Abstract
We have recently disrupted Slc12a2, the gene encoding the secretory Na–K–2Cl cotransporter in mice (NKCC1) (Delpire et al., 1999). Gramicidin perforated-patch and whole-cell recordings were performed to study GABA-induced currents in dorsal root ganglion (DRG) neurons isolated from wild-type and homozygote NKCC1 knock-out mice. In wild-type DRG neurons, strong GABA-evoked inward current was observed at the resting membrane potential, suggesting active accumulation of Cl− in these cells. This GABA-induced current was blocked by picrotoxin, a GABAAreceptor blocker. The strong Cl− accumulation that gives rise to depolarizing GABA responses is caused by Na–K–2Cl cotransport because reduction of external Cl− or application of bumetanide induced a decrease in [Cl−]i, whereas an increase in external K+ caused an apparent [Cl−]i accumulation. In contrast to control neurons, little or no net current was observed at the resting membrane potential in homozygote NKCC1 mutant DRG neurons.EGABA was significantly more negative, demonstrating the absence of Cl− accumulation in these cells. Application of bumetanide induced a positive shift ofEGABA, suggesting the presence of an outward Cl− transport mechanism. In agreement with an absence of GABA depolarization in DRG neurons, behavioral analysis revealed significant alterations in locomotion and pain perception in the knock-out mouse. Our results clearly demonstrate that the Na–K–2Cl cotransporter is responsible for [Cl−]i accumulation in DRG neurons and that via regulation of intracellular Cl−, the Na–K–2Cl cotransporter participates in the modulation of GABA neurotransmission and sensory perception.
- Na–K–2Cl cotransporter
- knock-out mouse
- dorsal root ganglion
- bumetanide
- chloride
- GABA
- nociception
GABA, viahyperpolarization of postsynaptic membranes, acts as an inhibitory neurotransmitter in the adult CNS. However, in several adult sensory neurons, GABA elicits depolarizingresponses. GABAergic terminals, mostly from interneurons, are presynaptic to central terminals of muscle and nociceptive afferent fibers ending in the dorsal columns of the spinal cord. The release of GABA by interneurons depolarizes the terminals of these primary afferent fibers leading to presynaptic inhibition. Presynaptic inhibition is thought to reduce or eliminate sensory “noise” originating from second-order neurons, enhancing sensory contrast and allowing only the passage of more potent sensory inputs (Willis, 1999). Thus, GABA via primary afferent depolarization and presynaptic inhibition participates in the modulation of excitability of the sensory terminals of both large (muscle) and fine (nociceptive, touch, and temperature) fibers.
Microelectrode and gramicidin perforated-patch studies in amphibian dorsal root ganglion (Alvarez-Leefmans et al., 1988) and spinal cord (Rohrbough and Spitzer, 1996) demonstrated a high intracellular Cl− concentration in primary sensory neurons. High neuronal Cl− concentration is consistent with GABA depolarization and presynaptic inhibition (for review, see Alvarez-Leefmans et al., 1998). Accumulation of Cl− in these cells was shown to be Na+ and K+dependent and sensitive to the loop diuretic bumetanide, three hallmarks of Na–K–2Cl cotransport.
Electrically silent coupled movement of Na+, K+, and Cl− across biological membranes occurs via two isoforms of the Na–K–2Cl cotransporter: NKCC1 (Slc12a2 gene) and NKCC2 (Slc12a1 gene). NKCC2 is exclusively expressed in the kidney, whereas NKCC1 exhibits a wide pattern of expression that ranges from Cl−-secreting epithelia to red blood cells, myocytes, and neurons (for review, see Haas and Forbush, 1998; Mount et al., 1998).
Using a polyclonal antibody directed against a C-terminal region of the NKCC1 protein (Kaplan et al., 1996), we demonstrated high levels of cotransporter expression in rat dorsal root ganglion neurons (Plotkin et al., 1997a), suggesting an important role for the cotransporter in accumulating [Cl−]i in mammalian sensory neurons.
In this study, we take advantage of a mouse knock-out model of the Na–K–2Cl cotransporter that we developed recently (Delpire et al., 1999) to examine the role of the cotransporter in regulating [Cl−]i in dorsal root ganglion neurons. We show that absence of the Na–K–2Cl cotransporter dramatically affects the reversal potential of GABA-mediated responses via a significant reduction in basal Cl− concentration. The strong depolarizing responses elicited by the GABAAreceptor in control mice turned into mild depolarizing or even hyperpolarizing responses in the NKCC1 knock-out mice. In agreement with these abnormal GABA responses in sensory neurons, we show that NKCC1 knock-out mice display both abnormal gait and locomotion as well as impaired nociception phenotypes. These experiments clearly demonstrate that the Na–K–2Cl cotransporter accumulates Cl− in mouse dorsal root ganglion (DRG) neurons and that no other Cl− transport pathways are present in these cells to achieve this function. Cl−accumulation by the Na–K–2Cl cotransporter is a key component of GABA depolarization and modulation of sensory input to the spinal cord.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Na–K–2Cl cotransporter knock-out mice.Slc12a2−/ −(NKCC1−/−) mice were obtained by deletion of exon 9 of the Na–K–2Cl cotransporter gene and replacement with a β-galactosidase/neomycin fusion cassette (Delpire et al., 1999). The disrupted gene was integrated by recombination in L129 embryonic stem cells and propagated into C57b6 mice. Homozygote animals survive and exhibit multiple phenotypes. They are smaller in size, are hypotensive, present impaired epithelial Cl− secretion, and present inner ear defects resulting in significant impairment of locomotion as well as deafness (Delpire et al., 1999; Flagella et al., 1999). Animals used in this study were 6–12 weeks old.
Immunofluorescence. Dorsal root ganglia, isolated from control and homozygote mice, were fixed overnight at 4°C with 4% paraformaldehyde, washed with PBS, and cryoprotected with 30% sucrose. For indirect immunofluorescence, 7-μm-thick frozen sections were thaw mounted on Superfrost Plus slides. The sections were treated with 1% SDS and 8% 2-mercaptoethanol for 5 min and then washed in PBS and 1% BSA at room temperature for 30 min, followed by incubation with affinity-purified antibody (Kaplan et al., 1996; Plotkin et al., 1997a,b) at a dilution of 1:200 in PBS and 1% BSA overnight at 4°C. After washes in PBS, slides were incubated with indocarbocyanine-conjugated goat anti-rabbit IgG (Jackson ImmunoResearch, West Grove, PA) diluted 1:800 in PBS and 1% BSA for 1 hr at room temperature, washed with PBS, and mounted with Vectashield (Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA). Sections were analyzed with a Nikon Eclipse E800 microscope equipped with an Optronics DEI-750 color CCD camera (Optronics Engineering, Goleta, CA) coupled to an IBM-compatible computer. Photographs were obtained with a color Tektronix Phaser 450 printer (Tektronics, Wilsonwill, OR).
DRG neuron isolation. Wild-type and homozygote mice were killed by cervical dislocation. DRGs were dissected from the lower thoracic to midlumbar regions of the vertebral column and placed in oxygenated DMEM (Life Technologies, Gaithersburg, MD). The connective tissue sheath around the ganglia was removed, and DRGs were minced two to three times with iridectomy scissors. The minced DRGs were then placed in a flask containing 5 ml of DMEM containing 1 mg/ml collagenase D (Boehringer Mannheim, Indianapolis, IN), 0.6 mg/ml deoxyribonuclease I (DNase; Sigma, Saint Louis, MO), and 0.6 mg/ml trypsin (Sigma). DRGs were incubated at 37°C in a shaking water bath for 30–40 min. At the end of the incubation period, 1 mg/ml soybean trypsin inhibitor (Sigma) was added, and individual neurons were isolated from the ganglia by vigorous shaking. Neurons were kept at room temperature (20–22°C) in the DMEM solution and were aliquoted as needed to 35 mm culture dishes.
Recording. Electrophysiological responses were recorded using the gramicidin perforated-patch whole-cell recording technique with an Axopatch 1D amplifier (Axon Instruments, Foster City, CA) and pClamp 6 software (Axon Instruments). DRG neurons were allowed to settle in a 35 mm culture dish placed onto an inverted microscope (Zeiss). After attachment to the dish (30 min), an external solution was perfused at a rate of 1.5–2.0 ml/min. The solution contained 150 mm NaCl, 5 mm KCl, 0.5 mmCaCl2, 1 mmMgCl2, 10 mm glucose, and 10 mm HEPES, pH 7.4. Osmolarity was adjusted to 340 mmol/kg with sucrose. Gramicidin perforated whole-cell voltage-clamp recordings were obtained from DRG neurons with capacitances ranging from 15 to 45 pF. Patch pipettes with resistances of 2–4 MΩ were made from borosilicate glass capillaries. The pipette tip was initially filled with gramicidin-free pipette solution by brief immersion, and the remainder of the pipette was back-filled with a 310 mOsm internal solution containing gramicidin plus 140 mm CsCl (or 140 mm KCl, when indicated), 5 mm EGTA, and 10 mm HEPES, pH 7.4. A 50 mg/ml stock solution of gramicidin (Calbiochem, La Jolla, CA) was prepared in dimethylsulfoxide (DMSO; Sigma). Gramicidin was diluted into the pipette solution to a final concentration of 100 μg/ml before use.
The junction potential between the patch pipette and the bath solutions was nulled before the gigaohm seal was formed. After the formation of a tight seal, the progress of gramicidin perforation was evaluated by monitoring the capacitative current transient produced by a 10 msec hyperpolarizing voltage step (5 mV) from the holding potential (−50 mV) every 30 sec. With 100 μg/ml gramicidin in the pipette, the access resistance dropped to <20 MΩ within 20 min after seal formation. GABA and other drugs were applied by gravity-driven perfusion as described previously (Choi and Lovinger, 1996). Drugs were dissolved in external solution and delivered from a linear array of microcapillary tubes (0.32 mm inner diameter). The tips of the drug application pipettes were placed within 100 μm of the neurons. For pretreatment with bumetanide or high potassium, solutions were delivered continuously over the cells for 10 min before application of GABA. GABA (100 μm to 1 mm) was applied with the membrane potential held at a series of set values.
The peak amplitude of GABA-activated current was calculated by taking the difference between the current amplitude during the predrug application baseline period and the amplitude at the maximal negativity during drug application. Amplitude measurements were performed by the use of the cursor-based system in the pClamp software. Peak current responses for each voltage were plotted three times, and the data were fit by the use of linear regression analysis. The reversal potential of GABA-activated current (EGABA) was extracted from the x-intercept value of the fit. All averaged values are expressed as the mean ± SE. Statistical significance was determined by the use of Student's t test with the criterion set at p < 0.05. The intracellular Cl− concentration was estimated by solving for [Cl−]i in the Nernst equation: ECl = RT/Fln [Cl−]i/[Cl−]o, using the measured EGABA to estimateECl, the usual value ofRT/F, and the [Cl−]oconcentration set in the experiment.
Behavioral testing. Subjects were 51 mice (22 NKCC1+/+, 15 NKCC1+/−, and 14 NKCC1−/−). Mice were group-housed, three to five per cage, and separated by gender. Food and water were available ad libitum. The colony room was maintained on a 12:12 hr light/dark cycle, with lights on at 6 A.M. All procedures were approved by the Vanderbilt University Medical Center Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee.
For the general neurological screen, a subset of tests from the Irwin Gross Neurological Screen (Irwin, 1968) was performed. First, physical characteristics such as coat color and consistency, skin color, appearance of fur, presence of whiskers, and wounds are examined. Then, the mice are weighed, and their body temperature is measured by the use of a rectal probe. The mice are then placed in an empty cage for 3 min, and the responses to the novel environment, such as wild running, excessive grooming or freezing, abnormal gait or postures, piloerection, palpebral closure, tail elevation, urination, and defecation, are observed and noted. For touch sensitivity, mice are stroked with a finger from above, starting light and getting firmer, and the intensity at which mice try to escape is recorded. Then, whisker, righting, reaching, and corneal reflexes are examined. Abdominal and limb tone, as well as pupillary contraction and responses to tail pinch and toe pinch, is also recorded. The Irwin test battery consists of assessments of body temperature and spontaneous behavior in a novel environment, as well as gross measures of vision, hearing, and nociception. Reflexes such as righting, touch escape, trunk curl, reaching, vibrissal, and corneal were also assessed.
The ability to maintain balance on a rotating cylinder was measured with a standard rotorod apparatus (model 7650; Ugo Basile, Varese, Italy). The cylinder was 3.2 cm in diameter and was covered with scored plastic. Mice were confined to a section of the cylinder 6.0 cm long by gray Plexiglas dividers. Each mouse was placed on the cylinder, which increased rotation speed over a 5 min period from 4 to 40 rpm. The latency at which the mouse fell off the rotating cylinder was measured. Mice that fell in <15 sec were given a second trial. Mice that did not fall during the 300 sec trial period were removed and given a score of 300 sec. Mice were tested three times daily for 3 d.
For nociception threshold measurement, each mouse was placed on a hot plate maintained at 52°C (model 35D; IITC Corporation, Woodland Hills, CA). A plastic cylinder 15 cm in diameter and 12.5 cm high confined the mouse to the surface of the hot plate. The time it took the mouse to jump or lick its paw was measured with a stop watch. Mice were then immediately removed from the hot plate. Because NKCC1 knock-out mice are spastic and may not be able to lick their paws as deftly as wild-type mice, changes in other behaviors thought to be indicative of a nociceptive response were also recorded. Shaking or lifting paws, increasing motor activity, and jumping were all considered responses to the thermal stimulus for all three genotypes.
Analyses of the independence of observations of the hot plate behaviors were performed using χ2 and Cramer's φ, a transformation of χ2 that results in a coefficient ranging from 0 to 1 representing the strength of association (Welkowitz et al., 1982). Hot plate latencies were analyzed by the use of ANOVA. Planned follow-up comparisons were conducted with Fisher's protected least significant difference (PLSD) statistic after a significant omnibus F ratio. Analysis of rotorod data was conducted by the use of repeated-measures ANOVA (RMANOVA). Follow-up analyses of rotorod data were made with two-group RMANOVA. Degrees of freedom for RMANOVAs were adjusted for sphericity by the use of the Greenhouse-Geisser ε.
RESULTS
Gramicidin perforated-patch (PP) recordings were successfully performed in a total of 48 DRG neurons isolated from 15 wild-type mice and 48 neurons isolated from 9 homozygote NKCC1 knock-out mice. The whole-cell capacitance of these neurons ranged from 13.2 to 57 pF, with several neurons from both wild-type and NKCC1−/− mice having capacitances in the 15–20, 20–30, and 30–50 pF ranges, indicating that the cells we examined spanned the entire range of sizes of primary sensory neurons. There was no significant difference in the whole-cell capacitances when comparing the wild-type and NKCC1−/−mice [average capacitance = 29.5 ± 1.1 pF for wild-type mouse neurons (n = 39) and 26.9 ± 1.5 pF for NKCC1−/− mice (n = 39);p = 0.15, unpaired t test].
Application of 1 mm GABA to the neurons during recordings from wild-type mice elicited inward current when the membrane potential was voltage-clamped at −50 mV in the large majority of cells (Fig.1A). Inward current was also observed after GABA application at −30 mV, but current reversed to an outward direction when the membrane potential was clamped at −10 mV in all DRG neurons tested (Fig. 1A). When 10 μm bumetanide was applied before and during GABA application, outward current was elicited after GABA exposure at −30 mV (Fig. 1B). Similar reversal potentials and effects of bumetanide were observed in 12 neurons from eight wild-type mice.
Subsequent to completion of recordings in the PP mode, we applied additional negative pressure to the recording pipette to rupture the cell membrane for whole-cell recording in the same DRG neurons. The intracellular solution contained a high CsCl concentration (140 mm). We allowed at least 5 min after rupture of the membrane before initiating GABA application in these whole-cell recordings to allow the high CsCl-containing solution to permeate the cell. We obtained successful whole-cell recordings in nine neurons in this experiment, and GABA-activated current was inward at all negative membrane potentials and outward at positive membrane potentials (Fig.1C). Figure 1D presents the current–voltage (I/V) relationship for responses to 1 mm GABA under PP and whole-cell recording conditions for the recordings from the same neuron that generated the responses shown in Figure 1A–C. It can be seen that the reversal potential for GABA-activated current is shifted to a slightly more negative value in the presence of bumetanide relative to that in the bumetanide-free condition (control = −31.3 ± 3 mV; bumetanide = −37.2 ± 2.9 mV; n = 12 cells per group; p < 0.05, unpaired ttest). However, it should be noted that the amplitude of GABA-activated current was reduced in the presence of bumetanide even at membrane potentials at which the current should be larger (e.g., −10 mV) if the only effect of the drug was to alter the reversal potential for the GABA-gated channel. This decrease in the amplitude of GABA-activated current at all membrane potentials reflects a change in the whole-cell GABAA receptor-channel conductance (Fig.2). Similar direct effects on GABAA receptor function have been observed previously in the presence of high concentrations of the loop diuretic furosemide (Nicoll, 1978; Inomata et al., 1988; Pearce, 1993; Korpi and Luddens, 1997). It can also be seen that the reversal potential moved to ∼0 mV during whole-cell recording in this cell. The reversal potential for GABA-activated current averaged −31.3 ± 3 mV (n = 12) during gramicidin PP recordings, −37.2 ± 3 mV (n = 12) during gramicidin PP recordings in the presence of bumetanide, and 0.6 ± 3 mV (n = 9) during whole-cell recordings in a subset of this group of neurons.
When 100 μm picrotoxin was applied before and during GABA application in gramicidin PP experiments, a large reduction in the amplitude of GABA-gated current was observed (Fig.1E). Similar results were observed in five neurons, and the average reduction in current amplitude in the presence of 50–100 μm picrotoxin was 65 ± 11%. Picrotoxin alone did not elicit any current.
The observation that bumetanide and other loop diuretics have direct actions on GABAA receptors raises some concern about the interpretation of experimental results generated with these reagents. Furthermore, these inhibitors only selectively block Na–K–2Cl cotransport over a limited concentration range and may have actions on other Cl− transporters at drug concentrations that are required to block the cotransporter completely (Russell, 2000). Thus, it is important to use alternative experimental approaches to determine the role of the Na–K–2Cl cotransporter in setting the Cl−equilibrium potential in neurons.
An experimental approach that would provide a strong test of the hypothesis that the cotransporter plays a role in setting the transmembrane Cl− gradient in DRG neurons would be to remove expression of the transporter altogether. This was accomplished by disrupting the cotransporter gene and thus generating the NKCC1 knock-out mouse (Delpire et al., 1999). Using a rabbit polyclonal antibody directed against a specific C-terminal epitope of the cotransporter (Kaplan et al., 1996), we demonstrated high expression levels of the cotransporter in wild-type DRG neurons (Fig.3A) and confirmed the absence of cotransporter expression in the NKCC1 homozygote mutants (Fig. 3B).
We then performed gramicidin perforated-patch recording experiments in DRG neurons isolated from wild-type and NKCC1 knock-out mice. We first wanted to determine whether the depolarizing effect of GABA normally observed in DRG neurons would be impaired in the NKCC1 knock-out mouse neurons. Using our standard CsCl/gramicidin PP recording conditions, we observed that EGABA was consistently more depolarized in DRG neurons from wild-type compared with neurons from NKCC1 knock-out mice. Figure 4illustrates representative current traces (Fig. 4A,B) and representative current–voltage (I/V) relationships (Fig. 4C) for GABA-activated current in wild-type and NKCC1 knock-out mouse DRG neurons. When recording under our initial gramicidin PP conditions (5 mm[K]o and Cs-based internal solution), the reversal potential averaged −37 ± 2.7 mV in wild-type mouse neurons (n = 35 cells) and −53 ± 2 mV in NKCC1 knock-out mice (n = 39 cells), which correspond to intracellular Cl− concentrations of 46 ± 4 and 24 ± 2 mm, respectively. Whole-cell capacitance and EGABA were not significantly correlated in neurons from either wild-type or NKCC1−/− mice. We also measured the reversal potential of GABA-activated current during whole-cell recordings performed subsequent to completing gramicidin PP recordings in the wild-type and knock-out mouse neurons. Reversal potentials were near 0 mV for neurons from both groups of mice, and no significant difference between the two groups was observed [wild type = 2 ± 1.3 mV (n = 24); knock-out = −1 ± 1.3 mV (n = 26); p = 0.082, unpairedt test]. This finding supports the idea that the difference in reversal potentials observed under the gramicidin PP condition was caused by a difference in the transmembrane Cl− gradient and not caused by a difference in the ion permeability of the GABAAreceptor-linked channel. The density of GABA-activated current in NKCC1 knock-out mouse neurons was similar to that measured in neurons from wild-type mice [8.01 + 1.16 pA/pF (n = 26 cells, 9 mice) for homozygote vs 10.22 + 1.49 pA/pF (n = 26 cells, 12 mice) for wild type; p > 0.2,t test, NS). Furthermore, the whole-cell GABA-activated conductance was similar in wild-type and knock-out mice [2.9 ± 0.83 nS (n = 12) vs 2.98 ± 0.67 nS (n = 11); p = 0.93, unpairedt test]. Thus, elimination of the transporter does not appear to have altered expression or function of GABAA receptor channels.
To determine whether application of GABA would produce depolarization or hyperpolarization at the resting membrane potential, we recorded the resting potential (0 current level) just after the initiation of PP recordings using a KCl-based intracellular solution (see Materials and Methods). We then voltage-clamped the membrane potential at the level of the measured resting potential and applied 1 mm GABA to activate the receptors. Application of GABA at the resting potential in DRG neurons from wild-type mice produced a substantial inward current, whereas GABA application at the resting membrane potential in DRG neurons from NKCC1 knock-out mice produced little or no net current flow, on average. We observed inward current at the resting potential in all of the six neurons from wild-type mice, but neurons from NKCC1 knock-out mice exhibited small-amplitude inward current at the resting potential in four of the six neurons examined, whereas the two remaining neurons responded to GABA with outward current. We determined the reversal potential for GABA-induced current in these neurons and compared this value with the resting membrane potential measured in the same neurons. These data are summarized in Figure 4D. This figure plots the difference betweenEGABA and the resting membrane potential (RMP) with the resting membrane potential for each cell shown on the x-axis. It can be seen from this graph thatEGABA was considerably positive to the RMP in almost all of the neurons from wild-type mice, whereasEGABA was observed at membrane potentials either slightly depolarized or slightly hyperpolarized relative to the RMP in NKCC1 knock-out mouse neurons. The averaged values for all of these neurons are shown in Figure4D, inset. This figure also shows that the RMP values for the different neurons did not differ systematically between wild-type and NKCC1 knock-out mouse neurons. The average resting membrane potential did not differ significantly in knock-out mouse neurons in comparison with wild-type mouse neurons [RMP = −68 ± 2 mV in wild-type mouse cells (n = 6) and −62 ± 3.3 mV in cells lacking NKCC1 (n = 6);p = 0.144, unpaired t test].
Elimination of NKCC1 expression should eliminate the bumetanide-induced hyperpolarizing shift in EGABA by eliminating the major diuretic-sensitive mechanism for Cl− accumulation in the neuron. Thus, we examined the effect of application of 10 μmbumetanide on current activated by GABA and the reversal potential of this current in DRG neurons from wild-type and NKCC1 mice. Figure5A shows that the reversal potential for GABA-gated Cl− current was actually shifted to a more depolarized potential during bumetanide application to DRG neurons from NKCC1 knock-out mice (control = −55.5 ± 3.7 mV; bumetanide = −47 ± 2.5 mV;p < 0.05, unpaired t test). This is the opposite of what is observed in neurons from wild-type mice as shown in Figures 1 and 5A. Thus, a bumetanide-sensitive mechanism for decreasing intracellular Cl− likely exists in DRG neurons. This mechanism is normally overshadowed by the activity of the Na–K–2Cl cotransporter but is unmasked when the inward cotransport mechanism is removed. The decrease in the GABA-activated conductance in the presence of bumetanide was observed even in the NKCC1 knock-out mouse neurons [2.98 ± 0.67 nS (n = 11) for untreated knock-out cells and 1.67 ± 0.33 nS (n = 11) for bumetanide-treated knock-out cells; p < 0.05], and this decrease was similar in magnitude to that observed in wild-type mice. This observation further supports a direct effect of the diuretic on the GABAA receptor complex.
Because cation–Cl− cotransporters are sensitive to extracellular cation concentrations, we examined the effect of altering extracellular K+ on the reversal potential of GABA-activated current to determine whether this manipulation produced a differential effect in wild-type versus NKCC1 knock-out mice. We observed that increasing the K+ concentration from 5 to 10 mm caused a shift in EGABAto more depolarized potentials that was approximately equivalent in both wild-type and knock-out mouse DRG neurons (Figure 5B). This finding indicates that a K-sensitive transporter is acting to lower ECl in the absence or presence of NKCC1. This observation also supports the idea that DRG neurons contain cation–Cl− cotransporters other than NKCC1.
We also lowered the extracellular Cl−concentration by reducing the NaCl concentration in our extracellular solution from 150 to 100 mm, substituting the remaining 50 mm with Na-gluconate. This treatment should reduce the inward transport mediated by NKCC1 and result in a reduction in intracellular Cl− concentration. TheEGABA values were shifted to slightly more depolarized values in both the wild-type and NKCC1 knock-out mouse neurons (from −34 ± 2 to −29 ± 2 mV in wild-type neurons and from −52 ± 5 to −45 ± 5 mV in NKCC1 knock-out neurons) after reducing [Cl−]o. In both sets of mice these shifts in EGABAwere statistically significant (p < 0.05, repeated measures t test). This is to be expected because of the change in Cl−-driving force produced by this treatment. Using these measuredEGABA values, we were then able to calculate the [Cl−]i value by the use of the Nernst equation (see Materials and Methods). Figure5C shows that the reduction in [Cl−]o produced a significant decrease in [Cl−]i in DRG neurons from wild-type but not knock-out mice (p< 0.05 for wild-type data, p > 0.2 for NKCC1 knock-out data, repeated measures t test). This finding is consistent with the idea that the intracellular Cl− concentration in DRG neurons is sensitive to [Cl−]o only when NKCC1 is present. Figure 5C also shows that [Cl−]i was lower in the NKCC1 knock-out mouse neurons in comparison with wild-type mouse neurons under both the normal and reduced [Cl−]oconditions, consistent with the outcome of previous experiments indicating that loss of NKCC1 reduces the Cl− concentration in DRG neurons. As expected, [Cl−]imeasured during whole-cell recording in the reduced [Cl−]o condition did not differ in wild-type compared with knock-out mouse neurons (data not shown).
To relate these abnormal GABAergic responses in DRG neurons to proprioception, we subjected the mice to a series of behavioral tests, starting with a gross behavioral analysis. To assess gross neurological function in the knock-out mice, a subset of tests from the Irwin (1968)screen was performed. The Irwin screen is a comprehensive set of assessment tools for detecting neurological and behavioral abnormalities in mice. The Irwin screen is the standard basic test battery used to control for procedural abilities (e.g., walking) required in more complex behavioral tests (e.g., memory) in both behavioral pharmacology and behavioral genetics (Irwin, 1968;Banfi et al., 1982; Hunter et al., 2000). The Irwin screen has been incorporated into or is the basis for a number of alternate behavioral test batteries (Crawley and Paylor, 1997; Rogers et al., 1997). Perhaps the most salient behavior observed during the gross neurological screen was the spontaneous retropulsive response to a novel environment on the part of the knock-out mice. In this test, mice were placed in a clean mouse tub cage and observed for a period of 3 min. Retropulsion was immediate after placement into the tub cage and was characterized by rapidly walking backward until reaching the edge of the cage and continuing until the rear legs were propped up on the side of the cage. The mice then maintained a vertical, head-down position, although spastic movements continued. After a short period in this vertical position, knock-out mice began moving again and exploring the rest of the cage. In addition to this behavior, knock-out mice appeared to struggle more when touched or restrained and when held aloft by the tail, although this may be a manifestation of their spasticity. Other behaviors assessed during the gross neurological screen were normal, including all reflexes tested, toe and tail pinch, and muscle tone.
The rotorod performance of the knock-out mice was severely impaired [F(2,48) = 53.0; p < 0.0001], as depicted in Figure 6. Follow-up analyses demonstrate that the knock-out mice fell off the rotorod much sooner than did both the wild-type [F(1,34) = 101.9; p< 0.0001] and heterozygous [F(1,27)= 92.5; p < 0.0001] mice. Interestingly, the rotorod performance of the knock-out mice improved significantly over the nine testing sessions [ε = 0.282;F(2,29) = 3.3; p = .0023], increasing from a mean of 15–18 sec on day 1 to 30–39 sec on day 3. This demonstrates that the knock-out mice retain some ability to learn motor skills in spite of their debilitating spasticity. The rotorod performance of wild-type and heterozygous mice did not differ significantly [F(1,35) = 1.0;p = 0.335] .
The types of behavior that the mice exhibited in response to pain differed significantly across genotype (df = 6; χ2 = 25.1; φ = 0.646;p = 0.0003). Wild-type (df = 1; χ2 = 11.8; φ = 0.789;p = 0.0006) and heterozygous (df = 1; χ2 = 12.7; φ = 0.798;p = 0.0004) mice were more likely to exhibit guarding behavior (lifting or shaking the paw), whereas the knock-out mice were more likely to increase locomotor activity in response to the thermal stimulus. There was no significant difference across genotypes in the proportion of mice that exhibited alternate behavior (i.e., other than licking) after being placed on the hot plate (df = 2; χ2 = 2.6; φ = 0.226;p = 0.278) or in the topography of nociceptive responding between wild-type and heterozygous mice (df = 2; χ2 = 0.9; φ = 0.2;p = 0.645).
Knock-out mice exhibited impaired pain perception on the hot plate, as demonstrated by longer response latencies [Fig.7;F(2,43) = 4.3; p = 0.0196]. Follow-up analyses showed that the knock-out mice were significantly less sensitive to the thermal stimulus compared with both the wild-type (PLSD = 4.72; p = 0.0054) and heterozygous (PLSD = 5.02; p = 0.0466) groups. The mean response latency of the heterozygous mice did not differ from that of the wild-type mice (PLSD = 4.16; p = 0.3974). Four knock-out mice were excluded from the analysis of hot plate latencies because they exhibited spontaneous retropulsion after being placed on the hot plate. Because this retropulsive response was immediate and occurred long before the typical latencies of other nociceptive responses in any of the mice, it was impossible to distinguish whether the retropulsion was in response to the thermal stimulus or in response to a novel environment, as observed in the gross neurological screen.
In spite of differences in behaviors in response to the thermal stimulus, all but five mice (three wild types and two knock-outs) also licked their paws when exposed to the high temperature, including two of the four knock-out mice that retropulsed and whose rear paws were in contact with the acrylic restraining wall and not with the hot plate. When only latency to lick was considered, knock-out mice were still significantly impaired relative to wild-type and heterozygous mice [F(2,43) = 5.9; p = 0.0054].
DISCUSSION
Removal of NKCC1 reduces [Cl−]i in mouse DRG neurons (Fig. 4), supporting the idea that this cotransporter plays a major role in determining the transmembrane Cl− concentration gradient in these cells. The experiments involving bumetanide application and manipulation of ion concentrations further support this idea. The predominant effect of bumetanide in wild-type DRG neurons is to shiftEGABA to more hyperpolarized potentials, indicating a decrease in [Cl−]i. In addition, reducing [Cl−]o reduces [Cl−]i in wild-type neurons, consistent with a reduction in activity of an inward Cl− transporter such as NKCC1. Both of these effects are lost in the NKCC1 knock-out mouse, strongly implicating this transporter in concentrating Cl− inside mouse DRG neurons under normal conditions. These data are consistent with those obtained from frog dorsal root ganglion neurons by the use of ion-selective microelectrodes or from Amphiuma Rohon-Beard spinal neurons by the use of the gramicidin perforated-patch method, which clearly indicated accumulation of Cl− via a bumetanide-sensitive, Na+- and K+-dependent Na–K–2Cl cotransporter (Alvarez-Leefmans et al., 1988; Rohrbough and Spitzer, 1996)
Our findings also suggest that another transporter, in addition to NKCC1, regulates intracellular Cl−concentration in mouse DRG neurons. We observed a bumetanide-induced depolarizing shift in EGABA in the NKCC1 knock-out mouse neurons that was not observed in neurons from wild-type animals. This suggests that bumetanide inhibits a transporter, other than NKCC1, that mediates Cl− extrusion from these neurons. Further support for involvement of another transporter comes from the observation that altering [K+]o altersEGABA, and thus [Cl−]i, to a similar extent in both wild-type and knock-out mouse neurons. One likely candidate transporter that could mediate this action is a K–Cl cotransporter. Indeed, the effect of increasing [K+]o is consistent with a role for this transporter because this treatment would be expected to increase [Cl−]i if this transporter is present, and our results indicate that such an increase actually takes place. Four isoforms of the K–Cl cotransporter have been cloned with one, KCC2, being expressed exclusively in neurons. Using PCR and immunofluorescence, we have demonstrated previously the presence of KCC2 in DRG neurons (Lu et al., 1999). Other isoforms of the K–Cl cotransporter might also be expressed in these cells. This K–Cl cotransporter may function in wild-type DRG, and its action would tend to oppose that of NKCC1.
Interestingly, Ehrlich et al. (1999) have observed a positive shift inEglycine induced by furosemide in postnatal day 8 (P8)–P10 lateral superior olive neurons, instead of the negative shift observed in younger P2–P4 neurons. The positive shift in mature neurons suggests inhibition of a net outward Cl− transport mechanism, likely KCC2, whereas the negative shift in glycine reversal potential observed in immature neurons indicates inhibition of a net inward Cl− transport system, likely to be the Na–K–2Cl cotransporter. Unlike the case in CNS neurons, it appears that the function of NKCC1 dominates Cl−transport in wild-type DRG neurons because bumetanide produces a hyperpolarizing shift in EGABA when NKCC1 is present and EGABA is shifted to more hyperpolarized values when NKCC1 is removed. This strong NKCC1 inward Cl− transport may overshadow a smaller bumetanide-sensitive component of transport.
The observed actions of bumetanide in the absence of NKCC1 suggest that some caution should be exercised in interpreting results from experiments examining the effect of this drug and other loop diuretics on transmembrane Cl− concentration and the physiological roles of distribution of this ion. The limitations of the usefulness of loop diuretics in studies examining GABAA receptor function and transmembrane Cl− distribution are further demonstrated by the past and present findings that these compounds inhibit the conductance of GABAA receptor channels (Figs.1D, 2) (Nicoll, 1978; Inomata et al., 1988; Pearce, 1993; Korpi and Luddens, 1997). These problems with the experimental use of bumetanide (or furosemide) highlight the importance of the genetic model that we have used in the present study. Selective knock-out of the NKCC1 gene provides strong support, independent of pharmacological approaches, for a role of this transporter in regulation of the neuronal Cl− gradient. Using this method, we have now firmly established that NKCC1 activity strongly contributes to the depolarizing effects of GABA in primary sensory neurons.
As alluded to previously, the functional data obtained in sensory DRG neurons can very likely be extended to immature CNS neurons (cortical and hippocampal) that have intracellular Cl− concentrations higher than the electrochemical potential equilibrium and exhibit GABA- or glycine-depolarizing responses (Ben-Ari et al., 1989; Owens et al., 1996; Ehrlich et al., 1999). These GABA- or glycine-depolarizing responses observed in immature neurons coincide with high NKCC1 expression (Plotkin et al., 1997b). Hyperpolarizing GABA or glycine responses develop shortly after birth in concert with a decrease in intracellular Cl−concentration because of a simultaneous decrease of NKCC1 expression (Plotkin et al., 1997b) and increase in KCC2 expression (Clayton et al., 1998; Lu et al., 1999).
This study did not reveal the existence of alternative pathways for Cl− accumulation. However, our experiments were performed in the absence of bicarbonate. Thus, we cannot exclude the possibility that, in the homozygote, bicarbonate could compensate and affect the direction and extent of GABA responses in vivo. It is important to note, however, that in CNS neurons such as GABAergic sensorimotor cortical neurons (Owens et al., 1996) and glycinergic lateral superior olive neurons (Ehrlich et al., 1999), furosemide induced a marked negative shift in the GABA or glycine reversal potential in the presence of bicarbonate or CO2, suggesting that Cl− accumulation via the cotransporter is indeed the basis for depolarizing GABA or glycine responses in immature neurons and that inhibition of this pathway does not result in any compensation by bicarbonate. Furthermore, we have shown that the absence of depolarizing GABA currents in homozygote NKCC1 mutant DRG neurons results in sensory perception defects, indicating one potential role of Cl− regulation via the Na–K–2Cl cotransporter in the somatosensory system.
GABAA-mediated primary afferent depolarization and presynaptic inhibition are known to regulate the excitability of sensory terminals of both large (muscle) and fine (pain, temperature, and touch receptors) afferent fibers. Abnormal synaptic transmission of type 1A afferent fibers to spinal cord motor neurons is likely to result in gait and movement coordination phenotypes. Spasticity and locomotion phenotypes definitely exist in the homozygote NKCC1 mutants. Knock-out mice were unable to remain on the rotorod for a substantial period of time because of motor uncoordination and spasticity (Fig. 6). Despite these differences, the knock-out mice made significant improvements over the course of the nine rotorod sessions. These results are consistent with the sensory neuron Na–K–2Cl cotransporter playing an important role in motor coordination, but other factors are involved as well. For instance, it is difficult at this stage to sort out the contribution of the spinal cord and of the inner ear in the uncoordination phenotype. We have reported previously that disruption of the secretory Na–K–2Cl cotransporter gene results in anatomical defects of the inner ear associated with an absence of K+ secretion. These defects result in deafness and abnormal control of balance phenotypes (Delpire et al., 1999) that could contribute to poor rotorod performance.
Type Aδ and type C fibers are both involved in the perception of pain. Abnormal synaptic transmission in these fibers is likely to affect pain threshold. Because of the wide range of capacitance values for the neurons from which we recorded, it is likely that we examined somata of Aδ and C type neurons. Using the standard hot plate test to evaluate the pain threshold, we have shown that homozygote NKCC1 mutant mice display a significantly higher pain threshold when compared with their control or heterozygote littermates (Fig. 7). This result cannot be attributed to a complete absence of sensory perception or to a general apathy and lack of reflexes because the mice passed the general tests involved in the gross neurological screen. Therefore, this result clearly demonstrates that elimination of the Na–K–2Cl cotransporter results in alteration of the processing of sensory signals. On the basis of the premise that primary afferent depolarization and presynaptic inhibition filter sensory noise, we propose that NKCC1 knock-out animals likely experience decreased sensory contrast resulting in a higher pain threshold. Our findings do not provide evidence of a strong causal link between the change in the sensory neuron transmembrane Cl− gradient and the increase in pain threshold. However, these observations indicate that the NKCC1 knock-out mice do experience alterations in pain perception that are consistent with what would be expected after alteration of GABAergic effects at primary afferent synapses. The change in pain thresholds that we observed is not caused by generalized hypokinesia or hypoflexia because the animals show vigorous retropulsive movement and reflexive response patterns. Interestingly, our data differ somewhat from experiments showing that intrathecal administration of GABA and GABAA receptor agonists increases the nociceptive threshold in rats (Hammond and Drower, 1984; Roberts et al., 1986) whereas bicuculline, a GABAAreceptor antagonist, results in profound agitation. However, these studies examine the short-term effect of applying pharmacological agents in the spinal cord that can affect both postsynaptic and presynaptic neurons, and it is not clear whether the NKCC1 knock-out affects postsynaptic GABAergic responses in the spinal cord. In agreement with our data, these studies stress the important role of GABAA-mediated transmission in peripheral sensation. In light of the nociception phenotype, it will be important to examine in future studies presynaptic inhibition in the NKCC1 knock-out mouse.
Footnotes
- Received May 23, 2000.
- Revision received July 13, 2000.
- Accepted August 9, 2000.
This work was supported in part by the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke Grant NS 36758, by a Vanderbilt University Medical Center Intramural Discovery Grant to E.D., and by the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism Grant AA 08986 to D.M.L. K.-W.S. was supported by a Korean Science and Engineering Foundation (KOSEF) grant. E.D. is an Established Investigator of the American Heart Association.
Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Eric Delpire, Anesthesiology Research Division, Department of Anesthesiology, Vanderbilt University, T-4202, Medical Center North, Nashville, TN 37232-2520. E-mail: eric.delpire{at}mcmail.vanderbilt.edu.
- Copyright © 2000 Society for Neuroscience