Pain is a potent stressor with immediate relevance to survival. Central regulatory circuits help orchestrate the body’s adaptive response to painful stressors, optimizing survival during threat and restoring homeostasis in its aftermath. Acute physical stressors activate the amygdala, an emotional processing center that signals the saliency of incoming sensory information (Anderson & Phelps, 2001; LeDoux, 2000) and triggers affective or emotional responses (LeDoux, 2003). As a result, the amygdala functions as part of an early alarm system that rapidly detects threat and coordinates defensive behaviors (Liddell et al., 2005). The amygdala also bridges stress and pain regulatory systems, suggesting that it plays an important role in balancing the response demands that painful stressors place on the brain.

While heightened amygdala activity helps mobilize the body’s response to threat, prolonged salience signals become unnecessary when pain is no longer relevant to behavior. To restore homeostasis, amygdala activity is instead reduced during the period after painful stimulation. Initially, the amygdala promotes activation of the hypothalamo–pituitary–adrenocortical (HPA) axis, which culminates in the release of the stress hormone cortisol from the adrenal medulla (Herman, Ostrander, Mueller, & Figueiredo, 2005). While this hormone plays an essential role in the stress response by stimulating glucose production (de Kloet, Oitzl, & Joëls, 1999), excessively high levels of cortisol can also produce the breakdown of muscle, bone, and neural tissue (Sapolsky, 1996). To limit the duration and magnitude of HPA activation, cortisol regulates its own release via a negative feedback loop with the hypothalamus (de Kloet & Reul, 1987) and target limbic structures, such as the medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC; Herman et al., 2005). The MPFC regulates the stress response by inhibiting the HPA axis (Boyle et al., 2005; Diorio, Viau, & Meaney, 1993). The amygdala also receives inhibitory inputs from MPFC, and this top-down regulation appears to be partially mediated by endogenous cortisol (Urry et al., 2006; Veer et al., 2011a). A positron-emission tomography (PET) study in humans supports this MPFC–amygdala cortisol feedback mechanism, demonstrating that greater stress-related MPFC metabolic activity anticorrelates with cortisol increase and is associated with decreased activity in the amygdala (Kern et al., 2008). Hydrocortisone has been shown to reduce amygdala activity 20 min after pharmacological administration (Lovallo, Robinson, Glahn, & Fox, 2010), which is consistent with the time when cortisol reaches peak concentration after acute stress induction (Dickerson & Kemeny, 2004). These results suggest that delayed cortisol feedback to the MPFC helps disable amygdala-driven HPA activation during latter stages of the stress response.

Importantly, the amygdala not only modulates HPA axis output but represents a major relay station for both afferent and efferent nociceptive information processing (Bernard & Besson, 1988). For instance, the degree of activation in the amygdala has been associated with subjective pain ratings (Bornhovd et al., 2002). In addition, successful pain regulation has been shown to alter activity in the amygdala (Lapate et al., 2012). Stressor-related amygdala deactivation likely depends on frontal cortical regions, which shape nociceptive processing through both direct and indirect influences on descending inhibitory pain circuits (see Hutchinson et al., 1999; Lenz et al., 1998; Xie, Huo, & Tang, 2009, for reviews). In particular, the engagement of dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) and lateral orbitofrontal cortex (LOFC) exerts an inhibitory influence on amygdala responsivity, especially during pain modulation and emotion regulation (Johnstone, van Reekum, Urry, Kalin, & Davidson, 2007; Lee, Heller, van Reekum, Nelson, & Davidson, 2012; Ochsner et al., 2004; Petrovic & Ingvar, 2002). A positron-emission tomography (PET) study has supported the notion of frontal control, demonstrating that longer pain exposure, wherein participants reported using more coping strategies, was associated with greater activity in the dACC and reduced activity in the amygdala (Petrovic, Carlsson, Petersson, Hansson, & Ingvar, 2004). Thus, the efficacy of fronto-amygdala functional interactions likely contributes to recovery from physical stressors, though this possibility has received little attention in humans.

To date, most stress studies have focused on activity changes in the amygdala itself, as opposed to changes in its interactions with other brain regions. Furthermore, previous observations of top-down modulation during the stress response have been limited to simple correlations between mean activity in the amygdala and a given frontal regulatory region (Kern et al., 2008). The few studies that have examined stress-related changes in amygdala functional connectivity are consistent with the notion of time-dependent shifts that reflect underlying response and recovery demands. For example, immediately after viewing aversive film clips, the amygdala shows enhanced functional connectivity with regions that amplify the stress response and aversive processing, such as dACC and locus coeruleus (van Marle, Hermans, Qin, & Fernández, 2010). In men, corticosteroid administration reduces amygdala functional connectivity with these brain regions approximately 2 h after oral intake (Henckens, van Wingen, Joëls, & Fernández, 2012). A separate study using acute psychological stress demonstrated that amygdala activity covaries positively with activity in the MPFC 1 h later, supporting the notion of homeostatic recovery once the stressor is no longer salient (Veer et al., 2011b). We aimed to expand upon these findings by determining how an acute painful stressor—the cold pressor task—affects subsequent amygdala functional connectivity.

During the cold pressor task, participants are asked to submerge their nondominant hand in ice water for several minutes. This painful task involves a combination of acute physical and psychological stress (Blandini, Martignoni, Sances, Bono, & Nappi, 1995) and has been shown to increase salivary cortisol, at least outside the scanner environment (Buchanan, Tranel, & Adolphs, 2006; Lighthall, Mather, & Gorlick, 2009; Lighthall et al., 2011; Lighthall, Gorlick, Schoeke, Frank, & Mather, 2013; Mather, Gorlick, & Lighthall, 2009; Pascualy et al., 2000; Porcelli et al., 2008). However, negative findings have also been reported, with some studies demonstrating either a moderate increase in cortisol responsivity (al’Absi, Hugdahl, & Lovallo, 2002) or no increase at all (Duncko, Cornwell, Cui, Merikangas, & Grillon, 2007; Duncko, Johnson, Merikangas, & Grillon, 2009; McRae et al., 2006). This discrepancy could be attributed to the cold pressor task engaging both pain- and stress-processing systems. Numerous pain studies have demonstrated that exposure to stress not only triggers the release of cortisol, but a host of stress-related neurotransmitters, including endogenous opioids (Drolet et al., 2001). Men, in particular, demonstrate greater μ-opioid receptor activation in the amygdala during sustained pain than do women (Zubieta et al., 2002), suggesting that males engage analgesic mechanisms to a greater extent after painful stressors. Since common frontal regions regulate amygdala responses to stress, pain, and emotion, it is possible that cortisol and pain modulation have separable contributions to amygdala-mediated stress adaptation. Thus, the goal of the present study was to distinguish how HPA axis responsivity and self-reported pain relate to amygdala functional connectivity during poststressor recovery in men.

Pulsed arterial spin labeling (PASL) serves as an optimal method for examining brain functional connectivity during sustained physiological states, such as stress. Unlike radiotracers used in PET imaging, PASL is noninvasive and uses magnetic inversion to “tag” arterial blood water so that it can be used as an endogenous tracer of cerebral blood flow (CBF). PASL also holds several advantages over more conventional resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging techniques, because it is not as susceptible to image artifacts that typically contaminate the blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) signal. Some of these benefits include improved intersession reliability (Aguirre, Detre, Zarahn, & Alsop, 2002; Wang et al., 2003), lower sensitivity to magnetic field inhomogeneity effects (Wang et al., 2004), and less sensitivity to participant motion and scanner drift artifacts (Aguirre et al., 2002). In addition, PASL targets CBF in arterioles and capillaries as opposed to venules, thereby acquiring signal that is better localized to the sites of neural activity than the BOLD signal (Zappe, Pfeuffer, Merkle, Logothetis, & Goense, 2008).

Here, we used PASL and seed-based resting-state functional connectivity (rsFC) to observe coherent perfusion signal fluctuations between the amygdala and other brain regions approximately 15–30 min after a cold pressor task. A group of 17 healthy male participants were scanned during two resting-state PASL sessions to acquire a within-subjects measure of stressor-related changes in amygdala rsFC. Only males were recruited for this study based on evidence that they show stronger analgesic mechanisms after painful stimulation than do females (Zubieta et al., 2002) Furthermore, men were selected to avoid confounding interactions between estradiol and cortisol responsivity (Kudielka & Kirschbaum, 2005) and cold pressor pain threshold and menstrual cycle phase (Aloisi & Bonifazi, 2006; Hapidou & De Catanzaro, 1988; Hellström & Lundberg, 2000). We hypothesized that the cold pressor task would significantly alter resting-state functional coupling between the amygdala and regions that contribute to HPA axis regulation and pain perception. Moreover, we predicted that distinct patterns of amygdala rsFC would be associated with individual differences in stressor-induced cortisol elevation and self-reported pain intensity, thereby elucidating how overlapping stress and pain regulatory networks interact with the amygdala to restore homeostasis.

Method

Participants

A group of 17 healthy, right-handed males (age range = 18–31 years, mean = 23.5) participated in the two-day study. On each day, participants were scanned between 2 and 5 p.m., when cortisol levels are relatively stable, in order to limit intersubject variability. The participants also refrained from eating, caffeine intake, and exercise for at least 1 h, and from sleeping for at least 2 h prior to arrival. All participants provided written informed consent approved by the University of Southern California (USC) Institutional Review Board.

Procedure

Upon arrival, participants gave informed consent and drank 8 oz of water. The first 45 min of the study involved completing various questionnaires to assess baseline mood and subjective self-reports of overall and relative stress levels. This portion of the experiment also served as an acclimation period before the stressor. After finishing the assessments, participants were asked to provide a 1-mL baseline saliva sample by passively drooling into a test tube. In this within-subjects design, participants underwent scan sessions on two separate days. The participants were randomly assigned to the cold pressor task or the control condition on the first day and were run in the other condition during the second session. Participants were not informed of the water temperature condition beforehand, and the experimenter emphasized that the ice water (stressor condition) could be administered on one or both days of the experimental procedure. During the cold pressor task, participants were instructed to immerse their left, nondominant hand in ice water (0°–3° Celsius) for at least 1 min and up to 3 min. For the control condition, participants immersed their left hand in warm water (37°–40° Celsius) for up to 3 min. One experimenter remained in the room to watch the participant during the hand immersion manipulation. Previous research had suggested that combining social–evaluative threat with the cold pressor task elicits greater HPA activation than does the cold pressor task alone (Schwabe, Haddad, & Schachinger, 2008). Following the hand immersion task, participants were given additional instruction about the scanning procedure and then entered the scanner. Approximately 15–30 min after the onset of the hand immersion task (M = 18 min, SD = 4; minimum = 10 min, maximum = 30 min), a brief localizer scan was conducted to ensure that the participant’s head was centered for the PASL resting-state scan. Participants were instructed to lay still with their eyes open during the 6-min 20-s resting-state sequence, but not to fall asleep. Immediately following the scan, participants were instructed to remain still while a second saliva sample was collected using a Sorbette, which they carefully placed in their mouth for 2 min (Salimetrics, LLC, State College, PA, USA). After performing an unrelated fMRI task for approximately 25 min, a high-resolution T1-weighted anatomical scan was acquired.

Data collection

Salivary cortisol collection and assay

Salivary cortisol collection times were based on previous findings that stressor-related cortisol levels peak approximately 21–40 min following the onset of an acute stressor (Dickerson & Kemeny, 2004). Since Saliva Sample 3 was acquired during the subsequent, unrelated fMRI task (M = 59 min, SD = 6; minimum = 49 min, maximum = 78 min), only the difference from baseline (Sample 1) to the post-PASL scan (Sample 2) was used as a measure of cortisol change in the subsequent analyses. Following the PASL scan, saliva samples were stored in a laboratory freezer at –30° Celsius. Once the study was completed, the frozen samples were sent to analytical laboratories (Salimetrics, LLC, State College, PA, USA) and stored at –80° Celsius until assayed. The samples were centrifuged at 3,000 rpm for 15 min to remove mucins. Duplicate assays were conducted for each saliva sample, and the mean of the two values was used in the final analysis. The intra-assay coefficient of variation (CV) between duplicates was determined to be 4.85 %. Three participants were excluded from the cortisol analyses, because the quantities of their saliva samples were not sufficient to yield an accurate measure of cortisol concentration. Specifically, cortisol concentrations were unable to be determined for both the baseline (before hand immersion) and post-hand-immersion samples in one participant, whereas two participants were missing concentration values for just the post-hand-immersion saliva samples; since we required both samples in order to assess stressor-related cortisol change, both of the latter participants had to be excluded from further analyses involving cortisol.

Pain and stress ratings

Pain intensity during the cold pressor task was assessed via participants’ self-reports immediately before and after hand immersion. The degree of pain was measured using two pain scales: the Faces Pain Scale–Revised (FPS-R) and a pain visual-analogue scale (VAS). For the FPS-R, participants made their ratings by circling the face that best reflected how much pain they experienced during hand immersion in ice or warm water. Similarly, participants also indicated their pain level on the pain VAS via a hash mark on a line, which ranged from no pain on the left to worst possible pain on the right. These responses were codified by measuring the distance of the mark from no pain at 0 cm to worst possible pain at 10.16 cm. Subjective mood was assessed prior to hand immersion using the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; Watson, Clark, & Tellegan, 1988). Participants also reported their overall and relative stress levels on a scale from 1 to 10 prior to hand immersion, with 1 indicating no stress and 10 indicating extremely stressed.

Image acquisition

Perfusion images were acquired on a 3-T Siemens MAGNETOM TRIO scanner using a 12-channel head coil. Resting-state arterial spin labeling was conducted with a PASL sequence using the QUIPSS-II method (Wong, Buxton, & Frank, 1997, 1998). In addition, the “proximal inversion with a control for off-resonance effects” (PICORE) mode was employed in order to provide high labeling efficiency (Luh, Wong, Bandettini, & Hyde, 1999). A total of 88 interleaved label and control volumes were acquired during a 6-min 20-s scan, along with one M0 image (axial slices = 20, TR/TE = 4,000/30 ms, TI1/TIs/TI2 = 700/1,600/1,800 ms, matrix = 64 × 64, FOV = 224 mm, flip angle = 90°, in-plane resolution = 3 × 3 mm2, slice thickness = 4 mm). The timing of the inversion pulses (TI) was optimized to reduce intravascular signal intensity at 3 T (Donahue, Lu, Jones, Pekar, & van Zijl, 2006). The scanner’s built-in 3-D online prospective acquisition correction (PACE) was used to correct for head motion artifacts during the PASL scan. A high-resolution T1-weighted anatomical image (MPRAGE) was also obtained to aid with PASL image co-registration (slices = 208 coronal; TR/TE/TI = 2,530/3.09/800 ms; FOV = 256 × 256 mm; in-plane resolution = 1 mm2; slice thickness = 1 mm with no gap; bandwidth = 220 Hz/Px). The total acquisition time for the anatomical scan was 10 min 42 s.

Statistical analysis

Cortisol and pain measures

Cortisol data were analyzed using a 2 (condition) × 2 (saliva sample time) repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA), with Condition Order (stressor condition on Day 1 or 2) as a between-subjects factor. Similarly, hand immersion duration and the subjective pain measures from the pain VAS and Faces Pain Scale were analyzed using separate 2 (condition) × 2 (rating time) repeated measures ANOVAs with Condition Order as a between-subjects factor. Post hoc t tests were performed to assess significant main effects of hand immersion condition and saliva sample or pain rating time. Paired t tests were used to ensure that the baseline positive and negative affect (PANAS questionnaire) and overall and relative stress levels did not differ between conditions. A paired t test was also used to verify that the mean time after the first and second salivary cortisol sample was comparable between the stressor and control conditions. Another paired t test was used to test whether the mean times between the onset of hand immersion and the start of the PASL scan were comparable between the stressor and control conditions.

Associations between pain and cortisol

Pearson’s correlations were performed between the stressor-induced changes in cortisol and pain measures (cold pressor duration, pain VAS, and Faces Pain Scale). The duration of hand immersion in the ice water varied across participants, so partial correlations were also performed between the pain and cortisol outcome measures after controlling for cold pressor duration. Since we identified a significant main effect of condition order on salivary cortisol levels, an additional partial correlation analysis was performed by controlling for condition order. Notably, only 14 participants had complete cortisol data. To take advantage of greater statistical power, the pain measures from the group of 17 participants were analyzed and reported. The strength of these associations was further validated by reanalyzing the pain rating data for the subgroup of 14 participants.

PASL analysis

Image preprocessing

PASL images were preprocessed using FSL Version 4.1.6 (FMRIB’s Software Library, www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl). The first volume (M0), which is used to normalize the quantification of perfusion maps, was discarded. The remaining 88 interleaved control and tag images were preprocessed using the following steps: motion correction, removal of nonbrain tissue, spatial smoothing using a Gaussian kernel of 6-mm full-width-at-half-maximum (FWHM), grand-mean intensity normalization of the entire 4-D data set by a single multiplicative factor, and a high-pass temporal filter of 100 s (i.e., 0.01 Hz). PASL volumes were registered to the individual participant’s T1-weighted high-resolution anatomical volume using an affine registration with 12 degrees of freedom. The T1-weighted image was then linearly registered to the 2-mm isotropic MNI-152 standard-space image (T1-weighted standard brain averaged over 152 subjects; Montreal Neurological Institute, Montreal, QC, Canada). The resulting transformation matrices were then concatenated to create a functional-to-standard-space transformation matrix, which was used to write the perfusion volumes into standard space.

Physiological and motion noise removal

A single-session independent component analysis (ICA) was performed to denoise the 4-D PASL data of participant motion and physiological artifacts, such as cardiac and respiratory pulsation. (Beckmann, DeLuca, Devlin, & Smith, 2005). Components were classified as noise if they met one or more of the following criteria: if the signal was predominately located in white matter or ventricles; if the component’s power spectra showed erratic fluctuations; if there were enclosed rings of activation around the edges or superior slices of the brain; or if spikes in the principal eigenvector time series exceeded six scale units, which is usually indicative of head motion. The components that were identified as noise were then filtered from each participant’s preprocessed PASL data.

To further reduce the influence of artifacts, mean time series from white matter (WM) and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) were modeled as nuisance regressors in the subsequent general linear model (GLM). This noise removal technique relies on the assumption that CSF and WM carry physiological fluctuations with little contribution from neural activity (Fox et al., 2005). WM and CSF masks were produced by segmenting each individual’s T1-weighted structural image into tissue-type partial volume masks using FMRIB’s Automated Segmentation Tool (FAST). The CSF and WM masks were then co-registered to functional space resolution (2 mm3) by applying the inverse transformation matrix produced by registering each participant’s T1 image to their mean perfusion volume. The resampled tissue masks were thresholded at 90 % partial volume fraction and binarized in order to minimize overlap with gray matter (GM). Finally, the resultant CSF and WM tissue masks were applied to each individual’s preprocessed perfusion data, and a mean time course was extracted by averaging the time series across all voxels within each mask.

Gray-matter template

A study-specific GM mask was also created in order to further exclude non-gray-matter contributions to the perfusion signal. To produce the GM template, each participant’s GM partial volume image was affine-registered to the MNI-152 standard-space brain. The registered images were then merged into a single group 4-D file, averaged. This group-averaged GM image was spatially smoothed with an 8-mm FWHM Gaussian kernel, thresholded at 30 % tissue-type probability, and binarized to constrain subsequent analyses to GM regions.

Amygdala seed-based correlation analysis

A seed-based correlation analysis was used to assess the effects of the cold pressor task on subsequent amygdala rsFC. Left and right amygdala seeds were defined using the Harvard–Oxford Atlas in FSL. To minimize overlap between the amygdala and neighboring brain regions, these seed masks were thresholded at 50 % amygdala probability. For each participant, a lower-level, fixed-effects GLM analysis was used to identify statistically significant amygdala rsFC patterns within each session. A variation of FSL’s full perfusion signal modeling was used in order to extract perfusion-only signal from control-tag image pairs (Mumford, Hernandez-Garcia, Lee, & Nichols, 2006). In this modeling technique, the control/label effect is built into the model implicitly. Specifically, the alternating control and label images are modeled as the “ON” and “OFF” periods of a square waveform, respectively, with each period equal to the duration of a single TR (4 s). Full perfusion signal modeling was chosen, because it provides optimal power for the analysis of arterial spin labeling data and has been shown to be more sensitive than the standard approach of pairwise differencing (Mumford et al., 2006). A second regressor was used to control for the effects of the slow BOLD signal, which contaminates perfusion signal. The BOLD nuisance signal, modeled as the mean of the tag and control volumes, was convolved with the square perfusion waveform using a standard hemodynamic response function (HRF) convolution.

A seed physiological regressor was created by extracting the mean time series of activity across all of the voxels in both left and right amygdala. The mean left and right amygdala time series were used as temporal predictors in separate whole-brain multiple linear regressions. Additionally, WM and CSF nuisance variables were modeled in the GLM and regressed from the data. Amygdala rsFC maps were produced for each participant/session, such that significant clusters represented brain voxels that contained synchronous perfusion signal fluctuations with the amygdala seed regions. Significant patterns of left and right amygdala rsFC were determined using cluster correction and a z threshold of 2.3 (p = .05). The resulting contrast images from the session-level analysis were then processed in a higher-level random-effects group analysis in order to account for intersubject variability. The study-specific GM group template was also applied in order to limit the testing to GM-only voxels. To identify amygdala rsFC differences between the stressor and control conditions, a paired t test was conducted on pairs of contrast images from each participant’s two scanning sessions. These spatial maps were also thresholded at a cluster size of z > 2.3 and a whole-brain significance of p < .05. To examine whole-brain patterns of positive and negative left and right amygdala rsFC, group averages were also calculated using one-sample t tests. Four separate whole-brain statistical parametric maps were produced in order to identify brain regions that were either positively or negatively correlated with perfusion signal fluctuations in the left and right amygdala seed regions, separately.

Amygdala connectivity-defined ROI analysis

An ROI analysis was conducted to determine the direction of functional connectivity between the right/left amygdala and significant clusters identified in the group-level seed-based correlation analysis. Significant mean group differences were only observed in the control–stressor contrast, so these ROIs represent brain regions that were functionally decoupled from the amygdala after the cold pressor task. The spatial location and extent of stressor-induced amygdala rsFC changes were highly consistent between the group of 14 participants with cortisol data and the full group of 17 participants (see Supplementary Fig. 1). Thus, the amygdala functional connectivity data from the full group of 17 participants were used in subsequent analyses.

Several of the significant control - stressor clusters were widely dispersed and covered brain regions with known functional heterogeneity. Therefore, smaller ROIs were defined as 8-mm-diameter spheres in MNI space, in which the peak coordinates corresponded to the four peak voxels from the control–stressor left and right amygdala rsFC contrasts. The four ROI spheres were centered about left dACC (−8, 26, 34) and right lateral OFC (22, 28, –24), for the left amygdala seed region, and left mOFC (−8, 34, –28) and right posterior cingulate cortex (16, –20, 44), for the right amygdala seed region. Significant clusters from the left and right amygdala control–stressor contrast maps were thresholded at z > 3 and intersected with their respective sphere ROIs in order to increase spatial specificity. The resulting masks were visually inspected to ensure that no overlap took place between any of the ROIs. The ROI masks were then binarized and applied to each session/participant’s lower-level amygdala rsFC maps in order to extract mean z scores. These z scores represent the strength of rsFC between the amygdala and the target brain regions; positive z scores would signify a positive correlation between the ROI and amygdala timeseries, whereas negative z scores would refer to a negative correlation between the ROI and the amygdala timeseries. In addition, we performed two-tailed single-sample t tests against zero to investigate whether functional coupling between the amygdala and the target ROI regions was significantly positive or negative across the group within the stressor and control sessions.

Associations between stressor-altered amygdala functional connectivity, pain, and cortisol outcomes

To determine whether the four stressor-related amygdala rsFC ROIs were associated with cortisol change or subjective pain measures, we correlated the mean connectivity z scores for each ROI with the following behavioral variables: (1) pain VAS rating, (2) Faces Pain Scale rating, (3) cold pressor hand immersion duration, and (4) cortisol change. In addition, partial correlation analyses were performed by controlling for cold pressor duration and condition order separately.

As a test of our main hypotheses, we performed a t test to test for any significant differences between the two dependent correlations—that is, pain and amygdala rsFC as well as cortisol and amygdala rsFC—with 11 (n – 3) degrees of freedom (Blalock, 1972). Only the 14 participants with complete pain and cortisol data were used, in order to allow for a direct comparison between the stressor-induced amygdala rsFC, cortisol responsivity, and pain measures.

Results

Cortisol

The results for the cortisol analyses are displayed in Fig. 1. Salivary cortisol concentrations were not significantly affected by the cold pressor task, as was indicated by the lack of significant main effects of condition, F(1, 12) = 1.0, p = .26, and time of the saliva sample, F(1, 12) = 0.9, p = .36, or of a Condition × Time interaction, F(1, 12) = 0.20, p = .66. Interestingly, we did find a significant main effect of condition order on salivary cortisol, F(1, 12) = 14.71, p = .002, indicating that cortisol levels were generally higher in the control-condition-first participants (see Fig. 1). We found no Condition × Order, F(1, 12) = 0.90, p = .36, or Condition × Time × Order, F(1, 12) = 0.55, p = .47, effects on cortisol concentration.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Comparisons of salivary cortisol between the stressor and control conditions pre-hand-immersion (Sample 1) and post-hand-immersion (Sample 2; measured after the pulsed arterial spin labeling scan). To illustrate the main effect of condition order on cortisol level, the measurements for participants who performed the cold pressor task on Day 1—that is, stressor condition first—are presented separately from participants who performed the cold pressor task on Day 2—that is, control condition first

Follow-up t tests revealed a significant difference between the stressor-condition-first and control-condition-first groups only in the poststressor cortisol sample (p = .007). Prestressor (baseline in the stress condition) cortisol samples did not differ significantly between these two groups (p = .40). Thus, elevated cortisol levels in the control-condition-first participants appeared to be driven by differences in the poststressor sample, as opposed to the prestressor sample. This makes it unlikely that anticipation of the stressor on Day 2 can account for the main effect of order observed in the ANOVA.

Separate follow-up repeated measures ANOVAs were conducted to determine the effects of time and condition on cortisol levels for the stressor-condition-first and control-condition-first groups. No significant time, condition, or Time × Condition interaction effects on salivary cortisol levels emerged in either of these two groups (ps > .1). Thus, regardless of condition order, no significant effects of stressor condition were seen on cortisol levels.

The amount of time between stressor onset and the beginning of the PASL scan did not significantly affect cortisol concentration, F(1, 15) = 1.77, p = .20, thereby verifying that variability in the measurement window could not account for differences in salivary cortisol concentrations. The mean times between the two saliva samples also did not differ between the stressor and control conditions (p = .79).

Subjective pain and stress

The results for the subjective pain measures are displayed in Fig. 2. Though a reliable elevation in stressor-induced cortisol level was not observed, the cold pressor task did elicit a statistically significant increase between pre- and poststressor ratings on the pain VAS, F(1, 15) = 61.01, p < .001, and the Faces Pain Scale, F(1, 15) = 73.30, p < .001. Subjective pain intensity was also highly significant when we analyzed both pain measures for the group of 14 participants with cortisol data (ps < .001). The condition order also marginally affected Faces Pain Scale ratings, with the mean pain ratings being higher for control-condition-first participants, F(1, 15) = 4.2, p = .058. This condition order effect was not observed for the pain VAS, F(1, 15) = 1.29, p < .27. The duration of hand immersion was significantly shorter for the ice water (M = 114 ± 14 s) than for the warm water (M = 180 ± 0 s, p < .001) and did not differ by condition order (p = .39). Paired t tests verified that participants’ baseline self-reports of overall stress level, relative stress level, and mood did not differ between conditions (ps > .05).

Fig. 2
figure 2

(A) Comparisons of mean self-reported pain on the pain visual-analogue scale between the stressor and control conditions. The means represent the post–pre hand immersion ratings, which were based on the distance of the hash mark on the line spectrum in centimeters. (B) Comparison of mean self-reported pain on the Faces Pain Scale between the stressor and control conditions. The means represent the post–pre hand immersion ratings, thereby indicating the effects of water temperature on perceived pain. Error bars represent standard errors of the means (SEMs)

Correlations between pain and cortisol outcomes

Results of the Pearson bivariate and partial correlations between stressor-related cortisol and pain ratings are presented in Table 1. As is indicated in the table, all correlations that were significant for the full sample were also significant for the subsample of 14 participants who had complete cortisol data. The stressor-related (stressor–control) changes in self-reported pain intensity were highly consistent between the two pain measures (r = .76, p < .001). Increased stressor-related pain VAS ratings were negatively associated with cold pressor duration time (r = −.57, p = .018), indicating that participants who were better able to endure the cold pressor task, or to maintain ice water hand immersion, were also more likely to report experiencing less pain. While this association between subjective pain intensity and cold pressor duration was only marginally significant for the Faces Pain Scale with the full sample (r = −.42, p = .094), it was significant for the 14 participants who had complete cortisol data (r = −.56, p = .036).

Table 1 Associations between stressor-related changes in pain and cortisol outcome measures

A marginally significant negative association was found between stressor-related cortisol change and pain VAS ratings (r = −.41, p = .14). Interestingly, this trend was almost significant after controlling for condition order (r = −.53, p = .064). However, the association between cortisol change and stressor-related Faces Pain Scale rating change was weaker (ps > .05).

Amygdala seed-based correlation and ROI analysis

The group random-effects rsFC analysis revealed distributed amygdala functional networks across the brain (Fig. 3). These whole-brain patterns of amygdala rsFC are consistent with previous studies (Robinson, Laird, Glahn, Lovallo, & Fox, 2010; Roy et al., 2009) that have described a dorsal–ventral dissociation between two amygdala-based circuits: positive correlations with regions implicated in emotion generation and regulation, such as MPFC and thalamus, versus negative correlations with regions involved in cognitive processes, such as middle frontal gyrus and precuneus. We also observed amygdala rsFC differences along the medial–lateral axis, such that the amygdala was positively correlated with medial brain regions and negatively correlated with more lateral cortical regions (Table 2).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Group average (A) left and (B) right amygdala resting-state functional connectivity (N = 17). The patterns of activation reflect gray matter regions where dynamic perfusion signal changes were significantly either positively correlated (red) or negatively correlated (blue) with perfusion signal changes in the amygdala seed regions. The amygdala rsFC spatial maps were collapsed across the stressor and control sessions, and whole-brain statistical parametric maps were calculated using single-sample t tests. Significant clusters were corrected for multiple comparisons using a cluster threshold of z > 2.3, p = .05

The results of the paired t test comparison between the stressor and control conditions are presented in Fig. 4 (Table 3). We observed greater left amygdala positive rsFC in the control versus stressor condition with the right LOFC, left ventromedial PFC (VMPFC), right frontal pole, right VMPFC, and a distributed cluster spanning the left dACC, left supplementary motor area (SMA), and left superior frontal gyrus (SFG). The stressor–control contrast yielded no significant connectivity differences. Extraction of the mean z scores from the dACC cluster revealed that this region was significantly positively coupled with the left amygdala during the control condition (p = .047). In contrast, this dACC cluster was negatively correlated with left amygdala activity during the stressor condition (p = .0016). In addition, we found significant positive functional coupling between the left amygdala and right OFC during the control condition (p < .001). The inverse left amygdala functional coupling with right OFC during the stressor condition was not significantly different from zero (p > .05).

Fig. 4
figure 4

(A) Left amygdala and (B) right amygdala resting-state functional connectivity clusters from the group-level random-effects analysis (control – stressor) contrast, cluster-corrected at z > 2.3, p = .05. Clusters are thresholded at z > 3 for greater spatial specificity. The mean connectivity z scores were extracted for each of the target brain regions from each participant/session’s lower-level stressor and control session spatial maps. Two-tailed, single t tests were performed against zero to determine whether amygdala coupling with each connectivity-defined target region was significantly positive or negative for the stressor or control condition. Overall, the cold pressor task led to significant reductions in functional connectivity between the amygdalae and prefrontal/cingulate cortex. The stressor also induced a significant negative association between perfusion signal fluctuations in the left amygdala and right dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC). Error bars represent standard errors of the means (SEMs). LOFC = lateral orbitofrontal cortex; VMPFC = ventromedial prefrontal cortex; PCC = posterior cingulate cortex. * p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .005

As compared with the stressor condition, the control condition showed greater positive right amygdala rsFC with left paracentral lobule/posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), right SMA, left VMPFC/OFC, right MPFC, left MPFC, left frontal pole, and right subcallosal cortex. No significant connectivity differences emerged in the opposite contrast (stressor–control). The right amygdala was significantly positively coupled with both the PCC and left VMPFC during the control session (ps = .0054 and .0034, respectively). During the stressor condition, inverse functional connectivity between the right amygdala and right PCC ROIs was marginally significant (p = .06), but right amygdala coupling with the left VMPFC was not significant (p > .05). Thus, the cold pressor task functionally decoupled the right amygdala from these target brain regions. We also found three small regions in VMPFC/OFC (31 voxels total) where the stressor-related left and right amygdala functional decouplings overlapped.

Relationship between stressor-altered amygdala functional connectivity, pain, and cortisol

We performed Pearson bivariate and partial correlations to determine the functional significance of the cortisol and pain outcomes on stressor-induced amygdala decoupling. The results of these correlations are presented in Table 4. We found a significant positive association between stressor-related pain VAS ratings and functional coupling between the left amygdala and right LOFC (r = .5, p = .042). This indicates that participants who reported experiencing greater pain during the cold pressor also exhibited enhanced amygdala–LOFC rsFC approximately 15–30 min later. The strength of this association increased after controlling for cold pressor duration time (r = .71, p = .002; Fig. 5). A significant positive association between stressor-induced changes in Faces Pain Scale ratings and left amygdala–LOFC rsFC also emerged after controlling for cold pressor duration (r = .54, p = .03). The relationship between pain VAS and left amygdala–LOFC rsFC was still significant for the subgroup of 14 participants with cortisol data (r = .59, p = .027), including after controlling for cold pressor duration (r = .61, p = .028).

Table 2 List of significant clusters from the group-level left and right amygdala seed-based resting-state functional connectivity analysis
Table 3 List of significant clusters from the random-effects group analysis showing stressor-induced left and right amygdala resting-state functional connectivity (rsFC) changes with other brain regions
Table 4 Associations between stressor-related changes in amygdala resting-state functional connectivity regions of interest, pain measures, and cortisol
Fig. 5
figure 5

Individuals who reported lower pain intensity during the cold pressor task also demonstrated greater inverse resting-state functional connectivity (rsFC) between the left amygdala and right lateral orbitofrontal cortex (LOFC). The blue cluster represents a functionally defined region of interest (ROI) identified by the left amygdala seed-based correlation analysis. The mean connectivity z scores were extracted for this ROI from each participant/session’s lower-level stressor and control left amygdala rsFC maps. The scatterplot depicts the association between stressor-induced amygdala–LOFC functional coupling (stressor – control; y-axis) and pain visual-analogue scale residuals (stressor – control values; x-axis) after controlling for cold pressor duration time

After controlling for condition order, stressor-induced changes in pain VAS ratings and left amygdala–LOFC rsFC became significant in the group of 14 participants (r = .59, p = .033), but not in the full sample (r = .44, p = .09). We did not observe any significant associations between cortisol or subjective pain intensity and the left amygdala–dACC, right amygdala–VMPFC, or right amygdala–PCC rsFC ROIs.

Though stressor-related left amygdala–LOFC rsFC was not significantly related to cold-pressor-induced cortisol increases (r = −.21, p = .49), this trend was in the opposite direction from that with the pain VAS. A t test revealed a marginally significant difference between the left amygdala–LOFC pathway’s associations with cortisol versus with pain intensity (t = 1.93, p = .08), suggesting that stressor-induced amygdala–LOFC rsFC was more closely related to previously experienced pain than to cortisol responsivity.

Discussion

Using pulsed arterial spin labeling, we found that an acute painful stressor alters amygdala functional connectivity with frontal and ACC regions 15–30 min after stressor onset. This study highlights the utility of PASL in detecting activation differences in brain regions that are vulnerable to signal loss, such as the OFC (Wang et al., 2004). Our functional connectivity results extend earlier findings that higher-order cortical regions modulate amygdala activity following an aversive experience.

Our first main finding was that exposure to the cold pressor reduced subsequent rsFC between the amygdalae and large regions spanning the VMPFC and OFC. This stressor-induced shift in amygdala rsFC is supported by evidence of dense reciprocal anatomical connections between the amygdala and the VMPFC/OFC (Amaral & Price, 1984; Ghashghaei & Barbas, 2002). Moreover, tracing studies in rodents have uncovered anatomical pathways linking the left and right amygdalae to contralateral prefrontal cortex (Cavada, Tejedor, Cruz-Rizzolo, & Reinoso-Suárez, 2000; McDonald, Mascagni, & Guo, 1996; Vertes, 2004). The posterior region of OFC receives and integrates polymodal sensory inputs, which enables encoding of the aversive value of a threatening stimulus through connections with the amygdala (Kringelbach & Rolls, 2004). Evidence from human lesion studies has corroborated this role of the OFC, showing that lesions in this region lead to deficits in the ability to label sensory events as either pleasant or unpleasant (Rule, Shimamura, & Knight, 2002). In the wake of physical insult, this disruption in amygdalo-frontal functional coupling could reflect attenuated processing of the aversive stressor. This aligns with evidence that prefrontal lobotomy patients show decreased spontaneous concern or rumination about their pain (Hardy, Wolff, & Goodell, 1952). In the period after the stressor is removed, response priorities shift away from threat-related processing. As a result, decreased functional connectivity in this pathway could be a mechanism for reducing any unpleasantness associated with acute pain, thereby enabling the reinstitution of homeostasis.

It is important to note that a previous study also showed reduced amygdala–MPFC coupling after an aversive event, but there it was instead proposed that this decoupling reflects persistent negative interpretations and rumination evoked by the unpleasant stimuli (Eryilmaz, Van de Ville, Schwartz, & Vuilleumier, 2011). Our differing interpretations may be due to several important design differences. While our study only involved a single exposure to an aversive stimulus, the intermittent presentation of fearful movie excerpts by Eryilmaz et al. likely enhanced anticipatory anxiety and sustained fear. Their resting-state scans were also performed immediately after participants had viewed each film clip, whereas we examined resting-state functional connectivity approximately 15–30 min after stressor offset. Previous studies have linked rumination to increased cortisol levels after a stressor (Roger & Najarian, 1998; Zoccola, Dickerson, & Zaldivar, 2008), but we did not observe a reliable cortisol increase after the cold pressor task. Moreover, we found that individual differences in cortisol response were not related to neocortical interactions with the amygdala. Thus, in our study, MPFC–amygdala decoupling did not appear to be due to enhanced ruminative processes.

Exposure to the cold pressor task also led to anticorrelated functional connectivity between the left amygdala and right dACC. This functional interaction is supported by robust reciprocal anatomical connections between these two regions (Vogt & Pandya, 1987). The anterior portion of dACC, in particular, receives direct input from the amygdala, which plays a central role in both fear and nociception (Bernard, Huang, & Besson, 1992; Whalen et al., 1998). In accord with our results, increased activity in this region is often inversely correlated with amygdala response (Etkin, Egner, Peraza, Kandel, & Hirsch, 2006; Hariri, Mattay, Tessitore, Fera, & Weinberger, 2003). Because we used a seed-based correlation approach, we were unable to infer any causal relationship between these regions. However, models of effective connectivity have suggested that the dACC exerts top-down influences over the amygdala (Stein et al., 2007). It should be noted that the strength of this coupling was not associated with participants’ self-reported pain intensity during the cold pressor task. In pain manipulation studies, the dACC is primarily implicated in appraising the affective–motivational component of nociception (Coghill, McHaffie, & Yen, 2003; Maihofner & Handwerker, 2005; Rainville, Duncan, Price, Carrier, & Bushnell, 1997). Other studies have shown that the reappraisal of aversive events is often associated with increased activity in the dACC and decreased activity in the amygdala (Modinos, Ormel, & Aleman, 2010; Ochsner, Bunge, Gross, & Gabrieli, 2002), despite evidence that regulation is primarily localized to more ventral ACC regions (Etkin, Egner, & Kalisch, 2011). Taking this into account, the negative association between left amygdala and right dACC activity after the cold pressor task could be indicative of negative emotion reappraisal.

Beyond group-level changes in stressor-induced amygdala functional connectivity, left amygdalar recruitment of the right posterior LOFC was negatively associated with self-reported pain intensity during the cold pressor task, especially after controlling for cold pressor duration. In a study of frontal-lobe-damaged patients, their ratings for cold pressor pain intensity and unpleasantness were found to be significantly lower than those among controls (Talbot, Villemure, Bushnell, & Duncan, 1995). Increased activation of the posterior OFC has also been linked to lower pain intensity ratings (Derbyshire et al., 1997), and electrical stimulation of the VMPFC/OFC produces analgesia in both primates and nonprimates (Thorpe, Rolls, & Maddison, 1983; Zhang, Tang, Yuan, & Jia, 1997). Neuroimaging studies in humans accord with these findings, revealing that greater activity in this region is inversely related to subjective pain intensity (Salomons, Johnstone, Backonja, Shackman, & Davidson, 2007; Wiech et al., 2006). However, the OFC is not the only brain region associated with shaping pain perception. Using a thermal pain paradigm, Lapate et al. (2012) showed that amygdala activity varies as a function of pain regulation success. In addition, suppression of the startle reflex, an autonomic reflex that is dependent on the amygdala (Davis, Walker, Miles, & Grillon, 2010), has been observed during the cold pressor task, suggesting that amygdala activity is regulated while participants are forced to endure acute cold pain (Tavernor, Abduljawad, Langley, Bradshaw, & Szabadi, 2000). Here, we reconciled findings from studies examining individual brain regions by showing that the relationship between posterior OFC and pain intensity may, at least indirectly, be mediated by altered OFC functional coupling with the left amygdala.

Interestingly, individuals who reported greater pain intensity during the cold pressor task also demonstrated a marginally significant decrease in salivary cortisol after the stressor. Exposure to stressors not only triggers the release of cortisol, but a host of stress-related neurotransmitters, including endogenous opioids (Drolet et al., 2001). Growing evidence suggests an antagonistic relationship between cortisol and opioids, as the endogenous opioid system has been shown to inhibit HPA axis activation (al’Absi et al., 2004; Drolet et al., 2001). Thus, one intriguing possibility is that individuals who felt more pain during the cold pressor task also triggered greater opioid release (Frew & Drummond, 2007). This is consistent with the notion that the endogenous opioid system underlies an “opponent process,” whereby greater pain intensity corresponds with greater pain relief during stressor offset (Leknes, Brooks, Wiech, & Tracey, 2008). Several limbic regions, including dACC, OFC, and amygdala, are central target sites for opioid binding, making them prime candidates for the modulation and processing of pain (Fields, 2004; Zubieta et al., 2005). Furthermore, endogenous opioids typically have a delayed onset of several minutes to their maximum effect (Price & Barrell, 2000). These delayed effects are slow to dissipate, which agrees with our finding of residual rsFC changes between the amygdala and higher-cortical nodes of this opiate-sensitive network 15–30 min after the stressor. Thus, the variability that we observed in stressor-induced cortisol increase may have been a function of opioid-mediated engagement of a descending pain network (Bingel & Tracey, 2008; Watkins & Mayer, 1982). Our study design did not allow for a direct assessment of opioid release, so inclusion of such measures in future studies may help clarify whether the patterns of amygdala rsFC that we observed were driven by opiodergic systems.

One limitation of our study was that we only used male participants. Thus, it is not clear if the lack of a reliable stressor-related increase in cortisol was a sex-specific effect. Previous studies have reported mixed results regarding whether men or women show greater HPA activation to the cold pressor task, with some indicating men (e.g., Dixon, Thorn, & Ward, 2004), and others, women (e.g., Lighthall et al., 2009). Considering the anticorrelated trend between cortisol and pain intensity ratings, these mixed results could be attributed to individual differences in pain sensitivity or opioid release. Men demonstrate greater μ-opioid receptor activation in the amygdala during sustained pain than women do (Zubieta et al., 2002), suggesting that different mechanisms are activated in women in response to painful stressors. Animal studies have also indicated that acute stress affects the amygdala differently in males and females (Mitsushima, Yamada, Takase, Funabashi, & Kimura, 2006; Wang et al., 2009). In humans, amygdala functional connectivity differs between males and females at rest (Kilpatrick, Zald, Pardo, & Cahill, 2006), and the cold pressor task affects amygdala functional connectivity differently in males and females while participants view emotional faces (Mather, Lighthall, Nga, & Gorlick, 2010). As in our findings, Mather et al. found that the cold pressor task reduced amygdalar recruitment of emotion-processing regions in men, but not in women. Together, these findings suggest sex differences in pain- and emotion-processing regions during stress, with men showing greater engagement of endogenous analgesic systems. Future stress studies should include female participants to elucidate how gender differences in amygdala reactivity influence recovery from painful stressors.

To our knowledge, this is the first study to show that the effects of an acute physical stressor can be detected in cognitive–affective circuits during rest, as opposed to single brain regions, approximately 15–30 min after its onset/offset. This finding complements emerging evidence that the amygdala plays an important role in persistent pain states (Neugebauer, Li, Bird, & Han, 2004). Chronic inflammatory pain causes changes in the excitability and synaptic strength of noxious-detecting neurons in the amygdala (Neugebauer & Li, 2003). Similarly, enhanced activity in the amygdala can occur one week after the onset of pain, supporting a pain-producing or pain-enhancing role of the amygdala (Ikeda, Takahashi, Inoue, & Kato, 2007). Studies examining pain-related structural changes align with functional findings, demonstrating that the experience of chronic pain corresponds with gray matter density declines in the amygdala, ACC, and OFC/MPFC. (Burgmer et al., 2009; see May, 2011, for a review). This pattern of structural plasticity suggests that changes in amygdala-related pain networks may contribute to the chronification of pain. While speculations on chronicity are beyond the scope of this study, our observation of stressor-induced amygdala rsFC changes 15–30 min later suggests that pain-related modulation of amygdala activity persists even after the physical sensations have dissipated.

While we obtained pain intensity ratings immediately before and after the hand immersion task, no further pain scales were completed during the PASL scan. As a direct follow-up to this study, future experiments could examine how pain and stress influence the temporal dynamics of amygdala functional connectivity by administering assessments of coping strategy, pain intensity, and stress level throughout the PASL scan. Furthermore, an evaluation of negative affect could help determine whether poststressor amygdala functional connectivity relates to emotion regulation, particularly with the dACC. Acquiring concurrent measures of stress reactivity and pain could help dissociate the neural mechanisms that facilitate poststressor recovery and help to identify the aberrant amygdala functional connectivity patterns that predict vulnerability to pathological pain and emotion states, such as depression, posttraumatic stress disorder, anxiety disorders, and chronic pain.