Abstract
Adaptive forms of synaptic plasticity that reduce excitatory synaptic transmission in response to prolonged increases in neuronal activity may prevent runaway positive feedback in neuronal circuits. In hippocampal neurons, for example, glutamatergic presynaptic terminals are selectively silenced, creating “mute” synapses, after periods of increased neuronal activity or sustained depolarization. Previous work suggests that cAMP-dependent and proteasome-dependent mechanisms participate in silencing induction by depolarization, but upstream activators are unknown. We, therefore, tested the role of calcium and G-protein signaling in silencing induction in cultured hippocampal neurons. We found that silencing induction by depolarization was not dependent on rises in intracellular calcium, from either extracellular or intracellular sources. Silencing was, however, pertussis toxin sensitive, which suggests that inhibitory G-proteins are recruited. Surprisingly, blocking four common inhibitory G-protein-coupled receptors (GPCRs) (adenosine A1 receptors, GABAB receptors, metabotropic glutamate receptors, and CB1 cannabinoid receptors) and one ionotropic receptor with metabotropic properties (kainate receptors) failed to prevent depolarization-induced silencing. Activating a subset of these GPCRs (A1 and GABAB) with agonist application induced silencing, however, which supports the hypothesis that G-protein activation is a critical step in silencing. Overall, our results suggest that depolarization activates silencing through an atypical GPCR or through receptor-independent G-protein activation. GPCR agonist-induced silencing exhibited dependence on the ubiquitin-proteasome system, as was shown previously for depolarization-induced silencing, implicating the degradation of vital synaptic proteins in silencing by GPCR activation. These data suggest that presynaptic muting in hippocampal neurons uses a G-protein-dependent but calcium-independent mechanism to depress presynaptic vesicle release.
Introduction
Adaptive forms of plasticity, including homeostatic synaptic plasticity, help maintain neuronal firing rates and prevent overexcitation, excitotoxicity, and information degradation caused by positive feedback inherent in glutamate signaling (Turrigiano, 1999; Turrigiano and Nelson, 2004; Maffei and Fontanini, 2009; Pozo and Goda, 2010). We have described adaptive presynaptic silencing after increased neuronal activity (Moulder et al., 2004, 2006). Indeed, presynaptically silent (mute) synapses have been observed in several preparations, but induction and expression mechanisms remain unclear (Malenka and Nicoll, 1997; Voronin and Cherubini, 2004; Atasoy and Kavalali, 2006).
In hippocampal neurons, depolarization-induced presynaptic silencing is induced rapidly by strong depolarization akin to that generated by stroke, seizure, and spreading depression (Gido et al., 1997; Walz, 2000; Somjen, 2001; Moulder et al., 2004). Physiological action potential firing induces silencing over more protracted periods (Moulder et al., 2006), so presynaptic muting operates over a range of physiological, and perhaps pathophysiological, conditions. Adaptive presynaptic silencing is selective for glutamatergic terminals and is slowly reversible (Moulder et al., 2004). Expression involves altered vesicle priming and requires the ubiquitin proteasome system (UPS) (Moulder et al., 2006; Jiang et al., 2010), but upstream induction mechanisms remain uncertain.
Circumstantial evidence implicates calcium and cAMP alterations in silencing induction. Calcium is involved in many forms of synaptic plasticity (Malenka, 1994; Fisher et al., 1997; Turrigiano, 2008). Prolonged decreases in cAMP signaling produce presynaptic silencing, and cAMP increases occlude depolarization-induced silencing (Moulder et al., 2008). Furthermore, calcium-sensitive adenylyl cyclase activity, which increases cAMP levels, is important for normal recovery from silencing (Moulder et al., 2008). If decreased cAMP signaling is needed for depolarization-induced presynaptic silencing, we might expect involvement of inhibitory G-proteins in its induction. Despite this evidence, double knock-out mice of the two major calcium-sensitive isoforms of adenylyl cyclase exhibit intact depolarization-induced silencing (Moulder et al., 2008), thereby questioning a direct role for either calcium or G-protein signaling. Clarification of how adaptive presynaptic silencing is induced will be crucial in identifying potential targets for therapies aimed at excitotoxicity-related dysfunction.
Here we explored whether calcium and inhibitory G-protein signaling are necessary for hippocampal adaptive presynaptic silencing. Surprisingly, intracellular calcium rises were unnecessary. However, we found that pertussis toxin, which inhibits Gi/o signaling, reduced depolarization-induced silencing. Direct agonist activation of two classes of G-protein-coupled receptors (GPCRs) induced silencing, supporting the hypothesis that silencing is G-protein dependent. In contrast, pharmacological blockade of five candidate receptors, including the two that induced silencing, failed to block depolarization-induced silencing. This suggests that depolarization activates an uncommon GPCR or a pertussis toxin-sensitive G-protein through a receptor-independent mechanism. Nevertheless, depolarization- and GPCR-induced muting share downstream mechanisms because both are sensitive to proteasome inhibition. Thus, adaptive presynaptic silencing is a unique form of calcium-independent, G-protein-dependent synaptic plasticity. Furthermore, we show that prolonged G-protein activation leads to persistent presynaptic silencing, an effect distinct from classical acute presynaptic GPCR effects on vesicle release probability.
Materials and Methods
Cell culture.
Cultures of primary hippocampal neurons were prepared as described previously (Mennerick et al., 1995). Briefly, hippocampi were removed from 0–3 postnatal d male and female Sprague Dawley rat pups and enzymatically treated with papain (1 mg/ml) before mechanical dissociation. Cells were then plated in 35 mm culture dishes precoated with 0.15% agarose as either mass cultures, seeded at ∼650 cells/mm2 on a confluent layer of type I collagen (0.5 mg/ml), or as microisland cultures, seeded at ∼100 cells/mm2 on microdots of collagen. Plating medium contained Eagle's medium (Invitrogen) supplemented with 5% heat-inactivated horse serum, 5% fetal bovine serum, 17 mm d-glucose, 400 μm glutamine, 50 U/ml penicillin, and 50 μg/ml streptomycin. Cultures were maintained at 37°C in a humidified incubator under controlled atmospheric conditions (5% CO2/95% air). Cytosine arabinoside (6.7 μm) was added 3–4 d after plating to inhibit cell division. One-half of the culture media was exchanged with Neurobasal medium (Invitrogen) plus B27 supplement 4–5 d after plating. All experiments were performed 10–14 d in vitro, and controls consisted of sibling cultures from the same litter that were treated and/or recorded on the same day to control for synapse development and culture conditions that vary by age and plating conditions. All 4 h treatments were performed in the presence of ionotropic glutamate receptor blockers d-(−)-2-amino-5-phosphonopentanoic acid (d-APV) (50 μm; Tocris Bioscience) and 2,3-dioxo-6-nitro-1,2,3,4-tetrahydrobenzo[f]quinoxaline-7-sulfonamide (NBQX) (1 μm; Tocris Bioscience), and nondepolarized controls always consisted of equimolar NaCl addition to the media to control for osmotic changes. Controls for calcium-free Neurobasal medium (Washington University Medical School Tissue Culture Support Center) supplemented with B27 and calcium chelators were given the identical medium except with 2 mm calcium and without chelators.
Electrophysiology.
All whole-cell voltage-clamp recordings were performed on autaptic neurons in microisland culture. To control for variability in maturity and development, all experiments used similar numbers of neurons from sibling cultures on any given day. Data were collected with an Axopatch 200B or Multiclamp 700B amplifier interfaced to a Digidata 1322A or 1440A data acquisition board (Molecular Devices).
Before recording, the culture medium was exchanged for saline solution containing 138 mm NaCl, 4 mm KCl, 2 mm CaCl2, 1 mm MgCl2, 10 mm glucose, 10 mm HEPES, and 25 μm d-APV with a pH of 7.25. For most experiments measuring EPSCs, internal pipette solution consisted of 140 mm K-gluconate, 4 mm NaCl, 0.5 mm CaCl2, 5 mm EGTA, and 10 mm HEPES at a pH of 7.25. For experiments measuring both EPSCs and IPSCs, 140 mm KCl was substituted for K-gluconate to provide a similar PSC driving force at the holding potential of −70 mV.
Electrode pipettes pulled from borosilicate glass (World Precision Instruments) had resistances of 2.5–6 MΩ, and access resistance was compensated 85–100%. Signals were sampled at 5–10 kHz and low-pass filtered at 2–5 kHz. Recordings were performed at room temperature, and membrane potential was typically clamped at −70 mV. Neurons were recorded within 1 h of exchanging the culture medium with the saline solution.
PSCs were evoked by a brief (1.5 ms) depolarization to 0 mV. Paired-pulse responses were evoked by two such depolarizations with an interstimulus interval of 50 ms. Sucrose-evoked EPSCs were elicited by 3 s local application of 0.5 m sucrose. All local solution applications used a multibarrel perfusion system with the common perfusion port placed within 0.5 mm of the neuron under study and yielding solution exchange times of ∼100 ms.
Calcium imaging.
All calcium imaging experiments used mass cultures. For fura-2 experiments, neurons were plated on glass-bottom dishes (MatTek Corporation). Cells were loaded with fura-2 by 60 min incubation with 5 μm fura-2 AM (Invitrogen) and 0.1% Pluronic F-127 (Invitrogen) in Neurobasal medium, pH 7.2, at room temperature, washed with Neurobasal medium, and incubated for another 60 min to allow for ester hydrolysis. After loading, the cells were imaged on an Eclipse TE300 inverted microscope using a 40× (1.3 numerical aperture) oil-immersion objective (Nikon). The microscope was equipped with a 75 W xenon arc lamp and a cooled CCD camera (Cooke Corp.). The fluorescence excitation was provided by a band-specific filter (340 and 380 nm; Semrock) in combination with DM400 dichroic beam splitter (Nikon). Pairs of images were collected at alternate excitation wavelengths. The images were divided by one another to yield ratio values for individual cell bodies after subtracting the matching background. Imaging was performed at room temperature, but cultures were returned to 37°C for the period between images early in the treatment and images at the end of the 4 h treatment. For this reason, the same neurons were represented at baseline and 5 min time points, but separate fields of neurons were used for 4 h time points in Figure 1. MetaFluor software (Molecular Devices) was used for image acquisition and analysis.
Fluo-4 was loaded into cells by addition of Fluo-4 AM (2 μm; Invitrogen) to the culture media 30 min before the end of the 4 h KCl treatment. Medium was then exchanged for extracellular recording saline containing elevated KCl (34 mm total) and 0 mm CaCl2. Cells were imaged at room temperature using an Eclipse TE2000-S inverted microscope with 40× (0.6 numerical aperture) objective (Nikon) and a cooled 12-bit CCD camera (Photometrics). Epifluorescence was provided by a metal halide lamp, and images were acquired at 1 Hz using MetaMorph software (Molecular Devices). Calcium was briefly elevated to 0.5 mm for 5–10 s after 5 s of baseline in 0 mm calcium.
FM1-43 loading and immunocytochemistry.
All FM1-43 [N-(3-triethylammoniumpropyl)-4-(4-(dibutylamino)styryl) pyridinium dibromide] imaging experiments used mass cultures plated on glass coverslips. FM1-43 loading and vesicular glutamate transporter 1 (vGluT-1) immunolabeling experiments were performed as described previously (Moulder et al., 2006, 2010). Briefly, within 60 s of replacing media with fresh saline, active presynaptic terminals were labeled for 2 min with 10 μm fixable FM1-43 (FM1-43FX) (Invitrogen) in recording saline supplemented with 45 mm KCl and 1 μm NBQX. Cultures were immediately washed for 5 s with extracellular recording saline containing 500 μm Advasep-7 (CyDex) and 1 μm NBQX, followed by saline plus 1 μm NBQX for 10 min. Afterward, cells were fixed for 10 min in 4% paraformaldehyde/0.2% glutaraldehyde in PBS, washed with PBS, and incubated in a blocking solution (2% normal goat serum/0.08% Triton X-100) for 15 min before 3 h incubation in guinea pig anti-vGluT-1 antibody (Millipore Corporation) diluted 1:2000 in blocking solution. After washing with PBS, cells were incubated for 40 min in Alexa Fluor 647-conjugated anti-guinea pig antibody (Invitrogen) diluted 1:500 in PBS. After washing again with PBS, coverslips were removed from culture dishes and mounted onto glass microscope slides with Fluoromount-G (Southern Biotechnology Associates).
Confocal images were acquired at random locations throughout the dish by an observer naive to the experimental conditions using a C1 scanning confocal laser attached to an inverted Eclipse TE300 microscope with a 60× objective (1.4 numerical aperture) and EZ-C1 software (Nikon). Alternating 543 and 633 nm laser lines were used to obtain z-stack images while pixel dwell time, image resolution, gain settings, field of view size, and z-stack parameters were held constant throughout all conditions from a single experiment. Monochrome images were converted into two-dimensional projected images and analyzed using MetaMorph software.
Trypan blue staining.
For experiments assessing toxicity via trypan blue staining, mass cultures were treated for 5 min with 0.4% trypan blue at 37°C in a humidified incubator under controlled atmospheric conditions (5% CO2/95% air). After one PBS wash at room temperature, neurons were fixed for 10 min in 4% paraformaldehyde/0.2% glutaraldehyde in PBS. After three additional PBS washes, 10 random fields of cells were counted in each dish using a 20× objective. Trypan-positive nuclei were counted under bright field and identified as unhealthy neurons, and phase-bright cell bodies devoid of trypan staining were counted under phase contrast and identified as healthy neurons.
Data analysis.
Electrophysiology data were collected and analyzed with pClamp 9 software (Molecular Devices). For electrically evoked PSCs, leak was subtracted offline before measuring peak amplitude, and the amplitudes of at least three responses were averaged for each cell. For sucrose-evoked EPSCs, currents were integrated over the rise and decay of the response to 10% of the steady-state current. This provided an estimate of the postsynaptic charge transfer corresponding to the readily releasable vesicle pool.
In calcium-imaging experiments using fura-2, the ratio values were normalized to the baseline values (in which no treatment had been applied) to control for differences in baseline calcium concentration. Normalization was achieved by dividing by the first value for each cell under baseline conditions, and 4 h time points were normalized to the average of the ratios at baseline. Values from at least five images were averaged for each neuron at each time point. To calculate the change in fluorescence in fluo-4 experiments, somatic fluorescence intensities from three images were averaged at baseline and subtracted from the average intensity of three images near the peak of fluorescence. The change in fluorescence was normalized to the average baseline fluorescence value to account for variability in fluo-4 loading.
To determine the percentage of active glutamatergic synapses, regions were manually drawn around 10 vGluT-1 puncta each from five images (fields) for each coverslip in a single experiment by an observer naive to experimental conditions. The strategy of using five fields per condition equalized the contribution of a single condition to the final results. Regions were drawn to include the pixels above threshold in the vGluT-1-positive punctum but not larger than the punctum to minimize the contribution of background pixels and/or neighboring puncta to the analysis. Thresholds were applied to all FM1-43FX images before vGluT-1 regions were loaded into the FM1-43FX image. “Active” synapses were defined as those regions that reached at least 10 pixels above threshold in the FM1-43FX image (Moulder et al., 2006). Cluster correlation analyses (Galbraith et al., 2010) and ANOVA statistics applied to the first FM1-43FX/vGluT-1 correspondence dataset (Fig. 1D) found no significant correlations between fields within an experiment (data not shown), so we treated the percentage of active synapses from each field as a statistically independent sample throughout the manuscript.
For trypan blue toxicity experiments, the total numbers of healthy and unhealthy neurons were added together from all 10 fields in each dish. The percentage of healthy neurons was then calculated from the number of healthy neurons relative to the total neurons counted (healthy and unhealthy) in each dish.
Graphs were created using SigmaPlot software (Systat Software). All data were presented as mean ± SEM unless otherwise indicated. To reduce type 1 error that may result from nonsignificant cluster correlations (Galbraith et al., 2010), all experiments used yoked controls (e.g., sibling cultures; see methods above), and we analyzed data conservatively using unpaired statistics with additional corrections (i.e., for multiple comparisons) as appropriate. Unpaired Student's t test was used to determine significance for comparisons of two groups, and Bonferroni's correction for multiple comparisons was applied to analyses of experiments involving more than two conditions. p < 0.05 was required for significance.
Materials.
SCH50911 [(2S)-(+)-5,5-dimethyl-2-morpholineacetic acid], bicuculline, LY341495 [(2S)-2-amino-2-[(1S,2S)-2-carboxycycloprop-1-yl]-3-(xanth-9-yl) propanoic acid], (1S,3R)-1-aminocyclopentane-1,3-dicarboxylic acid (ACPD), and 2-chloro-N6-cyclopentyladenosine (CCPA) were purchased from Tocris Bioscience. BAPTA-AM [AM derivative of bis(2-aminophenoxy)ethane tetraacetic acid] was purchased from Invitrogen, and 8-cyclopentyl-1,3-dipropylxanthine (DPCPX) was purchased from Research Biochemicals. MG-132 (carbobenzoxy-l-leucyl-l-leucyl-l-leucinal) was purchased from Enzo Life Sciences. All other chemicals and reagents were purchased from Sigma unless otherwise stated.
Results
Depolarization silences presynaptic terminals in the absence of extracellular calcium
Calcium is necessary for multiple forms of hippocampal synaptic plasticity, so we determined the role of calcium influx in adaptive presynaptic silencing by removing calcium from the extracellular media during a depolarizing induction challenge known to produce strong presynaptic muting. We used an induction paradigm of 30 mm KCl for 4 h in the presence of ionotropic glutamate receptor blockers d-APV (50 μm) and NBQX (1 μm). The short, strong induction circumvents inevitable developmental confounds of longer interventions with calcium and G-protein signaling and mimics pathophysiological extracellular potassium concentrations during stroke or seizure (Gido et al., 1997; Walz, 2000). KCl at 30 mm clamps the membrane potential at −20 to −30 mV (Moulder et al., 2003; Crawford et al., 2009) and induces adaptive presynaptic silencing without detectable changes in synaptic structure, vesicle release probability (pr), or postsynaptic function (Moulder et al., 2004, 2006). We incubated mass cultures in either calcium-rich culture media or calcium-free culture media supplemented with 500 μm EGTA to buffer residual free calcium. Using the ratiometric calcium indicator fura-2, we verified that calcium-free media prevented rises in cytosolic free calcium concentration normally produced by KCl depolarization (Fig. 1A,B). Control neurons also exhibited a late rise in cytosolic free calcium concentration (Fig. 1B). We speculate that temperature and pH variations between imaging sessions (at room temperature) and intervening periods (at 37°C) may have contributed to cellular stress and calcium influx in nondepolarized neurons. Regardless, calcium-free media prevented the rise in cytosolic free calcium concentration in both control and depolarized neurons over 4 h (Fig. 1B). We conclude, therefore, that our calcium-free media supplemented with EGTA effectively prevented calcium influx.
To assess presynaptic silencing after depolarization in the absence of calcium influx, we performed FM1-43FX labeling in the presence of calcium followed by retrospective immunostaining for vesicular glutamate transporter 1 (vGluT-1) to identify active glutamatergic terminals. In agreement with previous experiments under normal calcium conditions (Moulder et al., 2004), neurons depolarized in calcium-free media plus EGTA exhibited a decrease in the percentage of vGluT-1 terminals that colocalize with FM1-43FX (Fig. 1C,D). This suggests that calcium influx is not necessary to induce adaptive presynaptic silencing.
Depolarization silences presynaptic terminals in the absence of intracellular free calcium
Although influx of extracellular calcium is not necessary for induction of adaptive presynaptic silencing, calcium rises from intracellular sources may participate. To prevent intracellular calcium rises, we loaded cells with the fast intracellular calcium chelator BAPTA using the cell-permeant precursor BAPTA-AM. Because normal action-potential-driven neurotransmitter release is dependent on fast rises in local presynaptic calcium concentration and would be depressed by BAPTA-AM incubation, we used calcium-independent hypertonic sucrose-evoked EPSCs to determine the size of the readily releasable pool of vesicles (Rosenmund and Stevens, 1996), which is proportional to the total number of active synapses (Moulder et al., 2004). For these experiments, we used autaptic neurons in microisland cultures and applied hypertonic sucrose to evoke EPSCs during whole-cell recordings. Autaptic neurons were used because their isolation eliminates confounding polysynaptic effects during electrophysiological measurements. Similar to previous results (Rosenmund and Stevens, 1996), sucrose-evoked EPSCs were unaffected by 4 h incubation with 20 μm BAPTA-AM in calcium-free media (Fig. 2A,B). In contrast electrically evoked EPSCs, which are dependent on presynaptic calcium influx, were strongly depressed after 4 h BAPTA chelation (Fig. 2A,B). These results suggest that 4 h BAPTA incubation strongly chelates intracellular free calcium.
To test whether intracellular rises in calcium concentration are necessary for presynaptic silencing, we depolarized neurons for 4 h in 30 mm KCl in the presence of 2 mm calcium or in calcium-free media containing 20 μm BAPTA-AM. To ensure that intracellular calcium rises during induction did not saturate the ability of BAPTA to chelate calcium, we performed calcium imaging of treated neurons using the calcium indicator fluo-4. Briefly raising extracellular calcium from 0 to 0.5 mm increased cytosolic free calcium concentration in control neurons after 4 h depolarization in calcium-rich media, as assessed by changes in somatic fluo-4 fluorescence (45.5 ± 4.5% increase; n = 40) (Fig. 2C). This rise in cytosolic free calcium was not observed in neurons after incubation in calcium-free media supplemented with BAPTA-AM (1.0 ± 1.2% increase; n = 40; p = 1.0 × 10−15) (Fig. 2C). BAPTA, therefore, effectively chelates intracellular free calcium, even after a sustained depolarization.
To test whether BAPTA-AM incubation blocks presynaptic silencing, we incubated in BAPTA-AM and compared neurons after the KCl challenge to neurons challenged with an equimolar NaCl control. As shown previously, the 4 h depolarizing challenge in calcium-rich media depressed sucrose-evoked EPSCs by ∼45% (control, 1224.9 ± 147.7 pC; depolarized, 670.1 ± 135.7 pC; n = 29–30 neurons; p < 0.01) (Moulder et al., 2004). Despite BAPTA-AM incubation in calcium-free media, the charge of sucrose-evoked EPSCs was still significantly depressed in KCl-challenged neurons compared with sibling controls (control, 581 ± 100 pC; depolarized, 168 ± 27 pC; n = 26–29; p < 0.0001) (Fig. 2D). These results argue strongly that adaptive presynaptic silencing is calcium independent.
Activation of inhibitory G-proteins is necessary for depolarization-induced presynaptic silencing
Indirect evidence has previously implicated decreased cAMP levels in depolarization-induced presynaptic silencing (Moulder et al., 2008). Thus, prolonged activation of inhibitory G-proteins, which inhibit adenylyl cyclase activity and cAMP production, could participate in adaptive muting. We, therefore, tested the hypothesis that inhibitory G-proteins are necessary for depolarization-induced silencing.
We preincubated neurons for 24 h in 500 ng/ml pertussis toxin, which, by ADP ribosylation of the αi/o subunit, prevents inhibitory G-protein activation. This interferes with α subunit signaling and blocks the association of Gαi/o subunits with their upstream GPCRs (Brown and Sihra, 2008). To confirm that pertussis toxin treatment itself did not induce toxicity, we used trypan blue staining to identify unhealthy neurons. The percentage of healthy neurons did not differ between control neurons and those treated 24 h with 500 ng/ml pertussis toxin (control, 92.2 ± 1.6%; pertussis toxin, 93.5 ± 1.0%; n = 4 dishes; p = 0.54). After 24 h in pertussis toxin, we coincubated with 30 mm KCl or an equimolar control during an additional 4 h in pertussis toxin. After treatment, we assessed synaptic function by measuring EPSC amplitudes in autaptic neurons and vGluT-1/FM1-43FX correspondence in mass cultures. Pertussis toxin had no effect on EPSC amplitude in nondepolarized neurons, confirming that the toxin did not compromise cell health, but it prevented depolarization-induced depression of EPSC amplitude (Fig. 3A,B). Pertussis toxin did not, however, alter paired-pulse depression in any of the experimental groups (control, 29.9 ± 5.6%; pertussis toxin, 18.0 ± 4.3%; depolarized, 8.3 ± 6.2%; depolarized + pertussis toxin, 20.7 ± 5.9%; n = 13–14 neurons per condition; p > 0.05) (supplemental Fig. 1A, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material), as is expected from acute effects of GPCR activation on vesicle release probability (Mennerick and Zorumski, 1995; Dobrunz and Stevens, 1997; Zucker and Regehr, 2002; Brown and Sihra, 2008). Similarly, pertussis toxin prevented the depolarization-induced decrease in active synapses measured via FM1-43FX/vGluT-1 correspondence (Fig. 3C,D). Together, these results suggest that activation of inhibitory G-proteins is necessary for depolarization-induced presynaptic silencing.
A1 receptor activation is not required for depolarization-induced presynaptic silencing
To explore the role of GPCRs upstream of inhibitory G-proteins in presynaptic silencing, we examined whether the G-protein-coupled A1 adenosine receptor is involved. The A1 receptor is an excellent candidate because increased adenosine levels in the extracellular space occur via calcium-independent mechanisms: conversion from ATP (Latini and Pedata, 2001), which is released from astrocytes (Wang et al., 2000), and release by transporters (Dunwiddie and Masino, 2001). Adenosine release has also been implicated in neuroprotection, in part through acute A1 receptor-mediated inhibition of presynaptic glutamate release (Stone et al., 2009). Additionally, A1 receptors selectively inhibit synaptic transmission at excitatory, not inhibitory, terminals in the hippocampus (Yoon and Rothman, 1991), possibly explaining the selectivity of adaptive presynaptic silencing for excitatory over inhibitory terminals (Moulder et al., 2004).
To test whether A1 receptor activation is necessary for depolarization-induced presynaptic silencing, we incubated neurons with depolarizing KCl or equimolar NaCl control in the presence of the selective A1 antagonist DPCPX (200 nm). The decrease in EPSC amplitude in depolarized neurons treated with DPCPX was comparable with ESPC depression from neurons incubated in the absence of DPCPX (Fig. 4A,B). DPCPX incubation also did not alter paired-pulse depression (control, 11.8 ± 4.8%; DPCPX, 16.4 ± 5.1%; depolarized, 11.0 ± 8.7%; depolarized + DPCPX, 7.3 ± 11.7%; n = 9 neurons per condition; p > 0.05) (supplemental Fig. 1B, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). Additionally, we assessed presynaptic silencing by measuring the percentage of active glutamatergic presynaptic terminals via FM1-43FX/vGluT-1 correspondence. In neurons treated 4 h with 30 mm KCl, the percentage of vGluT-1-positive presynaptic terminals that colocalize with FM1-43FX fluorescence was decreased from control, regardless of whether the neurons had been coincubated with DPCPX (Fig. 4C,D). We noticed a trend-level difference in the percentage of active synapses in depolarized neurons treated with DPCPX compared with depolarized neurons without DPCPX (depolarized, 10.6 ± 1.5%; depolarized + DPCPX, 17.4 ± 2.1%; p = 0.052 after correction for multiple comparisons), potentially suggesting a minor role of adenosine A1 receptors in depolarization-induced silencing. Despite this trend, however, depolarization still decreased the percentage of active synapses in the presence of DPCPX (DPCPX, 42.6 ± 3.4%; depolarized + DPCPX, 17.4 ± 2.1%; p = 1.7 × 10−8 after correction for multiple comparisons). Together, these data suggest that blocking A1 receptor activation during depolarization does not prevent the induction of adaptive presynaptic silencing.
GABAB, metabotropic glutamate, CB1, and kainate receptors are not required for depolarization-induced presynaptic silencing
Although A1 adenosine receptor activation is not necessary to induce adaptive presynaptic silencing (Fig. 4), activation of inhibitory G-proteins is critical (Fig. 3). Multiple GPCRs linked to inhibitory G-proteins are expressed in rodent hippocampus. Our culture preparation, however, does not contain modulatory projection neurons from outside the hippocampus, so the list of GPCRs with ligands available in vitro is restricted. The calcium independence of induction also excludes transmitters/modulators released by classical calcium-dependent exocytosis. Nevertheless, reverse transport of GABA (Schwartz, 1982; Gaspary et al., 1998), reverse transport of glutamate (Nicholls and Attwell, 1990; Szatkowski et al., 1990), and nonclassical cannabinoid signaling (Di Marzo and Deutsch, 1998; Di Marzo et al., 1998; Wilson and Nicoll, 2001) could elevate ambient modulator levels and induce silencing in response to depolarization.
We first tested whether the G-protein-coupled GABAB receptor is responsible for inducing adaptive presynaptic silencing. To prevent GABAB receptor activation, we incubated neurons in the presence of the GABAB receptor antagonist SCH50911 (50 μm). To test effectiveness of SCH50911 in our system, we coapplied SCH50911 acutely to our autaptic neurons with the GABAB receptor agonist baclofen (10 μm). Baclofen depressed evoked EPSCs and IPSCs by 65.4 ± 8.1%, and addition of SCH50911 recovered the PSCs to 96.0 ± 4.8% of their original amplitude (n = 10 neurons) (Fig. 5A). Incubation of mass cultures in SCH50911 for 4 h significantly increased the percentage of active glutamatergic presynaptic terminals from baseline (Fig. 5B), suggesting a small endogenous GABAB receptor tone in this dataset. Despite this effect on baseline silent synapses, SCH50911 did not prevent the decrease in the percentage of active synapses induced by depolarization (Fig. 5B). These results demonstrate that, despite effectiveness of SCH50911, GABAB receptor activation is not involved in depolarization-induced adaptive presynaptic silencing. Similar results were obtained with the GABAA receptor antagonists bicuculline and picrotoxin (supplemental Fig. 2, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material), confirming that GABA signaling is not involved in depolarization-induced presynaptic silencing.
Other plausible candidate GPCRs for the induction of adaptive presynaptic silencing include Gi/o-coupled metabotropic glutamate receptors (mGluRs). LY341495, a nonselective mGluR antagonist at concentrations above 22 μm (Schoepp et al., 1999), reversed acute EPSC depression by a near-saturating concentration of the mGluR agonist ACPD (33.9 ± 7.2% depression in 50 μm ACPD; 92.2 ± 6.8% recovery in ACPD plus 25 μm LY341495; n = 7–8 neurons) (Fig. 5C). However, LY341495 failed to alter baseline active glutamatergic synapses or presynaptic silencing in response to depolarization (Fig. 5D). These results indicate that antagonism of mGluR activation does not prevent depolarization-induced presynaptic silencing.
To test G-protein-coupled CB1 cannabinoid receptor involvement, we incubated mass cultures in 1 μm AM251 [1-(2,4-dichlorophenyl)-5-(4-iodophenyl)-4-methyl-N-1-piperidinyl-1H-pyrazole-3-carboxamide], a CB1 antagonist, during depolarizing or control challenge. To ensure that AM251 inhibited CB1 activation, we applied the CB1 agonist (R)-(+)-WIN55,212-2 [R-(+)-(2,3-dihydro-5-methyl-3-[(4-morpholinyl)methyl]pyrol[1,2,3-de]-1,4- benzoxazin-6-yl)(1-naphthalenyl) methanone monomethanesulfonate] (WIN) (200 nm) acutely to autaptic neurons. WIN acutely depressed PSCs by 34.6 ± 8.6% (Fig. 5E), whereas preincubation in AM251 prevented the WIN-induced depression (101.1 ± 2.5% of baseline; n = 8 neurons per condition) (Fig. 5E). AM251 incubation during the 4 h depolarization challenge, however, altered neither the baseline percentage of active glutamatergic synapses nor depolarization-induced silencing (Fig. 5F). We conclude that CB1 activation does not participate in depolarization-induced presynaptic silencing.
It is plausible that multiple GPCRs are simultaneously activated by strong depolarization. If this occurs, then each receptor might contribute individually to a small, statistically insignificant effect on the percentage of active presynaptic terminals. Simultaneous activation, however, may synergistically upregulate the activity of downstream signaling cascades. To test this hypothesis, we incubated mass cultures for 4 h in a mixture of GPCR antagonists (200 nm DPCPX, 50 μm SCH50911, 25 μm LY341495, and 1 μm AM251) to simultaneously block the activation of A1 adenosine receptors, GABAB receptors, mGluRs, and CB1 receptors. This antagonist mixture did not alter the percentage of active glutamatergic presynaptic terminals at baseline (Fig. 5G), indicating that transmitter tone over 4 h was not sufficient to induce additional muting. Additionally, this mixture of GPCR antagonists did not prevent silencing in response to depolarization (Fig. 5G). Interestingly, we failed to observe the trend-level increase in the percentage of active synapses produced in depolarized neurons exposed to DPCPX (Fig. 4D). This suggests that these four receptor classes, although linked to inhibitory G-proteins, are not responsible for the induction of depolarization-induced presynaptic silencing.
Although 1 μm NBQX was present during all depolarization challenges to prevent ionotropic glutamate receptor activation, kainate receptors may not be blocked at this concentration (Bureau et al., 1999; Crowder et al., 2002). Kainate receptors can induce metabotropic effects dependent on G-proteins in addition to their ionotropic effects (Cunha et al., 1999; Lerma, 2003; Rodríguez-Moreno and Sihra, 2007). To ensure that kainate receptors and their downstream metabotropic signaling cascades are not activated during 4 h depolarization, we coincubated with 10 μm NBQX, which should block both kainate receptors and AMPA receptors (Lauri et al., 2001; Delaney and Jahr, 2002). After a 4 h depolarizing or control challenge, we recorded EPSC amplitudes in autaptic neurons. Control cultures were treated acutely for 1 min with 10 μm NBQX just before recording to account for any residual effects of the high NBQX concentration on AMPA receptors. The higher concentration of NBQX did not prevent depolarization-induced silencing and did not alter the EPSC amplitude compared with control neurons (Fig. 5H). This suggests that metabotropic effects downstream of kainate receptor activation are unimportant for depolarization-induced silencing.
A1 receptor activation and GABAB receptor activation induce presynaptic silencing
It is intriguing that A1 receptor, GABAB receptor, mGluR, CB1, and kainate receptor activation are not necessary for induction of presynaptic silencing despite our evidence that depolarization-induced silencing depends on pertussis toxin-sensitive G-proteins. These results suggest that inhibitory G-proteins are activated during strong depolarization but not by activation of these particular receptors. To further test our hypothesis that a G-protein-dependent mechanism, regardless of the source of activation, controls adaptive presynaptic silencing, we tested whether direct activation of several GPCR classes induces silencing.
We first determined whether increases in exogenous extracellular adenosine concentration induce presynaptic silencing. We incubated cultures in the presence of 100 μm adenosine for 4 h. A high concentration was chosen to counteract any degradation that may occur from the natural activity of extracellular enzymes or uptake through transporters (Dunwiddie and Masino, 2001). We used FM1-43FX loading of presynaptic terminals followed by vGluT-1 immunostaining to determine the percentage of active glutamatergic terminals. Incubation for 4 h with 100 μm adenosine decreased the percentage of active synapses (Fig. 6A,B). To determine whether this effect of adenosine resulted from activation of the A1 receptor, we coincubated with DPCPX (200 nm). DPCPX incubation alone did not alter the percentage of active glutamatergic synapses, but it prevented the adenosine-induced silencing (Fig. 6A,B). This result also serves as an effective positive control for experiments testing DPCPX effects on depolarization-induced muting (Fig. 4). Note that FM1-43FX uptake should be immune to the well-known transient, acute effects of adenosine agonists on pr because agonists and antagonists were washed out of the cultures before the FM1-43FX challenge (Wu and Saggau, 1994; Brown and Sihra, 2008). Also, FM1-43FX loading was conducted during 2 min of a strong depolarization designed to empty the entire recycling pool of vesicles regardless of pr. Thus, these results suggest that extracellular adenosine induces presynaptic silencing through an A1 receptor-dependent mechanism.
Results with adenosine do not exclude a contribution from A2 or A3 receptors, although this contribution is presumably minimal because the A1-specific antagonist DPCPX blocked the silencing. To confirm that A1 receptor activation alone is sufficient for presynaptic silencing induction, we incubated mass cultures in the presence of the highly selective A1 receptor agonist CCPA. CCPA (10 nm for 4 h) decreased the percentage of active glutamatergic terminals, and the CCPA effect was blocked by coincubation with 200 nm DPCPX (Fig. 6C). We also confirmed that CCPA persistently depressed presynaptic action-potential-driven vesicle release by measuring autaptic EPSC amplitudes after 4 h of CCPA incubation. Electrophysiological recordings were performed in extracellular recording saline in the absence of CCPA, so acute effects are unlikely. However, to ensure that EPSC depression was not caused by residual acute effects of CCPA, a high-affinity ligand that might have failed to dissociate from A1 receptors during the media exchange (Lohse et al., 1988), we used neurons from sibling cultures acutely exposed to 10 nm CCPA (<1 min) as our control. EPSC amplitudes were significantly decreased in neurons recorded after 4 h treatment with CCPA compared with those treated acutely with CCPA (control, −8.92 ± 1.19 nA; 4 h CCPA, −2.59 ± 1.10 nA; n = 10; p = 0.001) (Fig. 6D). Additionally, we measured paired-pulse depression to assess whether lingering CCPA altered vesicle pr. An increased paired-pulse ratio (e.g., a decreased paired-pulse depression) is typically interpreted to reflect decreased pr, which could be caused by presynaptic GPCR activation (Mennerick and Zorumski, 1995; Dobrunz and Stevens, 1997; Zucker and Regehr, 2002; Brown and Sihra, 2008). The paired-pulse depression in neurons treated acutely with CCPA (23.4 ± 5.5%; n = 10) was not significantly different from the depression in neurons treated for 4 h (25.8 ± 7.0%; n = 9; p = 0.79) (supplemental Fig. 1C, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). Together, these results suggest a novel presynaptic muting by prolonged selective activation of A1 adenosine receptors. Presynaptic silencing outlives agonist exposure and does not result from altered pr.
We next incubated neurons in 50 μm baclofen, the GABAB receptor agonist, for 4 h before evaluation in baclofen-free solutions. Baclofen, in the absence of any depolarizing stimulus, persistently depressed autaptic EPSC amplitudes and reduced the percentage of active glutamatergic presynaptic terminals (Fig. 6E). Baclofen did not, however, have a persisting effect on the paired-pulse depression (control, 20.8 ± 7.3%; baclofen, 1.1 ± 7.7%; n = 9; p = 0.08) (supplemental Fig. 1D, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). Additionally, we have confirmed that the effects of CCPA and baclofen on FM1-43FX/vGluT-1 correspondence do not result from transient actions of these agonists. We coapplied CCPA and baclofen for 4 h and allowed 10 min of recovery in untreated media before FM1-43FX loading. Neurons treated with CCPA and baclofen maintained a decreased percentage of active glutamatergic presynaptic terminals after this period of recovery (control + recovery, 79.0 ± 4.3%; CCPA/baclofen + recovery, 51.5 ± 4.5%; n = 20 fields from 4 coverslips; p = 0.00009). In contrast, saturating concentrations of neither ACPD nor another mGluR agonist, l-AP-4, induced silencing (supplemental Fig. 3A,B, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). Similarly, the CB1 agonist WIN did not induce silencing at a saturating concentration (supplemental Fig. 3C, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material), and kainate receptor agonist kainic acid did not produce silencing at concentrations of 1 or 10 μm (supplemental Fig. 3D,E, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). These results suggest that GPCR-induced silencing is induced by only a select group of receptors. The lack of silencing induced by mGluR and CB1 activation may be caused by low receptor expression levels in presynaptic terminals of cultured hippocampal neurons (as suggested by the weak acute effects of near-saturating agonist concentrations in Fig. 5C,E) or by divergent downstream signaling cascades.
GPCR-induced silencing is proteasome dependent
Inhibitory G-protein activation links depolarization-induced silencing and GPCR agonist-induced silencing, but it is unclear whether GPCR-dependent presynaptic silencing invokes similar downstream signaling cascades as depolarization-induced presynaptic silencing. For instance, maximum GPCR agonist-induced silencing tends to be weaker than depolarization-induced silencing (Fig. 6C,E), which could hint at different underlying mechanisms. It was shown recently that depolarization-induced muting is ultimately dependent on activity of the UPS. Depolarization-induced presynaptic silencing is prevented by coincubation with the proteasome inhibitor MG-132 (Jiang et al., 2010). To test whether GPCR agonist-induced silencing also recruits the UPS, we blocked proteasome activity by 30 min preincubation with MG-132 alone before coapplying MG-132 with GPCR agonists for 4 h. To maximize the inhibitory G-protein signal, we coapplied 10 nm CCPA and 50 μm baclofen to simultaneously activate both A1 receptors and GABAB receptors. This treatment did not result in more silencing than either agonist alone (compare Fig. 6C–E with Fig. 7) as expected from convergent downstream signaling in the two receptor systems (Brown and Sihra, 2008). MG-132 and CCPA/baclofen treatment did not alter paired-pulse depression in autaptic neurons (control, 21.1 ± 4.0%; MG-132, 24.3 ± 5.7%; CCPA/baclofen, 22.3 ± 5.8%; CCPA/baclofen + MG-132, 24.7 ± 7.9%; n = 14–15 neurons per condition; p > 0.05) (supplemental Fig. 1E, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). To confirm that a pr change should have been detected by paired-pulse measurements with our protocols and sample sizes, we tested the effect of acute 10 μm baclofen application on EPSC paired-pulse depression. As expected, paired-pulse modulation was significantly altered by acute baclofen (3.7 ± 5.2% depression in saline control; 32.3 ± 15.6% facilitation in baclofen; n = 12; p = 0.04) (supplemental Fig. 1F, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material). MG-132 alone did not alter the baseline EPSC amplitude, but it prevented silencing normally induced by GPCR agonist application (Fig. 7A,B). This was confirmed with FM1-43FX labeling of presynaptic terminals. The percentage of active glutamatergic presynaptic terminals was not altered by MG-132 alone, but coincubation with GPCR agonists prevented presynaptic silencing (Fig. 7C,D). These results suggest that GPCR agonist-induced silencing also recruits a proteasome-dependent mechanism, supporting our hypothesis that persistent GPCR-dependent depression uses similar signaling cascades as depolarization-induced silencing. This also supports our model that depolarization-induced silencing occurs through a calcium-independent inhibitory G-protein-dependent mechanism that ultimately depends on activation of the UPS.
Discussion
Adaptive modulation at synapses has been studied extensively, but induction mechanisms remain unclear (see Pozo and Goda, 2010). We tested whether calcium and G-protein signaling, two potential upstream regulators of cAMP, are necessary for presynaptic muting. Surprisingly, depolarization-induced presynaptic silencing is not dependent on calcium rises from extracellular or intracellular sources. Depolarization-induced muting is pertussis toxin sensitive, implicating Gi/o signaling, and prolonged, direct activation of two classes of Gi/o-linked GPCRs induces silencing, suggesting novel presynaptic Gi/o-induced modulation. Blocking five receptor classes failed to prevent depolarization-induced silencing, suggesting that G-protein signaling is activated by a pathway that remains untested. Furthermore, GPCR-dependent silencing is proteasome dependent, as documented previously for depolarization-induced silencing. Our results support the model that prolonged depolarization upregulates inhibitory G-protein signaling, possibly independent of receptor activation, to ultimately degrade vital regulators of neurotransmission.
Calcium and persistent forms of synaptic plasticity
Calcium from neither the extracellular space (Fig. 1) nor intracellular stores (Fig. 2) is necessary for adaptive presynaptic silencing. This result is surprising because multiple persistent forms of hippocampal synaptic plasticity depend on calcium (Dunwiddie et al., 1978; Wigström et al., 1979; Lynch et al., 1983; Williams and Johnston, 1989; Wickens and Abraham, 1991; Mulkey and Malenka, 1992; Xie et al., 1992; Tzounopoulos et al., 1998; Patenaude et al., 2003; Izumi et al., 2008; Kellogg et al., 2009). Other adaptive forms of plasticity, including synaptic scaling and plasticity of intrinsic excitability, require changes in intracellular calcium (Thiagarajan et al., 2002; Cudmore and Turrigiano, 2004; Frank et al., 2006; Ibata et al., 2008; Wu et al., 2008). Although it is possible that depolarization-induced presynaptic silencing requires postsynaptic depolarization, our BAPTA-AM experiments exclude an important induction role for any source of calcium, including presynaptic, postsynaptic, and glial sources. These results are consistent with our previous finding that calcium-sensitive adenylyl cyclase isoforms play no role in silencing induction (Moulder et al., 2008).
Depolarization-induced muting is among a few examples of calcium-independent plasticity. These include short-term, frequency-dependent synaptic depression (Betz, 1970; Stevens and Tsujimoto, 1995; Garcia-Perez et al., 2008), transient modulation by nontraditional messengers such as nitric oxide and steroid hormones (Meffert et al., 1996; Zadran et al., 2009), and some forms of hippocampal mGluR-dependent long-term depression (Fitzjohn et al., 2001; Ireland and Abraham, 2009). Because none of these mechanisms is implicated in presynaptic muting, the silencing described herein appears to be unique.
Presynaptic inhibition by G-protein activation
Acute activation of pertussis toxin-sensitive presynaptic and postsynaptic inhibitory G-proteins induces well-known, rapidly reversible effects on calcium currents and inwardly rectifying potassium channels (Brown and Sihra, 2008). Although acute presynaptic depression elicited by Gi/o stimulation persists for at least 4 h in the continued presence of agonist (Wetherington and Lambert, 2002a,b), several major features distinguish the depolarization-induced and GPCR-induced presynaptic silencing observed here from this classical presynaptic GPCR-induced depression.
First, acute Gi/o activation causes synaptic depression with rapid onset and rapid offset during agonist withdrawal (Brown and Sihra, 2008). Presynaptic muting, in contrast, requires prolonged stimulation, and recovery requires hours (Moulder et al., 2004). GPCR agonist-induced silencing failed to recover within 10 min in imaging experiments and persisted for at least 1 h after agonist removal in our electrophysiology experiments (Figs. 6, 7). Furthermore, only two of five receptor classes tested induced presynaptic silencing. It is possible that persistent silencing requires stronger Gi/o stimulation than acute depression, because CB1 and mGluR stimulation yielded weak acute depression and no detectable silencing (Fig. 5C,E and supplemental Fig. 3, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material).
Second, acute depression depends on Gβγ-dependent presynaptic inhibition of calcium channels to reduce vesicle pr (Brown and Sihra, 2008). Decreases in pr are reflected as increased paired-pulse ratios (Dobrunz and Stevens, 1997; Zucker and Regehr, 2002). Depolarization-induced and GPCR-induced muting did not alter paired-pulse ratios (supplemental Fig. 1, available at www.jneurosci.org as supplemental material) and may involve degradation of important presynaptic proteins through the UPS (Jiang et al., 2010). This is an unusual profile for presynaptic change and might explain why GPCR-induced silencing has been overlooked. Thus, persistent GPCR-dependent presynaptic silencing invokes different mechanisms than acute GPCR-dependent synaptic depression.
Other forms of slowly induced synaptic depression dependent on GPCRs have been described in the hippocampus. For example, GPCR activation is necessary for certain forms of long-term depression or depotentiation of long-term potentiation (Oliet et al., 1997; Palmer et al., 1997; Chevaleyre and Castillo, 2004; Izumi and Zorumski, 2008; Kellogg et al., 2009). Although a presynaptic change has been implicated in some of these examples, presynaptic muting has not been implicated because these are calcium-dependent and/or require activation of receptors that we have shown are unnecessary for depolarization-induced presynaptic silencing.
The finding that prolonged A1 and GABAB receptor activation induces silencing is reminiscent of evidence for tonic GPCR activation in situ. A1 receptors and GABAB receptors are constitutively active at low levels under physiological conditions (Canning and Leung, 2000; Thümmler and Dunwiddie, 2000; Jensen et al., 2003; Kukley et al., 2005) (but see Ariwodola and Weiner, 2004). Thus, GPCR-induced silencing may contribute to basally silent terminals in situ. Evidence for basal GPCR contributions to presynaptically mute synapses in our cultures was limited (Fig. 5B), possibly because of much larger dilution of transmitters than occurs in situ. Because evoked accumulation of adenosine and GABA likely achieves higher local concentrations over more prolonged periods in intact tissue (Mitchell et al., 1993; Masino and Dunwiddie, 1999; de Groote and Linthorst, 2007; Ghijsen et al., 2007), GPCR-induced silencing could play a dynamic role in sculpting the pattern of silent and active terminals.
Relationship between G-proteins and proteasome activity
Multiple downstream signaling cascades are activated by GPCRs. Our data implicate that, ultimately, GPCR agonist-induced presynaptic silencing is dependent on activity of the UPS because the proteasome inhibitor MG-132 blocked silencing induction (Fig. 7). One potential model is that inhibition of adenylyl cyclase by G-proteins decreases cAMP accumulation and associated protein phosphorylation by protein kinase A. We speculate that phosphorylation may protect key presynaptic proteins from degradation by the UPS. This view is consistent with evidence that downregulation of cAMP signaling leads to silencing (Moulder et al., 2008) and that depolarization-induced silencing is proteasome dependent (Jiang et al., 2010). In addition Munc13-1 and Rim1α, presynaptic proteins essential for vesicle priming (Augustin et al., 1999; Betz et al., 2001; Koushika et al., 2001), are selectively degraded relative to other presynaptic proteins after stimuli that induce presynaptic silencing (Jiang et al., 2010). This mirrors work in Drosophila showing that the Munc13 ortholog Dunc13 is regulated in a G-protein-dependent, cAMP-dependent, and proteasome-dependent manner (Aravamudan and Broadie, 2003) as well as work at the calyx of Held showing that GABAB receptor activation inhibits vesicle recruitment via reduced cAMP levels (Sakaba and Neher, 2003). Because Rim1α is a protein kinase A and UPS substrate (Lonart et al., 2003; Yao et al., 2007), phosphorylation-induced Rim1α stability could link increased cAMP levels to preserved presynaptic function. This speculative scenario does not preclude the possibility that induction stimuli modulate proteasome activity directly in a cAMP-independent manner (Jiang et al., 2010). Future work may clarify whether cAMP-independent G-protein signaling upregulates proteasome activity or whether depolarization increases proteasome activity by a pathway divorced from G-protein signaling.
G-protein signaling in depolarization-induced silencing
Although depolarization-induced presynaptic silencing requires activation of inhibitory G-proteins, how G-proteins are activated during prolonged depolarization remains unclear. We excluded four primary candidate GPCRs as well as metabotropic actions of kainate receptors (Figs. 4, 5). Depolarization may cause release of a ligand for Gi/o-linked GPCRs that we did not test. For example, neuromodulatory GPCRs such as dopamine and serotonin receptors are possible candidates (Catapano and Manji, 2007), but monoaminergic cells are absent from our cultures, making ligand presence extremely unlikely. Furthermore, the calcium independence of presynaptic muting excludes an important role of synaptic release of these potential mediators. Clearly, if a ligand is involved in induction of depolarization-induced muting, it is released through a nonclassical mechanism. Alternatively, cell-autonomous mechanisms such as voltage-sensitive proteins, including inhibitory GPCRs activated directly by voltage, could mediate silencing induction (Reddy et al., 1995; Ben-Chaim et al., 2006; Parnas and Parnas, 2007; Okamura et al., 2009). One intriguing possibility is that of receptor-independent activation of G-proteins, for instance, through recently described activators of G-protein signaling (AGS) (Blumer et al., 2007). AGS proteins interact with G-protein subunits to initiate downstream signaling in the absence of receptor activation. Some AGS proteins are expressed in hippocampal neurons, but it is unclear how they are activated (Fang et al., 2000; Blumer et al., 2002; Takahashi et al., 2003).
Although we have learned much about the induction of presynaptic silencing in excitatory neurons (Tong et al., 1996; Ma et al., 1999; Moulder et al., 2004, 2006, 2008; Voronin and Cherubini, 2004; Atasoy and Kavalali, 2006; Yao et al., 2006; Jiang et al., 2010), many segments of the signaling cascade have remained obscure. We show here that calcium-independent, G-protein-dependent, and proteasome-dependent mechanisms induce presynaptic muting, suggesting a unique form of synaptic plasticity. Because adaptive synaptic plasticity may counteract potentially damaging changes in neuronal activity (Turrigiano and Nelson, 2004), additional clarification of silencing induction pathways may lead to new therapeutic strategies.
Footnotes
- Received September 22, 2010.
- Revision received October 28, 2010.
- Accepted November 2, 2010.
-
This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Grants GM08151 (D.C.C.), NS066611 (D.C.C.), NS032636 (Mark P. Goldberg, supporting K.L.H.), NS036265 (Mark P. Goldberg, supporting K.L.H.), and MH78823 (S.M.) and National Institutes of Health Neuroscience Blueprint Core Grant P30NS057105 (to Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO). We thank Ann Benz and Amanda Taylor for assistance with cultures, Xiaoping Jiang for contributions to the imaging experiments, Charles Zorumski for helpful comments on this manuscript, and Timothy Holy, Robert Gereau, Narendrakumar Ramanan, and laboratory members for advice and discussion.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Steven Mennerick, Department of Psychiatry, Washington University in St. Louis, 660 South Euclid Avenue, Campus Box 8134, St. Louis, MO 63110. menneris{at}psychiatry.wustl.edu
- Copyright © 2011 the authors 0270-6474/11/310979-13$15.00/0