Article Text

Download PDFPDF

The neuropsychological impact of insular cortex lesions
  1. Catherine L Jones1,2,
  2. Jamie Ward1,
  3. Hugo D Critchley2,3
  1. 1Department of Psychology, University of Sussex, Falmer, UK
  2. 2Clinical Imaging Sciences Centre, Brighton and Sussex Medical School, University of Sussex, Falmer, UK
  3. 3Sussex Partnership NHS Foundation Trust, UK
  1. Correspondence to Miss Catherine L Jones, Clinical Imaging Sciences Centre, University of Sussex, Eastern Ring Road, Falmer, Brighton BN1 9RR, UK; c.l.jones{at}bsms.ac.uk

Abstract

Influential models based on an increasing body of neuroimaging evidence propose that insular cortex integrates cognitive, affective, sensory and autonomic information to create a consciously perceived, ‘feeling state.’ To appraise these models and evaluate interpretations of neuroimaging findings, the authors review evidence pertaining to the psychological and behavioural consequences of insula lesions. The authors focus on the emotional, perceptual, sensorimotor symptoms and disorders of body awareness associated with insula damage. This comprehensive review is intended to inform existing neuropsychological models of insula function in order to guide future research.

  • Cognitive neuropsychology
  • limbic system
  • stroke
  • tumours

Statistics from Altmetric.com

Request Permissions

If you wish to reuse any or all of this article please use the link below which will take you to the Copyright Clearance Center’s RightsLink service. You will be able to get a quick price and instant permission to reuse the content in many different ways.

Introduction

Our knowledge regarding the functions of the insula (insular cortex or Island of Reil) are derived from partly convergent evidence from animal experiments, clinical reports, stimulation studies and more recently functional neuroimaging experiments. Located deep within the Sylvian fissure, the human insula is regarded as the ‘hidden fifth lobe.’ Macroscopically, the structure is divided into anterior and posterior portions by the central insular sulcus. In phylogenetic terms, the insula evolved from a smooth structure, as in the new world monkey, to five to seven lobes in humans, reflecting increased organisational and thus probably functional complexity. Anatomical and cytoarchitectural studies also suggest subregional specialisation within the insula.1 2 Post-mortem dissections show that the main arterial supply to the insula is the middle cerebral artery (MCA), predominantly the Sylvian segment (M2). Isolated insular infarcts are uncommon, and occlusion of the MCA results in cerebral infarction which typically affects the insula but will often result in diffuse damage across neighbouring regions including the internal capsule and basal ganglia. The insula can be affected by both high-grade and low-grade gliomas. Due to its location and functional complexity, resection of insula tumours has traditionally been viewed as hazardous, and this procedure is not well documented.

Studies using direct cortical stimulation link the insula to visceral sensation and autonomic regulation. Patients describe gastrointestinal sensations involving nausea and associated chewing, swallowing, salivation and also inhibition of respiration3 and changes in heart rate.4 Indeed, a number of studies report autonomic dysregulation as a result of damage to the insula.4 5 Functional neuroimaging studies reveal insular cortex to be active in a more diverse set of behavioural contexts including language, auditory processing, risk processing, sensorimotor processing, pain processing, taste and flavour perception in addition to, or in conjunction with, autonomic processing. Enhanced insula activity also appears to be a signature of affective symptoms and neuropsychiatric disturbances including anxiety disorders,6 drug addiction7 and emotional dysregulation.8 Thus, one challenge in developing a satisfactory neuropsychological model of insula function is to account for its involvement across these different domains which require access across multiple functional circuits. It is thus unclear if a unitary account of insula functioning is feasible or realistic. In the most general terms, one could argue that the insula serves an integrative function. An alternative would be to consider the insula as having a core function (eg, in visceral control) from which, over the course of evolution, more specialised functions have emerged (eg, taste perception, disgust, limb awareness, articulation of speech). Craig9 attempts to explain a wide variety of insula functions within a unitary account. In his model, insular cortex integrates information about changes in bodily, special-sensory and cognitive states to create unitary feelings that encapsulate ‘emotional now,’ ‘emotional self’ or a ‘global emotional moment.’

Another important consideration is whether different regions of the insula subserve different functions. One apparent anatomical distinction is between the anterior and posterior sections. The more posterior, granular regions of the insula, receive stronger inputs from the sensory thalamus and from parietal, occipital and temporal association cortices. Posterior insula has been ascribed a role in somatosensory, vestibular and motor integration. The more anterior, agranular regions have reciprocal connections to ‘limbic’ regions including anterior cingulate cortex, ventromedial prefrontal cortex, amygdala and ventral striatum. Anterior insula is implicated in the integration of autonomic and visceral information with emotional and motivational functions. A second important anatomical distinction is between possible differences between right- and left-hemisphere insula functions.10 Although finer cut anatomical distinctions are possible,11 at present the neuropsychological literature does not speak to this.

Patients with damage to insular cortex provide privileged means of evaluating the many functions associated with insula engagement. The extensive connections with other subcortical and cortical regions suggest that patients with damage to the insula ought to exhibit widespread neurological and psychological disturbances. The aim of the present review is to appraise and inform current theories of insula function (driven primarily from neuroimaging studies) in the light of neuropsychological studies (which have been featured less prominently). A further goal is to highlight gaps in our present knowledge that could be addressed through studies of neurological patients. An exhaustive search of the empirical literature was performed with keywords, insular cortex, insula, lesion, entered as search criteria into the PubMed database. The literature is dispersed between case reports, case series and lesion mapping studies, typically using statistical techniques to compute voxel-level associations between lesion and symptom.12 Table 1 summarises these studies grouped with reference to these three methodologies. For ease of exposition, the functions that have been attributed to the insula are grouped together below into four general themes. These are: (1) interoception, emotion and emotional decision-making; (2) perceptual functions; (3) speech and sensorimotor integration; and (4) bodily awareness. We also suggest further research approaches for examining neuropsychological effects of insular cortical lesions in order to extend our understanding of how the insula contributes to thinking, feeling and behaviour, and to better predict clinical outcomes.

Table 1

Neuropsychological studies of the effects of insula lesions on humans: summary of human neuropsychological studies examining the role of the insula either studying patients with relatively focal lesions, or studying larger groups of patients and inferring the critical lesion site for a given symptom using lesion-symptom mapping methods

Interoception, emotion and emotional decision-making

Interoception

The link between interoceptive awareness and emotional cognition has its historical roots in the writings of James, Lange and even Descartes, who argued that emotions are the perceptions of bodily responses. For example, fear is associated with sweating and hyperventilating, and sadness with the physical act of crying. According to these views, emotion necessitates experience of such bodily responses. These accounts fell out of favour with the view that bodily arousal states lack the same degree of differentiation as emotional states34 and evidence that cognitive schema determine which specific emotion might arise from a change in bodily arousal.35 However, elements of such peripheral theories of emotion reappear in several contemporary accounts, most notably Damasio's somatic marker hypothesis.36 The somatic marker hypothesis was developed to account for why patients with focal frontal brain lesions repeatedly manifest problems with social and motivational behaviour, despite awareness of the appropriate behaviour for that context. Somatic markers, that is bodily responses (related to emotional feelings), are proposed to bias and shape behaviour rather independently of conscious thought and awareness. Experimentally, this was apparent during performance of the ‘Iowa gambling task’ where learning and avoidance of risky decisions develop over the course of repeated trials in healthy people who produce normal somatic bodily responses. A full discussion of this theory is beyond the scope of this report (but see Bechara and Damasio37). In developments of this model, insular cortex is highlighted as an important neural substrate for representing these emotional markers derived from internal bodily responses, and through which perception of physiological responses may give rise to a conscious ‘gut feeling’ or urge that guides behaviour and influences future decisions. Neuroimaging studies show enhancement of insula activity when threatening emotional stimuli are perceived consciously in conjunction with bodily arousal,38 and both the level of activity and spatial extent of right insula activity in fMRI predict conscious awareness of one's own physiological processes such as heart beat.39 Similarly, representations of changes in physiological state in mid- and anterior insula appear to underpin consciously experienced fatigue associated with inflammation associated sickness.40 Interoception may allow for representations of cardiac (baroreceptor) function within the insula41–43 to be integrated with beliefs about one's physiological state (eg, based on false cardiac feedback), to guide emotional attributions.44 Interestingly, bilateral insula activation also commonly accompanies anxiety states associated with enhanced physiological arousal.45 Naqvi et al27 showed lesions to the insula (in either hemisphere) diminished addictive behaviours; patients with strokes involving insula reported greater ease in giving up smoking, without relapse or persistent urge to smoke, in contrast to smokers with strokes affecting other areas. This effect may be the result of a reduced ability to detect interoceptive states linked to craving, or a reduction in hedonic feelings induced by smoking cigarettes.46 This finding is also in line with a Somatic Marker model of addictive behaviour in which exposure to or recollection of an experience associated with a drug can reactivate this ‘interoceptive memory’ state.7 Future patient studies will establish how general the effects of insula lesions are on other forms of addiction. Evidence within the animal learning literature highlights the contribution of visceral states and insula cortex to motivational leaning.47 In broad terms, lesions to insular cortex are predicted to compromise the central representation of bodily responses, and thereby impact on social and motivational behaviour. Neuropsychological evidence relating to this idea is discussed in the next two sections.

Emotional decision-making

Humans can appear irrational in their reasoning, often relying on fast, intuitive, instinctive reactions to uncertain situations rather than rational deliberation.48 However lesions affecting insular cortex may disrupt these intuitive processes. Bar-On et al49 recruited patients with non-focal lesions of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, amygdala and insular/somatosensory cortices (experimental group) and assessed levels of emotional intelligence, social functioning, decision-making and general cognitive ability against patients with focal lesions in brain regions outside limbic and viscerosensory regions (control group). There were no significant differences between the experimental and control group in general cognitive performance. When playing the Iowa gambling task, normal subjects learn over time, through a schedule of rewards and punishments, to choose the one pack associated with the highest overall gain (and least risk).50 In the Bar-On study, the experimental group made significantly more disadvantageous than advantageous choices over time, resulting in an overall loss compared with the controls. Patients in the experimental group also had significantly impaired social functioning and emotional intelligence compared with control patients. Due to the small group sizes in this study, the authors were unable to compare performance between lesion sites of interest—for example, vmPFC, amygdala and insula. Also, the lesions extended across other brain regions, making it difficult to infer if a specific region or set of regions was responsible for poor task performance. However, the findings do have wider implications for patient recovery in terms of their neuropsychological and neuropsychiatric outcome.

Clark et al28 compared performance of patients with focal stable ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) lesions and focal insula lesions (greatest overlap in anterior portion) with a ‘control lesion’ group (lesions affecting dorsal and lateral PFC) and healthy control subjects on the Cambridge Gambling Task, a paradigm quantifying risky decision-making. Patients with insula lesions differed significantly from other groups in that they consistently betted at a high level, even as the odds became less favourable, thereby showing no evidence of risk adjustment. Paradoxically, another study demonstrates that insula lesion patients were more cautious in their decision-making, displaying significantly reduced risk-taking behaviour when compared with healthy controls even in risk-advantageous situations where there is the potential of gaining money.29 Common to both these observations28 29 is the observed insensitivity of insula patients to the expected value associated with the outcome which Weller et al29 proposes reflects an ‘emotional bluntness towards risk.’

Emotion: experience and recognition

Neuroimaging studies indicate a role for human insular cortex in representing autonomic information in the context of both negative emotion and positive emotion.51 Again, the postulated role of the insula is in the ‘feeling states’ of the emotion, in particular, the associated bodily sensations. One prediction is that patients with insula lesions will be able to perceive the emotional nature of stimuli illustrated—for example, by accurately categorising fearful and happy faces. However, insular patients would lack the appropriate affective response. Adolphs et al52 studied 108 patients with focal brain damage and determined the degree of lesion overlap as a function of emotional recognition. Right somatosensory related cortices, insula, anterior supramarginal gyrus along with left frontal opercular lesions were associated with poor performance in categorising facial expressions, consistent with the notion that the representation of bodily reactions (somatosensory and viscerosensory knowledge) contributes to emotional recognition through a simulatory ‘as-if loop.’

In terms of specific emotional deficits following insula damage, neuropsychological evidence is sparse, with the exception of disgust recognition, where integrity of the left anteroventral insula appears important. In a single case report, Patient NK showed highly selective impairments for recognising disgust from facial and vocal cues and diminished subjective experience of disgust following a focal lesion involving the left insula and basal ganglia.53 Another single case, patient B, had impaired recognition for dynamic facial expressions of disgust resulting from bilateral insula lesions.54 To further clarify insula involvement in disgust, Kipps et al55 used voxel-based morphometry to investigate how differences in grey-matter volume, probably reflecting tissue atrophy, were related to deficits in emotion processing in patients with preclinical Huntington disease. Significant correlations were found in volume of grey matter in left anteroventral insula and disgust recognition (this finding did not generalise to other emotions) in HD mutation carriers; this correlation was not present in the control group. It would be useful to establish whether self-reported emotional intensity and autonomic responses to emotional stimuli are normal in such patients. These lesion studies are consistent with neuroimaging data supporting a function of insular cortex in the recognition and experience of disgust, perhaps more so in the left hemisphere.56 Differential hemispheric sensitivity has been proposed for different emotion types, with the right-hemisphere activity associated with negative emotion and left-hemisphere activity associated with positive emotion.57 Some argue this functional asymmetry in emotion processing applies to insular cortex and reflects asymmetrical representation of homeostatic states derived from peripheral autonomic nervous system activity.9 10 The lesion data discussed thus far do not fully support this view, in that impairment in disgust processing results particularly from left insula damage. However, insular cortex in both hemispheres is involved in gustatory representation58 59 and is an early point of convergence for gustatory, olfactory and autonomic inputs.60 Left anterior insular cortex with the frontal operculum dominates in declarative perception of taste (see below). A greater body of patient-based evidence is needed to resolve whether there is topographic or hemispheric insula organisation with respect to negative and positive valenced stimuli and require further tests specifically designed to assess lateralisation differences in patients with unilateral lesions.

Perception

In this section, the impact of insula lesions on representing sensory information to form integrated percepts, specifically in the context of taste, pain and audition, is addressed.

Taste and flavour

The primary taste cortex in primates and humans is located in the rostrodorsal insula both anterior and posterior60 61 where gustatory, olfactory and visceral sensory inputs required for taste representation and perception of flavour are integrated. Taste and interoceptive inputs enter distinct parts of the nucleus tractus solitarius forming two pathways, which first come together in the insula.62 Some patients with insula tumours and resulting epilepsy are reported to experience seizures preceded by a gustatory aura,63 and insula epilepsy can trigger gustatory hallucinations.64 Further, electrical stimulation of the insula in patients undergoing surgery for epilepsy can elicit nausea, unpleasant tastes, and unpleasant throat and stomach sensations.3 65

Interestingly, evidence suggests that right insula lesions induce ipsilateral perception and recognition deficits, whereas left insula damage results in an ipsilateral deficit in taste perception but a bilateral deficit in taste recognition.21 23 This suggests that the left insula receives input from both sides of the tongue and relays this information to secondary taste and appraisal regions accessing language. Insula lesions can disrupt acquisition and expression of conditioned taste aversion and the enhancement or suppression of conditioned immune responses coupled to either taste or olfactory cues, leaving the normal immune functioning unaffected.66 Damage to the left insula is also associated with difficulties in assigning taste adjectives to smell (eg, classifying an odour as ‘sweet’) in the absence of impairments in olfactory processing.19b

Pain

The insula is a major site for cortical pain encoding.14b Nociceptive information is relayed via afferent fibres from the second somatosensory cortex (SII) to the posterior insula and then to the anterior insula. Clinical evidence supports this idea with patients with posterior insula lesions (but not anterior) exhibiting heightened pain thresholds15 or asymbolia (pain is perceived, but its aversive nature has been abolished).22 67 Further, evidence suggests that both the posterior and anterior insular cortices are somatotopically organised, supporting graded rerepresentations of pain signals.68

Reciprocal connections with prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, amygdala, parahippocampal gyrus and the SII promote the idea that the insula uses cognitive information to modulate connected brain regions involved in processing sensory-discriminatory, affective and cognitive-evaluative components of pain. Therefore, one could postulate that insula lesions would result in a complex pattern of altered sensitivity to pain stimuli. Recently, Starr et al19a used sensory assessment and fMRI with two patients who had suffered large left MCA ischaemic strokes involving insular cortex, to assess pain perception, sensory thresholds and neural correlates of pain experience. Both patients were able to rate the intensity of noxious stimuli of their affected side (right) with no evidence of insula activation in either hemisphere. However, stimulation of the unaffected side (left) was associated with activation in right insular cortex. Consistent with the previous literature, regions activated in controls related to generation of pain experience included ACC, SMA, SII, insula, DLPFC and cerebellum. Perhaps unexpectedly, both patients rated pain stimuli as significantly more intense compared with the healthy control group when their affected side was stimulated. Interestingly, there was no difference in the groups for unpleasantness rating of pain experience, suggesting that a decoupling of the two dimensions of pain may have occurred. The authors19a conclude that subjective awareness of noxious stimuli involves multiple, distinct patterns of brain activity where insular cortex is not a prerequisite.

Auditory perception

The role of insular cortex in auditory perception is controversial. There are a number of clinical reports of auditory agnosia on the contralateral side following insula infarction.14a 69 Bamiou et al70 describe auditory temporal processing deficits following insula infarction (particularly left hemispheric), in patients who otherwise had preserved cognitive function. Central auditory function in a series of patients with ischaemic lesions of the insula and of adjacent cortical and subcortical areas, with and without involvement of other auditory structures, was assessed by means of a validated central auditory test battery.

There is also evidence to suggest that insula is part of a functional network supporting sensory integration through processing temporal information about stimuli across different modalities.71 Assuming that the insula does play a role in temporal synchrony detection, it is unclear whether this role is strictly for multisensory events or for two events in the same modality (a recent review of the latter highlights the role of a parietal ‘when’ route.18b

Important questions remain, such as the distinct functions of the posterior and anterior insula in auditory processing, eg, whether the posterior insula is a primary sensory region representing basic auditory signals, while the anterior insula reacts to bimodal information and integrates multimodal sensory, cognitive and affective information. It is also of interest how these processes contribute to the emotional experience of music, potentially another important function of the anterior insula.9 These ideas can be explored by comparing patients with damage affecting posterior insula to patients with anterior insula damage on performance of both pure auditory perception tasks and tasks that require integration of sensory information and the recruitment of higher associative brain regions.

Speech and sensorimotor integration

Dronkers24 used the overlapping lesion method based on CT and MRI scans to show that lesions to the left anterior insula are associated with problems in articulation termed ‘apraxia of speech,’ a form of aphasia. This finding is supported by a number of studies24 72 73 but is not a universally accepted view. Hillis et al74 argue that Broca's area, and not the insula, is associated with articulatory impairments. Detail of tissue damage was acquired using diffusion-weighted and perfusion-weighted imaging revealing that apraxia of speech was correlated with damage to the Broca area and not the insula. The authors conclude that in apraxia of speech, the insula is most commonly damaged because it is most vulnerable to disruption of the middle cerebral artery, and so this symptom and insular damage may be independent manifestations of large stroke.74 Other studies have also noted difficulties with speech that are significantly greater in speech repetition (ie, audio-motor transformation) than in reading aloud or other modes of speech production.13 18a Consistent with these findings, Schmid and Ziegler75 noted that performance in a multisensory matching task (involving audio-visual syllables) was predicted by both repetition ability and presence of apraxia for speech. Evidence for functional compensation of the insula in language arises from a study of 42 patients who underwent surgery for insula low-grade glioma. Intraoperative electrical mapping evoked language, sensory (pain) and vestibular disturbances in some patients. Immediately following resection, 21 patients experienced transient contralateral hemiparesis, 10 patients developed articulatory disorders, and in seven cases inertia with loss of interest and affect (abulia) was reported.76 However, 3 months postsurgery, there was no evidence of visceral, cardiovascular, sensorimotor, gustatory, auditory-vestibular or language disorders in any of the patients. Research which fully assesses cognitive functioning in tumour patients pre-, intra- and postoperatively will be an important future resource for determining regional functional specificity.77

Functional imaging studies show that both auditory speech perception and speech production activate a region in the left insula78 and that the bilateral insula responds to both heard mouth sounds (non-speech) and motor execution of the mouth.79 The generation of the illusory syllable in the McGurk illusion (synchronous presentation auditory /ba/ and visually presented /ga/ gives rise to illusory syllable /da/80), relative to non-illusory audio-visual synchronous syllables, has been linked to activation of motor representations of speech including the insula and Broca area.81

In sum, although there is evidence for an audio-visuo-motor representation of speech in the insula, there is presently insufficient evidence to distinguish its functional role (if any) from other speech and motor regions. Clinical findings are inconsistent, perhaps reflecting inadequacies of patient selection criteria, differences in patient assessment and radiology, involvement of neighbouring regions and patients' own inherent characteristics prelesion onset.

Bodily awareness

Some patients with right posterior insula lesions deny paralysis (anosognosia for hemiplegia) or may deny lack of tactile sensation (anosognosia for hemianaesthesia). This condition does not seem to reflect deficits in motor or sensory perception per se but a disturbance in the ‘highest level of organisation’ of a given function.82 Karnath et al30 used lesion overlay plots to show the posterior insula is the structure most commonly damaged in a group of patients with stroke with anosognosia for hemiplegia/hemiparesis compared with a group otherwise matched for clinical and demographic variables, where posterior insula damage occurred in half of the patients. Anosognosia for hemiplegia or hemiparesis predominantly originates from damage to the right side of the brain. It is a major neuropsychological disorder frequently observed in neurological patients with failure of recovery and rehabilitation often depending on its presence. Increasing severity of anosognosia is associated with larger lesions, suggesting that the condition may result from damage to or disconnection of a network of cerebral regions, typically frontal and parietal, and not a specific cortical or subcortical region and thus a limitation of the Karnath et al. A summary of human neuropsychological studies that have examined the role of the insula either studying patients with relatively focal lesions, or studying larger groups of patients and inferring the critical lesion site for a given symptom using lesion-symptom mapping methods30 study is that the patients sustained damage to multiple brain structures. However, Cereda et al23 report striking evidence that a focal lesion to the right posterior insula can be sufficient to induce a form of anosognosia—somatoparaphrenia. In this instance, the patient experienced delusions of being touched by a stranger's hand, and she thought of her upper limb as a foreign body. In healthy participants, the right posterior insula is activated in one multisensory illusion that distorts body ownership, namely the rubber hand illusion.83 In this illusion, the location of one's own arm/hand (hidden from view) is mislocalised to an observed arm/hand when both are stroked in synchrony. A patient group with lesions involving posterior insula, who are generally more resistant to the rubber hand illusion compared with other lesion groups, would be of future experimental interest. A functional imaging study used PET in healthy volunteers to show right posterior insula activation when visually observed movement corresponded to the participants' executed movements, with activity decreasing with a reduced sense of controlling the movement.84 Thus, the posterior insula seems to be significantly involved in the integration of sensory and motor information with input signals relating to self-awareness and expectations of the functioning of one's body.

In a recent review paper, Spinazzola et al32 argue that different disorders of unawareness such as varying forms of anosognosia, somatoparaphrenia and neglect are domain-specific disorders of consciousness, with the region of brain damage differentially disrupting self-monitoring processes that in normal circumstances act to regulate awareness of our physical and cognitive status. The insula is viewed as a comparator site, detecting mismatch between predicted and actual peripheral feedback, and therefore may act as a key structure in self-monitoring.

Summary and conclusion

Evidence from neuropsychological reports discussed in this paper indicates that insular cortex is active in multiple functional networks. Certain symptoms seem to be more closely associated with lesions involving the anterior insula, including disruption of affective responses, for example, in amusia (impaired ability to appreciate the emotional content of music) but also language functions, especially following left hemisphere damage. In contrast, lesions involving the posterior insula are more likely to result in disruption of the representation of interoceptive information, for example alterations in pain, temperature or tactile perception. While some previous reviews imply that anosognosia for hemiplegia/hemianaesthesia are disorders of emotional awareness that reflect anterior insula damage, evidence suggests that the disorder is more closely associated with posterior insula damage.

At present, models of insula function fall short of adequately explaining in detail the range of neuropsychological effects reviewed above. From lesion and neuroimaging data, we can conclude broadly that the insula supports an integration of body and mind, yet the mechanisms through which these interoceptive signals are perceived, how they are translated into perceptual sensation (‘feeling’), and how disparity between predicted and actual feeling states is detected, is less obvious in the presence of marked insula damage. Craig proposes a metarepresentational model of integration across insula cortex where primary interoceptive signals are represented in posterior insula and are integrated with affective, sensory and cognitive information to be represented as a ‘global emotional moment’ where awareness of feeling state occurs in the anterior insula.9 This re-representation does not imply redundancy, but rather the integrated whole may have an important role to play in behaviour and underpin certain subjective states such as those implicated in anxiety.39 85 Indeed, when interoceptive mismatch is induced experimentally using false physiological feedback, enhanced anterior insula activity correlates with increased emotional salience attributed to previously unthreatening neutral face stimuli.44 In this sense, a general function of the insula is as a comparator, detecting incongruence in predicted and actual emotional states. These concepts are embodied within a recent integrative model of insula function in which amplitude and variance data regarding internal physiological and external sensory information are represented as concurrent and prediction signals (sensory feeling states) and integrated into a dominant motivational feeling state.86 Neuroimaging has shown that the anterior insula stores an error representation when the states are mismatched; this information can be used in reinforcement learning, to adjust future behaviour.87 88 Damage to such a mechanism may, for example, result in the inability to adjust risk behaviour, as was observed recently29 which could have significant clinical implications. Establishing the precise role of the insula in decision-making under uncertainty is possible with tasks designed to probe specific questions, for example, whether the insula is chiefly involved in risk prediction learning or if it is also active in reward prediction learning.

Targeted evaluation (hypothesis testing) of the cognitive and emotional status of patients with insular lesions can be achieved using neuropsychological measures adapted to detect changes in functional integration—for example, cross-modal integration in speech. Neuropsychological assessments administered at different stages post-lesion onset will aid the discovery of independent predictor variables linked with ongoing cognitive dysfunction and functional outcome. This approach will help to address the many outstanding questions concerning the complex behavioural specialisations of insular cortex: its precise role in interoception, emotional awareness, self recognition, risk prediction and anticipation, time perception and performance monitoring. It also worth noting that few studies have used functional neuroimaging in insula lesion patients, a powerful tool for detecting remaining functionality or functional reorganisation associated with normal task performance.89 This is an important next step in research and is especially relevant to preoperative and postoperative functional imaging where insights can be gained into brain plasticity. The possibility of other brain regions compensating for damage to this multimodal area will have important implications, in terms of better understanding the pathophysiological role of this structure and its relationship with other brain regions. It is also important from a clinical perspective in tumoural surgery, due to the frequent location of gliomas in the insula, and in degenerative diseases such as Huntington and Alzheimer's disease, where insula changes may be associated with early-stage symptomatology.

References

  1. 1.
  2. 2.
  3. 3.
  4. 4.
  5. 5.
  6. 6.
  7. 7.
  8. 8.
  9. 9.
  10. 10.
  11. 11.
  12. 12.
  13. 13.
  14. 14a.
  15. 14b.
  16. 15.
  17. 16.
  18. 17.
  19. 18a.
  20. 18b.
  21. 19a.
  22. 19b.
  23. 20.
  24. 21.
  25. 22.
  26. 23.
  27. 24.
  28. 25.
  29. 26.
  30. 27.
  31. 28.
  32. 29.
  33. 30.
  34. 31.
  35. 32.
  36. 33.
  37. 34.
  38. 35.
  39. 36.
  40. 37.
  41. 38.
  42. 39.
  43. 40.
  44. 41.
  45. 42.
  46. 43.
  47. 44.
  48. 45.
  49. 46.
  50. 47.
  51. 48.
  52. 49.
  53. 50.
  54. 51.
  55. 52.
  56. 53.
  57. 54.
  58. 55.
  59. 56.
  60. 57.
  61. 58.
  62. 59.
  63. 60.
  64. 61.
  65. 62.
  66. 63.
  67. 64.
  68. 65.
  69. 66.
  70. 67.
  71. 68.
  72. 69.
  73. 70.
  74. 71.
  75. 72.
  76. 73.
  77. 74.
  78. 75.
  79. 76.
  80. 77.
  81. 78.
  82. 79.
  83. 80.
  84. 81.
  85. 82.
  86. 83.
  87. 84.
  88. 85.
  89. 86.
  90. 87.
  91. 88.
  92. 89.

Footnotes

  • Funding CLJ is supported by the Medical Research Council and the Brighton and Sussex Medical School. HDC is supported by a programme grant from the Wellcome Trust.

  • Competing interests None.

  • Provenance and peer review Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.