Abstract
Recent physiological studies show that neural responses correlated with the perception of brightness are found in cortical area V1 but not earlier in the visual pathway (Kayama et al., 1979; Reid and Shapley, 1989; Squatrito et al., 1990; Komatsu et al., 1996; Rossi et al., 1996;MacEvoy et al., 1998; Rossi and Paradiso, 1999; Hung et al., 2001;Kinoshita and Komatsu, 2001; MacEvoy and Paradiso, 2001). However, these studies are based on comparisons of neural responses in animals with brightness perception in humans. Very little is known about the perception of brightness in animals typically used in physiological experiments. In this study, we quantify brightness discrimination, brightness induction, and White's effect in macaque monkeys. The results show that, qualitatively and quantitatively, the perception of brightness in macaques and humans is quite similar. This similarity may be an indication of common underlying neural computations in the two species.
- brightness
- brightness perception
- color perception
- surface perception
- macaque
- visual illusion
For the purpose of understanding the neural basis of human visual perception, the best widely used animal model is the macaque monkey. What has been learned about the macaque suggests that both its visual physiology and perception are quite similar to that of humans. However, there are large gaps in our understanding of the visual capabilities of this animal that limit our ability to relate the results of neural recordings to visual perception.
Our own studies of the neural representation of brightness information (Rossi et al., 1996; MacEvoy et al., 1998; Rossi and Paradiso, 1999;MacEvoy and Paradiso, 2001) prompted us to study brightness perception in the macaque. There are previous studies of brightness perception in monkeys going back to the early 20th century (Crawford, 1935; Ash, 1951; Davis et al., 1965; Brooks, 1966; Schilder et al., 1971), but these are of limited utility for several reasons. First, some previous studies used diverse types of monkeys other than macaques. Second, almost all of the previous studies were limited to simple discrimination; visual illusions involving brightness were generally not explored. The study of visual illusions is especially valuable for comparisons with physiological results because one can test whether neural responses correlate with the physical or perceptual properties of objects.
The third limitation of previous studies is that they often did not lead to the construction of psychometric functions as is typical in human psychophysics experiments. In relating physiology to psychophysics, psychometric functions are of great value because they can be compared with “neurometric” functions to assess whether neurons in a particular brain area carry information comparable with that used by an animal in the performance of a visual task.
With the limitations of previous animal perception experiments in mind, we set out to explore brightness perception in the macaque. Animals were trained to make discriminations in three situations: simple brightness discrimination of isolated gray patches, simultaneous brightness contrast (brightness induction), and White's effect.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
General procedures. Two adult rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) weighing 5.2 kg (monkey PN) and 4.8 kg (monkey HN) served as subjects in these experiments. Before behavioral training, aseptic surgery with isoflurane anesthesia was performed to attach a head post and place a scleral search coil on the right eye (Judge et al., 1980). All procedures conformed to National Institutes of Health guidelines and were approved by the Brown University animal care and use committee.
During training and experimental sessions, animals sat in a primate chair facing a computer display (640 × 480 pixel resolution) at a distance of 93 cm. At the beginning of each session, animals were adapted to the darkened room for 10 min. Head position was fixed, and eye position was monitored using the scleral search coil or, in some cases with animal HN, an ISCAN (Burlington, MA) infrared eye tracking system.
Brightness discrimination. Monkeys were trained on a two-alternative forced-choice task to make saccadic eye movements to the brighter of two simultaneously presented gray disks. Each disk had a radius of 1.0°, and the centers of the two disks were separated by 6.0°.
Each trial began with the appearance of a small fixation cross centered on the display (Fig.1A). Animals were required to fixate within a 1.8° diameter window for 1 sec, after which the two disks appeared. The animals had to wait until the fixation cross was extinguished (a random interval between 1.2 and 1.8 sec) and then, within 300 msec of fixation cross offset, saccade to the disk that was brighter. Animals received a liquid reward for correctly choosing the brighter patch, but only after maintaining fixation on it for 700 msec (Fig. 1B). Trials were aborted and no reward was given if the animals broke fixation at any time while the cross was present, if they attempted to “change” their decision by glancing to one disk and then the other, or if they moved their eyes to any portion of the display other than either disk after the fixation cross disappeared. To negate any bias that the monkeys might have for saccades to one side or the other, the brighter patch randomly alternated between the right and left positions (any left–right differences in monitor output were corrected by adjusting lookup table values).
When performance stabilized after several weeks of training, brightness judgments were recorded. On data collection days, monkeys completed 40–100 warm-up trials before behavioral responses were recorded.
Brightness induction. The task in the brightness induction experiment was nearly identical to that described for simple discrimination. The stimulus differed in that each gray disk was surrounded by a 1.0° thick annulus (Fig. 1C). To human observers, a gray disk surrounded by a white annulus appears darker than a gray disk surrounded by black. In each block of trials, the luminance values of one disk and its annular surround (termed the “test” disk and surround) were held constant. The test disk was fixed at 12.6 or 32.2 cd/m2, and, across blocks, the test surround had six or seven different values. In each trial, the luminance of the other disk (termed the “comparison disk”) was chosen from a set of values ranging from far below to far above that of the test disk. The luminance of the second annular surround (the “comparison” surround) was always 1.3 cd/m2. In human psychophysics experiments, subjects would typically use the method of adjustment to set the comparison disk to match the test disk. With the monkeys, it was necessary to record discriminations with a range of comparison stimuli to quantify the brightness of the test disk, because the monkeys' task was simply to saccade to the brighter disk. The timing of stimulus presentation and behavioral response was identical to that used in the brightness discrimination experiments described above.
Training the animals to perform the induction experiment required great care, because the addition of the surrounds had, at the outset, an unknown impact on the animals' perception of brightness. One approach would have been to train the animals extensively on the simple brightness discrimination task in the absence of surrounds and then add the surrounds and immediately record responses while rewarding randomly. We rejected this approach because of concerns that the animals might start guessing or base their responses on the annuli rather than the disks. Instead, we trained the animals with the surrounds present but used a reduced set of stimulus conditions in which the only possible effect of the surrounds was to exaggerate a luminance-based difference in brightness between the test and comparison disks. For example, an animal would be shown one stimulus with a dark surround and bright center along with a second stimulus having a bright surround and dark center. The animal was rewarded for choosing the more luminous center without fear of introducing a bias, because a judgment based on luminance or brightness would be the same. This approach involved the minimal assumption that the annular surround had either no effect or an inhibitory effect on the brightness of the disk but not a facilitatory effect (i.e., the surround influence is not opposite in sign for monkeys and humans). This strategy did pose the risk of training the animals to pick the side with the darker surround rather than the lighter center. To counter this, we included control conditions in which the monkey was rewarded for choosing an extremely bright center with a bright annulus over an almost black center with a dark annulus.
In the actual experiments, a wider range of center and surround luminance values was used, and we used a probabilistic reward scheme to avoid introducing a bias into the monkeys' judgments. In cases in which decisions based on luminance or brightness would unequivocally be the same, the animal was rewarded 100% for choosing the more luminous patch, as during training. In all other cases, meaning in those conditions that would make up the critical portion of the psychometric curve, the animals were randomly rewarded on 50% of the trials. To ensure that this reward scheme did not encourage random guessing, we continued to interleave conditions which verified that the monkey was basing its judgments on the brightness of the disks and not the surrounds. Overall, assuming correct performance on control trials, the monkeys received juice on 75% of trials, on average.
At least 100 trials were recorded with each test surround luminance value, and the induction experiment was repeated with six or seven different values. Before data collection, animals completed 65–145 warm-up trials.
White's effect. The stimulus pairs used to explore White's effect closely resembled those used in human psychophysical experiments. They consisted of two 0.5 cycle/° square-wave luminance gratings side-by-side. On one of the gratings, a rectangular gray patch was substituted for a portion of a black stripe, and, on the other grating, a gray patch replaced part of a white stripe (Fig.1D). The perceptual illusion seen by humans is that the gray patch with its long sides surrounded by black appears darker than the other patch with long sides surrounded by white. Thus, the illusion seems opposite that of brightness induction in which a dark surround causes a gray patch to appear lighter rather than darker.
The animals were trained to saccade to the brighter of the gray patches, either the one with long black flanks or the other surrounded by long white flanks. For brevity, we will refer to the stimulus in which a gray patch is flanked top-and-bottom by long black bands as BGB (black–gray–black) and the stimulus with gray flanked by white as WGW (white–gray–white). The luminances of the black and white flanks were fixed at 0.2 and 81 cd/m2, respectively. To study White's effect, brightness discriminations were made with the WGW gray patch fixed at either 14.1 or 31.3 cd/m2 while the luminance of the BGB patch was varied.
Our initial experiments were conducted with a fixation paradigm similar to that used in the discrimination and induction experiments. However, it appeared that the complexity of the stimulus and the peripheral placement of the gray patches made the task difficult for the monkeys. Therefore, we changed to a free-viewing paradigm that gave more reliable results by allowing the animals to look back and forth between the stimuli before rendering a decision. This is the approach used in comparable human psychophysics experiments.
Eye position traces observed during data collection indicated that the free viewing period was used by the monkeys to examine the gray patches. After 2–3 sec of free viewing, a fixation cross appeared that the animals had to foveate within a 1.8° diameter window. After holding fixation for a variable interval (1.2–1.8 sec), the fixation cross was turned off, allowing the animals to saccade to the gray patch that was perceived brighter.
Training and reinforcement were managed in a similar manner to the brightness induction task. Once again, we assumed that the surrounding stimuli had either no effect on the brightness of the gray patches or an effect with the same sign as for humans. We initially trained the animals with a narrow set of stimuli for which the only possible effect of the flanks would be to exaggerate an already-present difference in patch brightness based on luminance. Unfortunately, this approach could have allowed the animals to correctly complete the task by picking the patch with brighter flanks rather than by judging the brightness of the patches themselves. To counter this, we also trained the animals with conditions in which they were rewarded for picking a very bright patch with dark flanks over a very dark patch with bright flanks. Once the animals were trained, we interleaved experimental conditions from the narrow set for which the animals were veridically rewarded, with a wider array of flank and patch luminance values for which the animals were rewarded randomly with a probability of 50%.
RESULTS
Brightness discrimination
Figure 2A shows data collected from the two monkeys in the brightness discrimination experiment. The abscissa indicates the luminance difference between the two disks, and the data show the percentage of saccades made to the brighter, more luminous, disk. The error bars represent the SE based on at least five blocks of 20 trials each. Discriminations were made at six different mean luminance levels for monkey PN and three different levels for monkey HN. At each mean luminance, the data points form a psychometric curve for brightness discrimination. As mean luminance increased, larger luminance differences were required between the two gray patches to achieve the same level of performance. This point is emphasized in Figure 2B, which shows the luminance difference necessary for each monkey to reach 80% correct at each mean luminance. Consistent with Weber's law, the data lie along straight lines for both animals, indicating that the difference in luminance (dL) needed to perform at 80% correct is a fixed fraction of the mean luminance (L). The average value of dL/L is 0.11 ± 0.02 (mean ± SD) for monkey PN and 0.18 ± 0.03 for monkey HN.
Brightness induction
Figure 3 shows data from brightness discriminations made by the two monkeys in a stimulus situation that produces perceptual brightness induction in humans. The stimuli in this experiment consisted of test and comparison disks, each with an annular surround. The test disk luminance was fixed at 12.6 or 32.2 cd/m2, and the comparison surround was fixed at 1.3 cd/m2. To quantify brightness induction, the luminance of the comparison patch was varied with the test surround fixed at one of six to seven different luminance levels in different blocks of trials (indicated by differentsymbols in Fig. 3A).
Each curve in Figure 3A shows the percentage of saccades that the animal made to the comparison disk as a function of the difference in luminance between the comparison and test disks. For all data points to the left of the vertical line, the test disk was more luminous than the comparison disk, and, for all to the right, it was less luminous. Thehorizontal line marks 50% performance; any data point in the space above the line indicates that the monkey perceived the comparison disk to be brighter than the test disk, and, for any point below the line, the monkey perceived the comparison disk to be darker than the test disk. If there were no effect of the annular surrounds, every curve should pass through the 50% level of performance when there was no luminance difference between test and comparison patches. All of the data in Figure 3A come from experiments with a test disk luminance of 32.2 cd/m2.
We fit sigmoidal functions to the set of responses obtained with each test surround luminance, and we took the comparison luminance in which each curve crossed the horizontal line at 50% as the theoretical brightness match for the test disk with that surround intensity. For example, with animal PN (top panel), the curve drawn through open squares represents the psychometric function obtained when the test and comparison surrounds were of equal luminance. The curve crosses 50% performance when the comparison disk had the same luminance as the test disk, consistent with the fact that this special case was simple brightness discrimination. As test surround luminance increases (different symbols in the figure), the curvescross the 50% performance line when the comparison disk luminance was progressively lower than the fixed test disk luminance. In other words, test surrounds of higher luminance diminished the apparent brightness of the test patch (i.e., brightness induction). Very similar results were obtained with the second monkey (Fig. 3A, bottom panel).
Figure 3B shows the brightness matching points (obtained from the sigmoidal fits) plotted against test surround luminance. This manner of data presentation is more typical of human psychophysical experiments in which the method of adjustment is used. For monkey PN (top panel), the two curves correspond to results obtained with two different test disk luminances (32.2 and 12.6 cd/m2); monkey HN (bottom) was tested with a single test disk luminance (32.2 cd/m2). These curves confirm the trend evident in the raw data: higher values of test surround luminance elicit lower estimates of test disk brightness. Of particular note is the rapid change in the brightness matching points from test surround luminance 28.1 to 38.8 cd/m2 on the square-symbol curves in Figure 3B. These two test surround luminances span the test patch luminance of 32.2 cd/m2 and thus correspond to a switch from the patch being an increment to a decrement.
Although these data strongly suggest that monkeys perceive a disk on a dark background to be brighter than one on a bright background, the data were collected in a different manner than typical side-by-side comparisons in human induction experiments. As an additional test of the animals' perception of induction, for one monkey (PN), we included conditions that allowed a side-by-side comparison of stimuli. In other words, the monkey simultaneously saw two identical disks with different surround luminances as a human would in a typical experiment. We used surrounds only from neighboring points in Figure3B (i.e., the surround luminance from the first data point vs the second point, the second pointvs third, etc). We found that, across the six possible test surround comparisons from the top curve in Figure3B (test disk, 32.2 cd/m2), 91% of the time monkey PN judged the disk with the less luminous surround to be brighter. From the five possible comparisons in thebottom curve of Figure 3B (test disk, 12.6 cd/m2), 88% of the time the disk with the less luminous surround was judged to be brighter. Thus, these data collected with a paradigm identical to that used in human experiments show judgments consistent with the perception of brightness induction.
White's effect
Figure 4 shows psychometric functions derived from brightness judgments using a White's effect stimulus. As in the discrimination and induction experiments, animals were trained to saccade to the brighter of two stimuli. Each data point represents the percentage of saccades made to the gray patch flanked by black bars (BGB patch) as a function of the difference in luminance between the BGB gray patch and the WGW gray patch. Thevertical line indicates the condition in which the two gray patches had the same luminance. If there were no brightness illusion (and no inherent response bias), the animals should make ∼50% of their saccades to the BGB side when the gray patches are equally luminous. When the 2° long gray patches had 7° long black and white flanks (open symbols), a situation that evokes White's effect in humans, this was clearly not the case: saccades were not made equally to the two gray patches until the BGB gray patch luminance was 18.6 cd/m2 higher than the WGW patch (for animal PN) and 37.3 cd/m2higher for animal HN. We cannot tell to what extent this deviation represents a perceptual lightening of the WGW patch versus a darkening of the BGB patch. However, it is clear that, for monkeys, like humans, the patch with white flanks appears brighter than one with dark flanks. The fact that the percentage of saccades to the BGB patch changed systematically as its luminance was varied indicates that the animals were using the patch itself rather than some other configuration cue or flank intensity to make their decisions.
Conceivably, the shift of the curves to the rightof the vertical lines could be a result of response bias rather than a perceptual illusion. We examined this possibility by repeating the experiment with the black and white flanks reduced to 2° in length, matching the length of the gray patches (Fig. 4,filled data points). To humans, this is a situation that evokes brightness induction rather than White's effect. Consistent with the sign reversal in human perception, the curves shift from right of the vertical lines to theleft of the vertical lines in Figure 4. To achieve equal numbers of saccades to the two gray patches, the BGB gray patch had to be 10.7 cd/m2 lower than the WGW patch for animal PN and 5.7 cd/m2lower for animal HN. Thus, with a simple manipulation of the stimulus that reverses the perceptual effect in humans, the judgments of the monkeys also reversed. This finding strongly suggests that monkeys perceive White's effect in addition to brightness induction.
The data discussed above show that the monkeys perceived White's effect when the WGW patch luminance was 31.3 cd/m2. To test the generality of this finding, we also collected data in one monkey with a WGW patch luminance of 14.1 cd/m2. With no previous experience with this lower luminance gray patch, the monkey immediately made responses consistent with White's effect (Fig.5B). This is shown by the displacement of the dashed curves to the right of the vertical lines. With the WGW gray test patch at 14.1 cd/m2 (Fig. 5B), the BGB gray patch had to be 14.9 cd/m2 higher for the animal to make equal numbers of saccades to both patches. As already noted, with the gray test patch at 31.3 cd/m2, the BGB gray patch had to be 18.6 cd/m2 higher. For comparison, thesolid lines in Figure 5 show brightness discriminations made with no flanks.
We conducted a number of additional experiments to confirm and extend the basic findings. In demonstrations of White's effect in humans, unlike the monkey experiments reported so far here, the two gray patches typically have the same luminance. Although we needed to vary the patch luminances to quantify White's effect in monkeys, we confirmed our results with a configuration identical to that used in human demonstrations. The BGB and WGW gray patches were given identical luminances of 8.5, 14.1, 21.7, or 31.3 cd/m2, values that all evoke White's effect in humans. Consistent with the human results, animal PN made 100, 99, 91, and 96% of her saccades to the WGW gray patch, indicating that it appeared brighter, although it actually had the same luminance as the BGB patch.
The results shown in Figure 6 suggest part of the mechanism responsible for White's effect. The difference between the stimuli in Figure 6A and6B is that the band collinear with the gray patch (and the other light bands on this stimulus) on the BGB side is 81.0 cd/m2 in Figure 6A but 3.6 cd/m2 in Figure 6B. To humans, the perceptual effect of this difference is that, with the same luminance, the BGB gray patch appears lighter in Figure6B in which the bands collinear with the gray patch are darker (Spehar et al., 1995). As the figure indicates, the monkey also made a much higher percentage of saccades (100%) to the BGB gray patch with the stimulus in Figure 6B than in Figure6A (65%). Thus, for both monkeys and humans, darkening the bands collinear with the gray patch makes the gray patch appear relatively brighter. In other words, the collinear flanks produce an induction effect.
Comparing Figure 6A with 6C, the difference is that the WGW flanks in Figure 6A have a luminance of 81.0 cd/m2 and, in Figure6C, they have a luminance of 3.6 cd/m2. Note that this is the same luminance difference as between Figure 6A and6B, except here the flanks on the WGW side were changed rather than the bands collinear with the gray patch on the BGB side. The human perceptual effect of darkening the WGW flanks is to make the WGW gray patch lighter. Consistently, in the monkey tested, darker WGW flanks led to an increase in the percentage of saccades to the WGW gray patch (Fig. 6, from 35% in A to 47% inC). Thus, an induction effect is produced by the long flanks as well as the collinear bands.
In summary, in both comparisons (Fig. 6, compare A withB and A with C), it appears that induction effects are present. However, these influences push the brightness of the test patch in opposite directions. The critical observation that distinguishes the effects of collinear and flanking bands is that the change in the animal's decisions was far smaller when the same luminance difference occurred in the flanks than in the collinear bands. Stated another way, White's effect appears to be based on antagonistic induction processes, but the induction from collinear bands is significantly more potent than induction from flanking areas. For this reason, the overall effect appears to be the opposite of induction if one focuses only on the contrast of the long flanking borders with the gray patches.
In Figure 6D, both the flanks on the WGW side and the collinear bands on the BGB side are darker than the gray patches. In this situation, the monkey saccaded 100% of the time to the more luminous gray patch. This is consistent with the finding in humans that White's effect disappears if the test patch has a luminance higher than the lighter bands in the square wave (Spehar et al., 1995).
DISCUSSION
The results of this study show a remarkable qualitative and quantitative similarity between brightness perception in humans and macaques. The discussion below addresses the implications of this finding for human–macaque comparisons and brain mechanisms underlying perception.
Did the animals base their decisions on brightness?
Animals in these experiments clearly used the intensity of visual stimuli to make judgments, but was brightness the quality that they assessed? In the brightness discrimination experiments, brightness covaried with luminance, so the results might validly be called luminance discriminations. It is in the induction and White's effect experiments that the perceptual quality of brightness is distinguished from the physical measure of luminance. The most straightforward evidence that brightness was used is the shifts in the discrimination curves away from simple luminance discrimination.
In the induction experiments, if the animals based their judgments on the luminance of the central disks, they should have made saccades to the comparison and test disks 50% of the time when the disk luminances were identical. Instead, the curves shifted in a direction that would be called induction in humans. However, might the animals have used something other than the disk brightness, for example, the luminance of the annuli? Several observations argue that both monkeys made their judgments based on the brightness of the disks and not on the luminance of the surrounds. First, in Figure 3A, each curvewas constructed from trials in which both test and comparison surrounds had fixed luminance. The perceptual judgments varied systematically and correlated with the brightness of the comparison disk as judged by humans. The judgments did not correlate with either surround luminance or test disk luminance. Additionally, on trials in which the test and comparison disks were presented in the absence of surrounds, as well as when their surrounds had identical luminance, both monkeys correctly made saccades to the more luminous center on ∼99% of trials.
In the experiments on White's effect, the stimulus configuration was more complex, allowing for more possible strategies on the part of the animals. For example, on the WGW stimulus, the fixation point was adjacent to a black band, but, on the BGB side, the fixation point was adjacent to a white band (Fig. 1D). Might it be that the high luminance white flank near the fixation point on the BGB side biased the animal to saccade to that side? One piece of evidence against this possibility has already been described: when the gray patches were identical, the animal saccaded almost entirely to the WGW side. This result is consistent with White's effect but inconsistent with the use of the bright band on the BGB side. The same conclusion can be reached referring to Figures 4 and 5. In all cases when the luminance of the two gray patches was the same, very few saccades were made to the BGB side.
Another possible strategy is that the animals were biased toward the WGW side. A conceivable reason for this might be that the animals averaged over an area larger than the gray patch. On the WGW side, averaging the white flanks along with the gray patch would give a higher luminance than averaging the black flanks with the other gray patch on the BGB side. A bias to the WGW side based on coarse spatial averaging is probably not responsible for the data because the percentage of saccades to the BGB side increased when the luminance of the black collinear bands on the BGB side was decreased (Fig.6B). This is opposite the prediction of coarse spatial averaging but consistent with all the previously described evidence that the animals did base their decisions on brightness.
Comparisons with human perception and other animal studies
The first experiment we conducted examined brightness discrimination, and we derived psychometric functions (Fig. 2) that are qualitatively similar to functions for humans. In both species, there is a similar increase in the increment needed to discriminate brightness as mean luminance increases. Moreover, a quantitative comparison can be made of the Weber fractions in the two species. In our experiments, the Weber fraction for animal PN was 0.11, and, for monkey HN, it was 0.18. Human discrimination thresholds have been measured across a wide range of luminance, stimulus duration, stimulus size, and other variables, but the macaque values are well within the normal variance in the human data. For example, Cornsweet and Pinsker (1965) obtained a value of 0.14. Our results are also consistent with previous work in nonhuman primates. In the brightness discrimination experiments, we used mean luminance settings of 12.3, 25.2, 37.0, 50.8, 81.9, and 123.7 cd/m2. Crawford (1935)computed a Weber fraction of ∼0.1, with mean luminances of 2.5, 23, and 176 cd/m2, and Brooks (1966) computed ∼0.2.
In brightness induction, the monkey psychophysical curves are also comparable with those from humans. Brightness induction in humans has a nonlinear dependence on the luminance of the background annuli. The annulus surrounding a disk can cover a wide range of luminance, but as long as it is darker than the disk, it has relatively little effect on the brightness of the disk. When the luminance of the disk is raised near to, and then crosses through, the luminance of the disk, there is a precipitous drop in the brightness of the disk (Heinemann, 1955). These same features are exhibited in the macaque data in Figure3. Some human studies (Heinemann, 1955) show that, when the annulus luminance is well below the disk luminance, the brightness of the disk actually increases as annulus luminance increases (i.e., brightness assimilation rather than brightness contrast). We did not see such assimilation effects in our macaque data, but, at the disk and annulus luminances we used in the monkey experiments, assimilation is not seen with humans either. Our results are qualitatively consistent with a previous study of brightness induction that reported the perceptual effect in several primate species (Davis et al., 1965). Unfortunately, it is difficult to quantitatively compare the previous results with our data. Davis et al. had animals compare two Munsell chips on black and white pieces of Masonite. Results were presented as single lightness matching points (i.e., one datum per animal); there were no data curves equivalent to our Figure 3. More importantly, because the properties (e.g., reflectance) of the Masonite backgrounds were not specified, it is not possible to establish a correspondence with the luminances in our experiments.
There have been no previous monkey studies of White's effect, but this interesting illusion is clearly perceived by macaques. Just as in humans, it appears to be opposite in sign to brightness contrast: when a gray patch is surrounded predominantly by black rather than white, the gray looks darker rather than lighter. We found that the basic effect generalizes across different test patch luminance levels. Also as in humans, if the flanks are reduced in size so that their length matches that of the gray patches, White's effect disappears and, instead, brightness induction is perceived (Moulden and Kingdom, 1989).
Brightness processing and the brain
Human brain areas critical for brightness processing have not yet been identified, but animal studies suggest that visual cortex performs important computations on the thalamic input. Responses correlated with brightness, brightness induction, the Cornsweet illusion, and lightness constancy have been found in area V1 (Reid and Shapley, 1989; Rossi et al., 1996; MacEvoy et al., 1998; Rossi and Paradiso, 1999; Hung et al., 2001; Kinoshita and Komatsu, 2001; MacEvoy and Paradiso, 2001). These findings do not prove that brightness computations necessarily take place in V1, but relevant signals exist in V1 unlike earlier in the system (Rossi and Paradiso, 1999). The quantitative similarity of brightness perception in monkeys and humans suggests that, if V1 performs critical brightness computations in animals, it might play a similar role in humans.
It is also possible that additional processing beyond V1 takes place. As mentioned in Results, our data are consistent with the idea that White's effect is a form of brightness induction that obeys particular “higher” rules. There is considerable debate about the extent to which White's effect requires more than bottom-up filtering (Moulden and Kingdom, 1989; Anderson, 1997; Todorovic, 1997; Blakeslee and McCourt, 1999; Kelly and Grossberg, 2000; Ross and Pessoa, 2000; Howe, 2001). Although controversial, a variety of brightness phenomena have been reported to depend on “higher” processing, including the analysis of illumination, depth, transparency, and grouping (Knill and Kersten, 1991; Adelson, 1993; Schirillo and Shevell, 1993; Buckley et al., 1994; Taya et al., 1995; Anderson, 1997; Kingdom et al., 1997;Schirillo and Shevell, 1997; Todorovic, 1997; Wishart et al., 1997;Gilchrist et al., 1999; Purves et al., 1999; Paradiso, 2000). It is only speculation at this point, but it seems likely that not all of these factors are analyzed in primary visual cortex. For this reason, the similarity of human and macaque brightness perception may indicate that visual processing of brightness is comparable in the two species well beyond V1.
Footnotes
- Received April 22, 2002.
- Revision received August 20, 2002.
- Accepted August 22, 2002.
This research was supported by the United States National Eye Institute. We thank Lisa Kinsella for technical assistance and Drs. Rick Born, Ken Britten, Greg DeAngelis, Jennifer Groh, Nikos Logothetis, Earl Miller, and Michael Shadlen for valuable technical advice.
Correspondence should be addressed to Michael A. Paradiso, Department of Neuroscience, 192 Thayer Street, Brown University, Providence, RI 02912. Email: michael_paradiso{at}brown.edu.
- Copyright © 2002 Society for Neuroscience