decision drift process is determined by the end point of the first, accumulator drift process (Fig. 6). Application of the two-stage TopDDM process to temporal bisection data thus provides a decomposition of both accumulator and decision processes during timing.

In the present study, we focused our modeling effort solely on the second-stage process. Justification for this comes from results demonstrating that modeling the second-stage process alone is sufficient to account for bisection data and exhibits the same properties as if both stages had been modeled38,39. These properties include: (1) the drift rate (v) should be negative for durations shorter than the BP and positive for durations longer than the BP; (2) The non-decision time (t) should decrease linearly for longer durations; (3) the starting point (z) should linearly increase for longer durations; (4) the variability of the starting point (sz) should increase for longer durations; (5) The threshold (a) should decrease for durations closer to the BP. To accomplish this, we applied hierarchical Bayesian estimation for these parameters by using routines provided by the HDDM toolbox for Python36, which implements a Bayesian inference, via Markov chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) sampling, for inferring and selecting model parameters. HDDM estimates individual subject parameters by accounting for group-level response distributions36. Markov Chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) methods were then used to determine posterior probability distributions for each parameter included in the model. As described above, we decomposed choice and reaction time data to obtain four measures: threshold (a), drift-rate (v), non-decision-time (t), and starting point (z). For additional details on modeling steps and validation, see supplementary materials.

EEG Re-analysis

Wiener & Thompson33. In order to establish what role, if any, beta oscillations may play in temporal perception, we first re-analyzed data from a previous temporal bisection EEG study33. This study consisted of an auditory temporal bisection task, using the exact same durations and setup as in the present study, with the exception that an auditory stimulus (white noise burst) was used instead of a visual disc. Subjects performed three runs, consisting of 512 trials each. EEG data were collected via 64-channel setup (Synamps2) at 500 Hz with a bandpass filter of 0.1–100 Hz. All preprocessing steps were identical to our earlier study for these data; briefly, the data were rereferenced to the average of two Mastoid electrodes, noisy channels were removed and interpolated, and a trialwise cutoff of ±120 μV was used to eliminate trials with excessive noise. Data were further epoched into both onset-locked (−400–1000 ms) and response-locked (−1000–200 ms) data.

To examine time/frequency effects, we applied a Morlet wavelet convolution, via the EEGLAB newtimef function, by convolving a mother wavelet at 100 log-spaced frequencies spanning 10 to 40 Hz, with 3 cycle wavelets and a 0.5 scaling factor. To examine activity immediately prior to the response, we calculated the difference between spectral power for trials on which the subject classified the stimulus as “long” vs “short”. Significance was assessed via a cluster-based permutation statistic85, based on the maximum cluster size of paired-samples t-tests at each time/frequency point. This procedure most adequately controls for the false-positive rate in cluster inference86. For onset-locked data, we separately examined the impact of the present trial duration (termed direct effect) and the prior trial duration, collapsing across all present trial durations (termed carryover effect). Our original study had demonstrated that the frontocentral CNV signal, maximal at FCz, covaried linearly with both the present and prior trial durations. As such, we focused our analysis on FCz; this was done to remain faithful to the analysis of the original study and provide results here that are directly comparable. To measure direct and carryover effects, we modeled the impact of duration as a linear regression at each time/frequency point with duration. The resulting slope values of the best fitting regression line indicate if power values at a particular time/frequency linearly increased or decreased with duration. These slope values were then tested for significance with a one-sample t-test versus zero (no change with duration) with a cluster correction for multiple comparisons, as described above.

Wiener, et al.34. As a second means of establishing what role, if any, beta oscillations play in temporal perception, we also re-analyzed data from a study by Wiener and colleagues34. In this study, nineteen participants performed a visual temporal discrimination task, wherein a comparison duration must be judged as longer or shorter than a standard duration (600 ms) presented on each trial. The visual stimulus used was a 4 × 4 cm red square; the comparison durations were individually titrated via a baseline adaptive staircase procedure, and included three possible values: 600 ms and the upper and lower thresholds for determining a stimulus as different than 600 ms. EEG was collected via a 32-electrode high-impedance system (BioSemi) at 512 Hz. Additionally, a brief train of repetitive TMS was administered at 10 Hz (3 pulses), at 500 ms prior to the onset of the standard duration. A single run consisted of 180 trials, half of which randomly included TMS. Two separate sessions on separate days were conducted, in which TMS was administered to either the right supramarginal gyrus (rSMG) or the midline-occipital parietal cortex (Mid-Occ). Target locations were individually determined via high-resolution T1-weighted anatomical images and localized using Brainsight (Rogue Research). The rSMG target was approximately over electrode CP6, whereas the Mid-Occ target was approximately over electrode Pz. Each of these electrodes were unplugged and removed during the stimulation session to reduce the distance between the TMS coil and the scalp. Each electrode was interpolated in the resulting EEG analysis.

Preprocessing steps were similar to the previous study, with some exceptions. Data were again offline referenced to the common average of all available channels, with noisy channels removed and interpolated, but a wider offline bandpass filter (infinite impulse response) was applied between 1 and 50 Hz. Data were epoched between −700 and 1000 ms to the onset of the standard stimulus with a ±90 μV cutoff to eliminate noisy trials. This wider epoch, necessary to adequately resolve time/frequency effects for the baseline, included the artifacts induced by TMS. To resolve this, we used a similar strategy for reducing and eliminating the TMS artifact as our previous study by the application of a spatial filter with independent component analysis (ICA). The steps for artifact removal are similar to those outlined by Herring and colleagues87. Specifically, the space of EEG spanning the artifact (−500 to −200 ms prior to standard stimulus onset) was removed from each trial. Infomax ICA was then applied to the data in order to identify components related to residual artifacts resulting from muscle artifacts and the return to baseline. ICA components related to resulting artifacts as well as eye-blinks were determined on a per subject/session basis by visual inspection of the topography and power spectrum of the component. Noise components were then used as a spatial filter for the data by their removal and projection back to the original dataset. Finally, the lost space between −500 and −200 ms was interpolated. Visual inspection of our data confirmed that the ERP results looked qualitatively similar to our original report, with a larger CNV amplitude for rSMG stimulation compared to Mid-Occ stimulation at electrode Cz. As such, we focused our re-analysis here on electrode Cz, again to provide comparable results to the original study.

For the analysis of time/frequency effects, we focused our analysis on the stimulation condition only for each condition. Again, we applied a wavelet approach, using similar parameters to the above re-analysis. One difference for this analysis from above is that we applied a trialwise baseline (−200 to onset) via single-trial division for each stimulation condition. We opted for this method of baseline correction, instead of the standard method of averaging spectral estimates before baseline removal, as it has been shown to reduce the sensitivity to noisy trials in time/frequency analyses88. As the number of trials in each condition was low (n = 90), we wanted to avoid contamination by noise or outliers. Significance was assessed by calculating the difference between rSMG and Mid-Occ conditions at electrode Cz via a paired-samples t-test, cluster corrected. This comparison was opted for to control for non-specific effects of stimulation that may have resulted from the coil click or scalp sensation that may come from TMS; comparison with a control site provides thus a stronger mechanistic interpretation of the observed data89.