Skip to main content

EDITORIAL article

Front. Integr. Neurosci., 08 January 2015
Volume 8 - 2014 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnint.2014.00097

Habituation mechanisms and their importance for cognitive function

  • 1Anatomy and Cell Biology, University of Western Ontario, London, ON, Canada
  • 2Emotional Brain Institute, Nathan Kline Institute for Psychiatric Research, Orangeburg, NY, USA
  • 3Department of Physiology and Neuroscience, New York University Medical Center, New York, NY, USA
  • 4Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, New York University Medical Center, New York, NY, USA
  • 5Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
  • 6Brain Research Centre, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada

How does our brain form and store memories? One way to approach this mystery is to study a very basic form of learning—habituation—in a relatively simple nervous system. Habituation describes the progressive decrease of the amplitude or frequency of a motor response to repeated sensory stimulation that is not caused by sensory receptor adaptation or motor fatigue. A multitude of different organisms, behaviors, and experimental approaches have been used to study habituation, but still surprisingly little is known about the underlying mechanisms. A theoretical framework of the concept of habituation has been laid by Thompson and Spencer (1966), and by the dual process theory of Groves and Thompson (1970), which describes habituation and sensitization as two independent processes that interact to yield the final behavioral outcome. In a symposium in 2009, the original concept was revisited and the definitions of habituation (and dishabituation) were slightly revised for clarity; however, remarkably there were only few changes to the defining characteristics (Rankin et al., 2009). It is becoming evident that behavioral habituation is caused by different mechanisms depending on time frame of stimulation, type of sensory pathway studied, and hierarchical level of signal processing. On the other hand, habituation mechanisms seem to be highly conserved, underlining the importance of habituation for the survival of a species (see Schmid et al., 2010). The scope of this Frontiers Research Topic is to give an overview over the concept of habituation, the different animal and behavioral models used for studying habituation mechanisms, as well as the different synaptic and molecular processes suggested to play a role in behavioral habituation.

Fischer et al. (2014) studied short-term habituation of the gill-withdrawal reflex in Aplysia. In accordance with the notion of different mechanisms mediating habituation in different time frames and different pathways, they report an intrinsic mechanism that is specific for short-term habituation at short training intervals of 1s. Typlt et al. (2013b) investigated the role of a voltage-and calcium activated potassium channel (BK channel) in short- and long-term habituation of an elicited behavior (acoustic startle) versus a motivated exploratory behavior using transgenic mice, and further confirm disparate, yet evolutionary highly conserved habituation mechanisms. Pilz et al. (2014) tackled a contentious issue of whether long-term habituation of acoustic startle in mice is context specific. They report that long-term habituation is stimulus-modality specific, but not context specific, confirming it as a non-associative form of learning. Dutta and Gutfreund (2014) review data from barn owls and primates on computation of saliency in the optic tectum/superior colliculus and how this is linked to habituation and neural adaptation. Perez-Gonzalez and Malmierca (2014) review different forms of spike adaptation in auditory neurons of different levels of auditory processing hierarchy. These mechanisms lead to sensory filtering and habituation of perception. Manella et al. (2013) studied how the modulatory norepinephrine system in the brain influences odor habituation and odor memory in rats.

Besides the importance of understanding the underlying mechanisms of habituation as a basic form of learning or sensory filtering, some articles go beyond understanding mechanisms of habituation and explore how its disruption impacts other cognitive domains and higher cognitive function. Typlt et al. (2013a) link habituation deficits to impairments in spatial learning. The Mini Review of De Luca (2014) sheds light on the mechanism of the habituation phenomenon of mesolimbic and mesocortical dopamine transmission in response to taste stimuli, and its putative role as a marker of cortical dysfunction in specific conditions such as addiction. Related to this topic, Lloyd et al. (2014) review the habituation of reinforcer effectiveness and the role of dopamine neurotransmission in habituation to the reinforcer. They indicate that behavioral disorders such as obesity or attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) may be caused by abnormal habituation to the reinforcer due to genetic or environmental factors.

Interestingly, studying the electrodermal orienting reflex in humans, Steiner and Barry (2014) argue against the dual-process theory's explanation that dishabituation is caused by sensitization, and instead suggest that dishabituation is a disruption of the habituation process, with its magnitude determined by the corresponding arousal level. It is certainly debatable to what extent this can be generalized to other modalities and pathways. In a theoretical essay Cevik (2014) argues that the impact of a stimulus on behavior and its potential to modulate the effects of other stimuli increase as its distance from the body decreases, an interesting and certainly also debatable concept.

In summary, this research topic contains original research articles, reviews, and theoretical essays that provide an updated view on different models for studying habituation, its underlying mechanisms, and its importance as prerequisite for higher cognitive function. The number and high quality of the papers on this topic provide support for the notion that habituation is a rich area of study, touching on a number of important questions related to behavioral plasticity.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Cevik, M. O. (2014). Habituation, sensitization, and Pavlovian conditioning. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 8:13. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2014.00013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Luca, M. A. (2014). Habituation of the responsiveness of mesolimbic and mesocortical dopamine transmission to taste stimuli. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 8:21. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2014.00021

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dutta, A., and Gutfreund, Y. (2014). Saliency mapping in the optic tectum and its relationship to habituation. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 8:1. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2014.00001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischer, T. M., Jacobson, D. A., and Demorest-Hayes, K. (2014). Network processes involved in the mediation of short-term habituation in Aplysia: contribution of intrinsic regulation of excitability and synaptic augmentation. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 8:15. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2014.00015

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Groves, P. M., and Thompson, R. F. (1970). Habituation: a dual-process theory. Psychol. Rev. 77, 419–450. doi: 10.1037/h0029810

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lloyd, D. R., Medina, D. J., Hawk, L. W., Fosco, W. D., and Richards, J. B. (2014). Habituation of reinforcer effectiveness. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 7:107. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2013.00107

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manella, L. C., Alperin, S., and Linster, C. (2013). Stressors impair odor recognition memory via an olfactory bulb-dependent noradrenergic mechanism. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 7:97. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2013.00097

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perez-Gonzalez, D., and Malmierca, M. S. (2014). Adaptation in the auditory system: an overview. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 8:19. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2014.00019

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pilz, P. K., Arnold, S. W., Rischawy, A. T., and Plappert, C. F. (2014). Longterm-habituation of the startle response in mice is stimulus modality, but not context specific. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 7:103. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2013.00103

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rankin, C. H., Abrams, T., Barry, R. J., Bhatnagar, S., Clayton, D. F., Colombo, J., et al. (2009). Habituation revisited: an updated and revised description of the behavioral characteristics of habituation. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 92, 135–138. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2008.09.012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmid, S., Brown, T., Simons-Weidenmaier, N., Weber, M., and Fendt, M. (2010). Group III metabotropic glutamate receptors inhibit startle-mediating giant neurons in the caudal pontine reticular nucleus but do not mediate synaptic depression/short-term habituation of startle. J. Neurosci. 30, 10422–10430. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0024-10.2010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steiner, G. Z., and Barry, R. J. (2014). The mechanism of dishabituation. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 8:14. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2014.00014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, R. F., and Spencer, W. A. (1966). Habituation: a model phenomenon for the study of neuronal substrates of behavior. Psychol. Rev. 73, 16–43. doi: 10.1037/h0022681

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Typlt, M., Mirkowski, M., Azzopardi, E., Ruettiger, L., Ruth, P., and Schmid, S. (2013a). Mice with deficient BK channel function show impaired prepulse inhibition and spatial learning, but normal working and spatial reference memory. PLoS ONE 8:e81270. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0081270

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Typlt, M., Mirkowski, M., Azzopardi, E., Ruth, P., Pilz, P. K., and Schmid, S. (2013b). Habituation of reflexive and motivated behavior in mice with deficient BK channel function. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 7:79. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2013.00079

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: habituation, sensorimotor gating, sensory filtering, synaptic mechanisms, behavioral plasticity, learning and memory

Citation: Schmid S, Wilson DA and Rankin CH (2015) Habituation mechanisms and their importance for cognitive function. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 8:97. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2014.00097

Received: 02 December 2014; Accepted: 16 December 2014;
Published online: 08 January 2015.

Edited by:

Sidney A. Simon, Duke University, USA

Reviewed by:

Adriano B. L. Tort, Federal University of Rio Grande do Norte, Brazil

Copyright © 2015 Schmid, Wilson and Rankin. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: susanne.schmid@schulich.uwo.ca

Download