Abstract
The contribution of the sympathetic nervous system (SNS) to pain, mechanical allodynia (MA), and hyperalgesia in humans is controversial. A clearer understanding is crucial to guide therapeutic use of sympatholytic surgery, blocks, and drug treatments. In rats, capsaicin-evoked MA, and to some extent, pinprick hyperalgesia (PPH), can be blocked with α-adrenoreceptor antagonists. In this study, we examined the contribution of the SNS to MA and PPH in normal human subjects by blocking α-adrenoreceptors with intravenous phentolamine.
In a double-blinded, placebo-controlled, crossover study, subjects were given IV saline or phentolamine, 1 mg/kg over 20 min. Ten minutes after the start of the infusion, subjects received 100 μg of intradermal capsaicin on the foot dorsum with the temperature of the injected site clamped at 36°C. The temperature of the uninjected foot was used to monitor the degree of α-adrenoreceptor blockade produced by phentolamine. Ongoing pain and MA and PPH areas were measured every 5 min for 60 min.
A significantly greater increase in temperature on the uninjected foot was seen during the phentolamine infusion compared with the saline infusion, indicating α-adrenergic blockade. Significantly less MA was observed with the phentolamine infusion 10–25 min after capsaicin injection than with the saline infusion. No significant differences in ongoing pain or PPH areas were seen between the two infusions at any time.
Our results suggest that capsaicin-evoked MA and PPH have different mechanisms, with the SNS having a role in MA but not in PPH or ongoing pain.
- sympathetic nervous system
- mechanical allodynia
- mechanical hyperalgesia
- capsaicin
- phentolamine
- pain
After some cases of nerve injury, pain phenomenon such as mechanical allodynia (MA), pain caused by normally innocuous stimuli) and pinprick hyperalgesia (PPH), greater pain than that normally induced by a painful stimulus) are often seen within and outside of the peripheral nerve territory of the injured area (Kelly, 1952; Dykes, 1984; Marchettini et al., 1992). The mechanisms of MA and PPH are not completely understood. One explanation is that these changes are caused by sensitization of secondary neurons of the CNS by intense peripheral nociceptor input, resulting in abnormal processing of painful and normally nonpainful stimuli. These neurons may require continued peripheral input to sustain these changes (Levine et al., 1985; LaMotte et al., 1991; Gracely et al., 1992). Animal data have indicated that the sympathetic nervous system (SNS) may contribute to this heightened peripheral input. In the normal, uninjured state, somatosensory primary afferent nociceptors do not appear to have sympathetic sensitivity. However, after injury, catecholamine sensitivity is often seen (Sato and Perl, 1991). Neuromas proximal to the site of injury develop ectopic discharges that increase with close intra-arterial injection of catecholamines and with stimulation of the sympathetic trunk (Wall and Gutnick, 1974; Korenman and Devor, 1981;Jänig, 1990).
In addition to nerve injury, pain produced by direct chemical activation of C-fibers with capsaicin, the purified active ingredient of chili peppers, has been used as a model to study MA and PPH. Capsaicin has been shown to cause MA and PPH, with symptoms similar to those of nerve injury (Koltzenburg et al., 1992; Torebjörk et al., 1992). In both the nerve injury and capsaicin animal models, surgical and chemical sympathectomies and peripheral α-adrenoreceptor antagonists have relieved MA (Kim and Chung, 1991; Kinnman and Levine, 1995).
Patients with MA, PPH, and trophic changes possibly suggestive of increased sympathetic tone often obtain relief after block of the sympathetic innervation to the affected area (Loh and Nathan, 1978;Raja et al., 1991). These patients are considered to have “sympathetically maintained pain” (SMP) (Roberts, 1986). Many of these patients also obtain relief with the nonspecific α-adrenergic antagonist phentolamine and with regional sympatholytic treatment with guanethidine (Loh and Nathan, 1978; Blanchard et al., 1990; Raja et al., 1991). Phentolamine provides pain relief comparable with that seen with local anesthetic block of the sympathetic ganglion (Raja et al., 1991).
However, the importance of the SNS in sensory changes remains controversial. Some investigators claim that the sensory changes outside the territory of an injured nerve and the apparent relief with sympathetic block are attributable to psychological factors (Verdugo and Ochoa, 1994; Verdugo et al., 1994; Ochoa and Verdugo, 1995). An experimental model of SMP in normal volunteers would facilitate research into the sympathetic contribution to pain. In this study, we examined the possible role of the SNS, specifically the α-adrenergic receptors, in the production of MA and PPH produced by capsaicin in normal human volunteers.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Healthy volunteers were entered into this double-blinded, placebo-controlled, two-period crossover study. Because of the large variability in responses to capsaicin (Liu et al., 1995a; Park et al., 1995), all subjects were screened for their MA response to intradermal (ID) capsaicin before entry. Patients with MA responses ≥ 5 cm2 for at least 20 min advanced to the infusion portion of the study.
During all experimental sessions, subjects were placed in the seated position with their lower leg 135° to their thigh. We preferred subjects to be in a sitting position, because we had used that position in a previous study of MA and PPH on the forearm and foot dorsum and found larger areas of MA and PPH on the foot dorsum (Liu et al., 1995b). In addition, we had observed lower cutaneous blood flow in the foot dorsum with the foot dependent compared with the foot placed at the same level as the legs, suggesting higher sympathetic tone in the dependent position (Liu M, Max MB, Robinovitz E, Gracely RH, Bennett GJ, unpublished observations). Subjects were asked to inform the investigators whether they had any sensations of the leg “falling asleep.”
Because of the known temperature sensitivity of capsaicin-evoked sensory changes (Koltzenburg et al., 1992; Liu et al., 1995a), we stabilized the skin temperature of the injection site on the dorsum of the left foot at 36°C (range, 35.8–36.2°) using a feed-back-controlled heat lamp with the thermistor placed 1 cm from the injection site. Once the temperature was stable, 100 μg (10 μl) of ID capsaicin (Fluka, Ronconocow, NY) in Tween 80 was injected into that foot. Temperature was monitored in the uninjected right foot to assess the effect of phentolamine on peripheral adrenoreceptors. Ongoing pain was measured at the time of injection using a 0–100, 20 cm Visual Analog Scale (VAS). Ongoing pain, MA, and PPH were measured every 5 min after injection for a total of 60 min. MA was measured using a #2 flat artist’s brush moving along eight radial spokes, beginning in an area with normal sensation ∼8 cm from the injection site and moving toward the site at a rate of 1 cm/sec, as described by Simone et al. (1989). To ensure beginning in an area of normal sensation, brush sensation of the injected foot ∼8 cm from the injection site was compared with the brush sensation of the uninjected foot 5 min after injection. Subjects were instructed to inform the investigator whether and when the sensation changed to “pain or discomfort.” This spot was marked lightly with a marking pen. PPH was assessed using a 3.8 cm safety pin pressed lightly into the skin to cause a just barely visible indentation. Testing began 8 cm from the injection site, comparing the sensation with that of the uninjected foot to ensure that initial sites of stimulation had normal pinprick sensation. Subjects did not observe the sensory testing. Subjects were instructed to inform the investigators when the pinprick sensation changed into “something more painful or a different type of pain.” If the subjects answered positively, he or she was asked to describe the sensation. This spot was then marked with a marking pen. The marks for the areas of MA and PPH were then transferred onto clear acetate sheets and connected to form polygons.
All sensory testing was done by three nurse investigators. Subjects who advanced to the phentolamine/placebo portion of the study had the same nurse for all three sessions. Nurse investigators were thoroughly trained in the brush and pin techniques before performing sensory testing on any subjects, and all were observed to perform the testing in a similar manner. Nurse investigators and the subjects were blinded as to the infusion condition. The physician monitoring the vital signs and temperature of the uninjected foot was not blinded but did not participate in the sensory testing. Pulse-oximetry, blood pressure, and ECG monitors were hidden from the view of the subjects and the nurses. Subjects were questioned for presyncopal symptoms (light-headedness, dizziness, nausea) 2 min before capsaicin injection and 30 sec and 5 min after injection during each session.
Subjects were randomly assigned to either of two possible treatments orders: 1 mg/kg phentolamine (Ciba-Geigy, Summit, NJ) and saline or saline and phentolamine. A 20 gauge intravenous catheter was placed into a right antecubital vein, and a bolus of 1000 ml of saline given over 30 min before the start of the phentolamine/placebo infusion as prophylaxis against hypotension that might result from phentolamine. Once the bolus was completed, the phentolamine/placebo solution (100 ml) was infused over 20 min with a 2 ml/kg/hr background infusion rate of saline. Infusion sessions were all conducted in the afternoon. Subjects were instructed to have both breakfast and lunch before the sessions. Sessions, including the capsaicin screen, were separated by a minimum of 1 week. The subject’s surface anatomy (veins, nevi, scars, etc.) was traced onto acetate sheets and the point of injection marked on this sheet to ensure that areas used previously were not reinjected. To keep injection sites relatively uniform, sites were chosen so that they were 1–2 mm adjacent to veins on the mid-dorsum of the foot. Continuous EKG and pulse-oximetry and intermittent blood pressure readings (every 10 min before the start of the drug infusion, every 2 min during the infusion and the initial 15 min after the completion of the infusion, and every 5 min 15 min after the end of the infusion) were obtained throughout each infusion session. The skin temperature of the left foot was stabilized, as described above. Temperature of the uninjected foot was measured continuously with a contact thermistor to assess presence of α-adrenergic blockade. Ten minutes after the start of the phentolamine/placebo infusion, the subject was given 100 μg of ID capsaicin to the foot dorsum.
MA and PPH areas were obtained by copying the acetate sheets onto standard photocopy paper, cutting out and weighing the obtained areas, and comparing these weights with the weight of a known area.
Statistical analysis. Because preliminary analysis revealed a non-Gaussian distribution, MA and PPH areas for the two treatments at each time point were compared using the Wilcoxon signed rank test. Ongoing pain was analyzed using ANOVA with repeated measures. Peak temperature difference was analyzed using a t test. All results are shown as mean ± SEM.
RESULTS
Twenty-three subjects were prescreened with capsaicin to obtain the final sample size of 16. Six subjects had inadequate MA areas during the screen, and one subject chose not to continue to the drug portion of the study. During the screening session, subjects with MA and PPH were asked to describe the sensation they had. MA was consistently described as a burning or tenderness and PPH was described as an initial increased sharpness followed by a burning sensation with removal of the pin. The study population consisted of 3 women and 13 men whose ages ranged from 22 to 39 years.
All subjects reported high pain VAS 10 sec after capsaicin injection during both the phentolamine and placebo infusions, 86.5 ± 1.2 versus 85.5 ± 1.2, respectively, on a scale of 100 (NS). Significantly smaller MA areas were noted during the phentolamine session as compared with the placebo session at each time point from 10 to 25 min after capsaicin administration (p < 0.05) (Fig. 1). No significant difference in ongoing VAS pain scores were noted between the two sessions at any time point, although there was a nonsignificant tendency for lower pain with phentolamine (p > 0.3, ANOVA) (Fig.2). There were no differences in PPH areas between the two sessions at any time point (Fig. 3). No differences in the descriptions of the PPH sensations were observed between the saline and phentolamine infusions.
Higher temperatures were seen in the uninjected foot from the end of the phentolamine infusion (10 min after capsaicin injection) to the end of the session (Fig. 4). A mean peak increase (±SEM) in the temperature of the uninjected foot (measured before the start of the drug infusion to the peak temperature during the 60 min testing interval) of 2.9 ± 0.6°C was seen in the phentolamine session versus 0.6 ± 0.2°C in the placebo session (p < 0.001), indicating presence of α-adrenergic blockade (Raja et al., 1991, 1996). On average, maximal temperature change was noted 15 min after the end of the infusion or 25 min after capsaicin injection (Fig. 4).
Vital signs remained stable through both infusions. Vasopressors were not required by any subject for treatment of hypotension during either infusion.
Unlike previous investigators (Raja et al., 1991), we did not use β-blockers to blunt any possible tachycardia. In three pilot patients, we had administered the recommended intravenous 1 mg propranolol just before phentolamine infusion. Two of the three subjects developed near-syncopal symptoms consisting of complaints of nausea and light-headedness accompanied by bradycardia and hypotension ∼2-5 min after capsaicin injection. We thought that this resulted from the blockade of hemodynamic compensatory mechanisms evoked by the combination of phentolamine and propranolol with the subject in the sitting position, causing unacceptable bradycardia and hypotension. Based on these reactions, propranolol was not used in our study and the prestudy saline bolus was increased from 500 to 1000 ml. No clinically significant incidents of bradycardia or hypotension were seen after the propranolol was discontinued. Although none of the subjects were informed of the symptoms of α-adrenoreceptor blockade, 13 of the 16 subjects reported nasal stuffiness during the phentolamine infusion.
DISCUSSION
Our results extend to humans the findings obtained by Kinnman and Levine (1995) in rats. These investigators observed a complete or nearly complete prevention of capsaicin-evoked MA by phentolamine and by prazosin, a specific α-1 adrenoreceptor antagonist. However, in our experiments, MA was not completely blocked by phentolamine. The discrepancy may be attributable to a species difference in the amount of sympathetic tone in the extremities, which is likely to be greater in humans because of the greater distance from the heart and the species difference in the thermoregulatory role of cutaneous blood flow (rats thermoregulate primarily by spreading saliva over their fur). In addition, the dose of phentolamine that we used (1 mg/kg over 20 min) results in only partial blockade (∼88%) (Raja et al., 1994).
We observed a significant decrease in MA only during the period of 10–25 min after capsaicin injection. The lack of a significant effect at the 5 min test point, when 75% of the phentolamine dose had been delivered, may reflect an inadequate block. This is supported by the temperature data (Fig. 4), which showed little of the expected temperature increase at 5 min after capsaicin administration. The lack of a phentolamine effect after 25 min may have been at least partly attributable to the metabolism of phentolamine, the elimination half-life of which is 19 min. The waning effect after 25 min was also probably attributable to the natural time course of capsaicin-evoked MA. As has been described previously, many subjects showed little MA 30 min after injection (Simone et al., 1989; LaMotte et al., 1991; Liu et al., 1995a; Park et al., 1995). The decrease in MA with phentolamine observed in the present study is not attributable to a temperature effect, because the site of capsaicin injection was kept at a fixed temperature of 36°C, a temperature higher than the peak temperature reached in the uninjected foot during the phentolamine session.
Although we cannot rule out the possibility that phentolamine-induced nasal congestion unblinded the subjects and introduced self-report bias, we would have expected such a bias to produce similar effects on MA, PPH, and ongoing pain. Although the nurses doing the sensory testing could also potentially introduce bias after hearing subjects report this symptom, these clinicians had no investment in any particular outcome of the study, particularly the complex set of findings that we report.
The present data and the animal experiments of Kinnman and Levine (1995) suggest that capsaicin-evoked MA is reduced by blockade of sympathetic α-adrenoreceptors. The mechanism of this effect is unknown.
A CNS mechanism is unlikely, because Kinnman and Levine (1995) showed that preganglionic sympathectomy has no effect on capsaicin-evoked MA. With the high dose of phentolamine used in the present experiments, we cannot exclude the possibility of a central effect of the drug. However, one would expect that central α-adrenoreceptor block wouldincrease pain via a reduction in the activity of the descending pain-inhibitory noradrenergic pathway that terminates in the spinal dorsal horn. Thus, it seems probable that the effects seen in the present experiments, like those of Kinnman and Levine (1995), are attributable to an effect in the periphery. There are two possible peripheral sites for an α-adrenoreceptor effect on capsaicin-evoked MA: Aβ low-threshold mechanoreceptors (Aβ LTMs) or C-fiber nociceptors.
An effect on Aβ LTMs is an unlikely explanation for our results. MA is evoked by Aβ LTM stimulation (Torebjörk et al., 1992; Sang et al., 1996), and there is no reason to suspect that α-adrenoreceptor blockade has any effect on stimulus-evoked activity in these afferents. Roberts (1986) has suggested that a tonic sympathetically maintained Aβ LTM input sustains the hyperalgesic and allodynic sensations seen in nerve-injured patients. It is difficult to imagine how such a mechanism might be relevant to the present case, in which normal subjects exhibited phentolamine-blockable MA 10 min after capsaicin injection.
If there is an α-adrenoreceptor effect that is necessary for or enhances the capsaicin-evoked C-nociceptor discharge, then α-adrenoreceptor blockade would prevent or reduce the amount of central sensitization and thereby reduce the substrate for MA. However, there is no direct evidence that an α-adrenoreceptor action on C-nociceptors modulates their activation by capsaicin. Capsaicin is an inflammatory stimulus and inflammation sensitizes C-nociceptors and “awakens” normally silent “sleepy” C-nociceptors (Handwerker et al., 1991; Davis et al., 1993; Sato et al., 1993; Schmidt et al., 1995). If an α-adrenoreceptor action is necessary for sensitization or for awakening sleepy nociceptors, then phentolamine block might have reduced the amount of C-nociceptor activity during at least the latter stages of the postinjection interval. If this occurred, one would expect phentolamine to blunt the perceived intensity of capsaicin-evoked pain. Our results indicate a trend toward decreased pain with phentolamine, but this did not reach statistical significance (p > 0.3; Fig. 2).
Phentolamine had no effect on capsaicin-evoked PPH. Several lines of evidence indicate that capsaicin-evoked MA and PPH are at least partly independent phenomena. Capsaicin-evoked PPH is found in a distinctly larger area than MA, and it lasts a much longer time (Simone et al., 1989; LaMotte et al., 1991; Park et al., 1995). MA and PPH sensations are distinctly different qualitatively: MA is felt as a burning pain or a dysesthetic “raw” feeling like that of a sunburn; PPH is initially a distinctly localized, sharp, pricking pain with an electric shock-like component that evokes a flinch (M. Liu, unpublished observations). The afferent input that evokes PPH is not known, but it is likely to be Aδ- and/or C-fiber nociceptors. This is supported by a report of a patient with loss of Aβ function and preservation of Aδ and C-fibers, showing that capsaicin produced ongoing pain and PPH but not MA (Treede and Cole, 1993). It may be of significance that trauma does not evoke α-adrenergic sensitivity in Aδ afferents (Bossut and Perl, 1995).
It has been argued that the relief of neuropathic pain in patients receiving sympathetic or α-adrenoreceptor blockade is a placebo response and, further, that this and other evidence indicate that many patients diagnosed with SMP and related conditions suffer from a disorder that is primarily or exclusively psychogenic (Verdugo and Ochoa, 1994; Verdugo et al., 1994; Ochoa and Verdugo, 1995). The results reported here cannot be explained in this way, because neither the experimenters nor the subjects had any reason to believe that phentolamine would affect MA but not PPH or ongoing pain.
The human capsaicin model may be useful for elucidating the mechanisms by which sympathetic activity contributes to pain. MA and other abnormal pain sensations are exacerbated by α-adrenoreceptor agonists (Drummond, 1995) and relieved by sympathetic nerve and α-adrenoreceptor blockade in patients and animals with painful peripheral neuropathies (Bonica, 1990; Bennett and Roberts, 1996) and in animal models of inflammation (Levine et al., 1985, 1986; Hu and Zhu, 1989; Sato et al., 1993). The sensory changes obtained with capsaicin occur within minutes, and their mechanism may differ from those occurring in some chronic SMP syndromes in which new adrenoreceptor formation has been hypothesized. Although there may be differences in the sympathetic efferent and adrenoreceptor contribution to inflammatory, neuropathic, and capsaicin-evoked pain, our results suggest that animal and human capsaicin models may be used in tandem to work out the details of sympathetic interactions with each class of primary afferent and secondary neurons. The resulting insights into neural integration are likely to contribute to the understanding of many pain conditions.
Footnotes
- Received June 10, 1996.
- Revision received August 19, 1996.
- Accepted August 29, 1996.
We thank Elaine Robinovitz and Mary Wells for their help in conducting this study, Dr. Robert Coghill for help with the statistics, and Drs. Eli Eliav and Andrew Mannes for reviewing this manuscript.
Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Mitchell B. Max, National Institutes of Health, 9000 Rockville Pike, Building 10, Room 3C-405, Bethesda, MD 20892.
Dr. Bennett’s current address: Department of Neurology, Allegheny University of the Health Sciences, Philadelphia, PA 19102.
- Copyright © 1996 Society for Neuroscience