Abstract
Brain iron is vital to multiple aspects of brain function, including oxidative metabolism, myelination, and neurotransmitter synthesis. Atypical iron concentration in the basal ganglia is associated with neurodegenerative disorders in aging and cognitive deficits. However, the normative development of brain iron concentration in adolescence and its relationship to cognition are less well understood. Here, we address this gap in a longitudinal sample of 922 humans aged 8–26 years at the first visit (M = 15.1, SD = 3.72; 336 males, 486 females) with up to four multiecho T2* scans each. Using this sample of 1236 imaging sessions, we assessed the longitudinal developmental trajectories of tissue iron in the basal ganglia. We quantified tissue iron concentration using R2* relaxometry within four basal ganglia regions, including the caudate, putamen, nucleus accumbens, and globus pallidus. The longitudinal development of R2* was modeled using generalized additive mixed models (GAMMs) with splines to capture linear and nonlinear developmental processes. We observed significant increases in R2* across all regions, with the greatest and most prolonged increases occurring in the globus pallidus and putamen. Further, we found that the developmental trajectory of R2* in the putamen is significantly related to individual differences in cognitive ability, such that greater cognitive ability is increasingly associated with greater iron concentration through late adolescence and young-adulthood. Together, our results suggest a prolonged period of basal ganglia iron enrichment that extends into the mid-twenties, with diminished iron concentration associated with poorer cognitive ability during late adolescence.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Brain tissue iron is essential to healthy brain function. Atypical basal ganglia tissue iron levels have been linked to impaired cognition in iron deficient children and adults with neurodegenerative disorders. However, the normative developmental trajectory of basal ganglia iron concentration during adolescence and its association with cognition are less well understood. In the largest study of tissue iron development yet reported, we characterize the developmental trajectory of tissue iron concentration across the basal ganglia during adolescence and provide evidence that diminished iron content is associated with poorer cognitive performance even in healthy youth. These results highlight the transition from adolescence to adulthood as a period of dynamic maturation of tissue iron concentration in the basal ganglia.
- adolescence
- basal ganglia
- cognition
- development
- iron
- R2*
Introduction
Brain tissue iron is essential to healthy brain function. Distinct from heme-iron, tissue iron binds to the intracellular protein ferritin and is stored in neurons, oligodendrocytes, astrocytes, and microglia (Todorich et al., 2009; Mills et al., 2010; Ward et al., 2014). Within these cells, iron is required as a cofactor for multiple fundamental neural processes. As part of the process of cellular respiration, iron is necessary for ATP synthesis (Ward et al., 2014; Paul et al., 2017). In oligodendrocytes, iron supports the high metabolic demand of myelin maintenance and synthesis and functions as a cofactor for lipid synthesis (Connor and Menzies, 1996; Todorich et al., 2009; Möller et al., 2019). Iron is also a necessary cofactor for tyrosine hydroxylase (Ramsey et al., 1996) and monoamine oxidase (Youdim et al., 1976; Lu et al., 2017; Youdim, 2018), enzymes essential for dopamine synthesis, as well as tryptophan hydroxylase (Kuhn et al., 1980), which is necessary for serotonin synthesis. Tissue iron is heterogeneously distributed in the brain, with the greatest iron concentration in basal ganglia structures such as the caudate, putamen, nucleus accumbens, and globus pallidus (Hallgren and Sourander, 1958; Thomas et al., 1993; Brass et al., 2006). The reason for pronounced iron enrichment of the basal ganglia and the mechanism by which it occurs are not fully understood, but the basal ganglia are rich in dopamine and play a critical role in a multitude of cognitive and motor functions as part of the corticobasal ganglia-thalamo-cortical loops (Haber and Knutson, 2010).
Brain tissue iron content is homeostatically regulated (Moos, 2002; Burdo and Connor, 2003), and atypical iron concentration in the basal ganglia is associated with neurological disorders and impaired cognitive function. Abnormally high brain iron content in aging is associated with Parkinson's disease (Lv et al., 2011; Zucca et al., 2017), Alzheimer's disease (Griffiths and Crossman, 1993; Bartzokis et al., 2000), and Huntington's disease (Bartzokis et al., 1999; Bartzokis and Tishler, 2000; Kell, 2010), as well as cognitive decline (Pujol et al., 1992; Penke et al., 2012; Adamo et al., 2014; Daugherty and Raz, 2015; Ghadery et al., 2015). In contrast, iron deficiency in early development (e.g., gestation through childhood) leads to atypically low brain tissue iron concentration (Erikson et al., 2000, 2001; Beard, 2003) and is similarly associated with abnormal basal ganglia dopaminergic neurobiology and neurochemistry (Erikson et al., 2001; Lozoff and Georgieff, 2006; Jellen et al., 2013), and cognitive impairment (Grantham-McGregor and Ani, 2001; Halterman et al., 2001; McCann and Ames, 2007; Shafir et al., 2008; Carter et al., 2010; Lozoff, 2011). While substantial work has been done to delineate these critical links between atypical brain tissue iron concentration and neurocognitive function in early development and late-life, less is known about how tissue iron concentration normatively develops during adolescence, and whether normative variation in the developmental trajectory of tissue iron concentration is related to cognitive ability.
Here, we address this question by assessing the development of tissue iron concentration in a normative sample of 922 adolescents and young adults (aged 8–26y) with up to four longitudinal measurements per participant, resulting in 1236 total scans after strict quality assessment. In this sample, we assess iron content using R2* relaxation (1/T2*), which is linearly related to postmortem tissue iron concentration (Langkammer et al., 2010; Sedlacik et al., 2014). We used generalized additive mixed models to characterize the normative developmental trajectory of tissue iron concentration across the basal ganglia and to assess whether these normative trajectories vary with individual differences in multiple domains of cognitive ability. As described below, we find that tissue iron concentration continues to increase throughout adolescence and into young adulthood, with the greatest increases occurring in the putamen and globus pallidus. Further, we find that individual differences in cognitive performance are associated with the developmental enrichment of iron concentration in the putamen.
Materials and Methods
Participants.
Neuroimaging data were obtained from a community-based sample of 1543 youth that were part of the Philadelphia Neurodevelopmental Cohort and related follow-up studies (Jirsaraie et al., 2019; Roalf et al., 2019) (ages 8.17–26.92 at first visit, M = 15.19, SD = 3.75; male/female = 728/819). This neuroimaging dataset included up to five longitudinal visits per participant, resulting in 2321 total scans. From this original sample, 309 participants (419 scans) were excluded based on health criteria, including psychoactive medication use at the time of study, medical problems that could impact brain function, a history of psychiatric hospitalization, and gross structural brain abnormalities. A total of 641 scans were excluded for poor T2* image quality, including head motion, hyperintensities, and susceptibility artifacts, which was assessed with manual quality assurance procedures (see below). In sum, following health exclusions and rigorous quality assurance we retained 922 participants (ages 8.17–26.92 at first visit, M = 15.1, SD = 3.72; male/female = 436/486) with up to four longitudinal neuroimaging visits per participant, resulting in 1236 total scans (Fig. 1). From this neuroimaging sample, cognitive performance data (described below) was available for 818 individuals, with up to three time points per individual (ages 8.17–26.92 at first visit, M = 14.84, SD = 3.57; male/female = 389/429), resulting in 1086 total cognitive assessments.
Final neuroimaging sample characteristics. A, Final imaging sample—after exclusions—was composed of 922 individuals and 1236 total scans. Each point depicts an individual scan, with lines connecting individual participants. Participant sex is indicated as red (female) or blue (male). B, Stacked histogram of acquisitions used for scans across the age range (Table 1).
Structural image processing.
A high-resolution T1-weighted image was acquired for all visits for the purpose of nonlinear registration to a study specific template. The study-specific template was generated from a sample of 120 PNC subjects balanced across sex, race, and age bins using the “buildtemplateparallel” procedure in ANTs (Avants et al., 2011a). Each participant's high-resolution structural image was processed using Advanced Normalization Tools (ANTs) software and the structural processing pipeline included bias field correction (Tustison et al., 2010), brain extraction using a hybrid registration/segmentation approach (Avants et al., 2011b), and diffeomorphic registration to the study-specific template using SyN deformation provided by ANTs (Avants et al., 2008; Klein et al., 2009).
R2* acquisition and preprocessing.
T2* data were estimated from multiecho gradient echo (mGRE) images acquired for each participant. Sequence parameters are summarized in Table 1. It should be noted that a subset of the data was acquired with minor variations in acquisition parameters. However, all scans were acquired on the same scanner, 99% used the same repetition time (TR, 1000 ms), and 86% used the same echo spacing (ΔTE, 2.58 ms). Notably, for the minority of participants for whom ΔTE varied, the difference between acquisitions was only 0.12 ms. As a conservative step, to ensure that the variability in T2* acquisition parameters did not affect our results, T2* data were harmonized across acquisitions before statistical analysis using ComBat (Johnson et al., 2007) (see below).
Sequence counts and descriptions
Acquired mGRE images were first smoothed with a 3 mm FWHM kernel. T2* was calculated from the pixel intensity of the two echo images according to the following:
where ITE1 and ITE2 refer to the signal intensity at the first and second echo. T2* images were brain extracted using coregistration to the brain-extracted structural image (above). T2* images were finally spatially normalized to the study-specific template by concatenating the affine registration between the T2* image and structural image and the nonlinear registration of the structural image to the study template. R2* was calculated as
.
R2* quality assurance.
All R2* images were visually inspected by BL for data quality and the presence of artifacts, including motion artifacts, susceptibility artifacts, extreme values, and T2* hyperintensities, any of which could impact the R2* signal in the basal ganglia. Visual inspection was performed blind to all experimental variables. Each image was rated as “pass” or “fail,” and only images that passed inspection were included in statistical analyses. This procedure assured that only high-quality scans would be included in analyses (Fig. 2).
Outcome of quality assurance procedures. The voxelwise mean of all R2* scans that passed quality assurance (n = 1236) is depicted on the left, and the voxelwise mean of all R2* scans that failed (n = 641) is depicted on the right.
Region of interest identification.
We evaluated basal ganglia regions of interest as in prior work (Langkammer et al., 2010; Sedlacik et al., 2014; Daugherty and Raz, 2015), including the caudate, putamen, nucleus accumbens, and globus pallidus. These areas were identified in template space by a segmentation of the structural template brain using ANTs joint label fusion (Wang et al., 2013). Regional R2* values were calculated as the mean across all voxels in each region across both hemispheres. As the regional values were extracted from spatially normalized data, the number of voxels did not differ across participants.
Cognitive assessment.
The Penn computerized neurocognitive battery (CNB) was administered to 818 participants with up to three time points per participant (ages 8.17–26.92 at first visit, M = 14.84, SD = 3.57; male/female = 389/429), resulting in 1067 total cognitive assessments. The CNB consists of 14 tests adapted from tasks applied in functional neuroimaging to evaluate a broad range of cognitive domains (Gur et al., 2010, 2012). These domains include executive control (abstraction and mental flexibility, attention, working memory), complex cognition (verbal reasoning, nonverbal reasoning, spatial processing), episodic memory (verbal, facial, spatial), social cognition (emotion identification, emotion intensity differentiation, age differentiation), and motor speed. Accuracy and speed for each task were z-transformed and combined into an efficiency variable. Factor scores were calculated separately for an overall factor and four correlated subfactors. For the overall factor, tests were modeled in a unidimensional (one-factor) model with least-squares extraction. The subfactor scores (complex cognition, executive function, social cognition, and memory) were calculated using an exploratory factor analysis (EFA) with least-squares extraction and oblique geomin rotation. The dimensionality was determined theoretically based on the design of the CNB and prior work (Moore et al., 2015; Gur and Gur, 2016). Note that unidimensional models were used for the overall scores despite multidimensionality because the ratio of first to second eigenvalues for efficiency were all >3.0, providing some support for use of a unidimensional model (Reise et al., 2015).
Group-level statistical analysis.
Before all statistical analyses, R2* data were harmonized to control for subtle differences in T2* acquisition parameters using ComBat, a batch-effect correction tool developed for genomics data (Johnson et al., 2007) and previously applied to harmonize multisite neuroimaging data, including in developmental studies (Fortin et al., 2017, 2018). ComBat removes unwanted interacquisition variability while preserving variability in biological variables of interest. ComBat was performed separately for observations included in developmental models (including covariates for age, sex, and visit number) and cognitive models (including covariates for age, sex, visit number, and cognitive performance).
We first tested for mean level differences among our basal ganglia regions of interest. These region of interest comparisons were performed as post hoc contrasts of estimated marginal means from a linear mixed effects model that included covariates for age, sex, and visit number. Significance values were adjusted using the Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons.
To assess developmental trajectories of R2*, we used splines within a generalized additive mixed model (GAMM) to flexibly capture linear or nonlinear age effects and account for longitudinal repeated measures. For each region of interest, we first tested for a significant age-by-sex interaction (i.e., factor-smooth interaction). If the interaction was not significant, the GAMM was refit removing the interaction term and modeling sex as an additive covariate. To test for windows of significant change across the age range, we calculated the first derivative of the smooth function of age from the GAMM model using finite differences, and then generated a simultaneous 95% confidence interval of the derivative (Simpson, 2018) using the gratia package in R. Intervals of significant change were identified as areas where the simultaneous confidence interval of the derivative does not include zero.
We next investigated the relationship between cognition and R2* and whether developmental trajectories of R2* interacted with cognitive ability. To test for a main effect of overall cognitive performance, we fit a GAMM with additive smooth terms for cognition and age and a main effect of sex. To assess whether developmental trajectories of R2* varied by cognitive ability, we allowed the smoothed age effect in the GAMM to interact with cognitive performance. All GAMMs were fit in R using the 'gamm' function from the mgcv package (Wood, 2004, 2017). To model this continuous interaction, we compared two interaction models within the GAMM framework: a fully nonlinear interaction (bivariate smooth) and a nonlinear-by-linear interaction in which the smooth effect of age interacts with a linear effect of cognitive performance (varying coefficient model). Model selection was performed using Bayesian information criterion (BIC), and the significance of the interaction term was evaluated for the selected model. Finally, we evaluated three-way age-by-cognition-by-sex interactions, which were not significant and not considered further.
All models included visit number as a covariate factor of no interest and participant ID as a random effect. For all GAMMs, significance of the fixed effect smooth terms was assessed in a fixed degree-of-freedom context to ensure stable and accurate estimation, and all p-values of interest were confirmed using a parametric bootstrap likelihood ratio test (pbkrtest package) for significance estimation in the mixed model context (Halekoh and Højsgaard, 2014). Multiple-comparison adjustment was performed with Bonferroni correction.
Code accessibility.
Custom R code for all primary statistical analyses as well as example bash code for R2* calculation is available at https://github.com/PennBBL/Larsen_IronDevelopment.
Results
Regional differences in iron concentration
Model estimated regional R2* values—covarying for sex and visit number—closely matched prior studies (Hallgren and Sourander, 1958; Bartzokis et al., 1997, 2007; Aquino et al., 2009; Péran et al., 2009; Haacke et al., 2010 p.201; Langkammer et al., 2010; Sedlacik et al., 2014; Ghadery et al., 2015; Hect et al., 2018). Globus pallidus had the greatest model estimated marginal mean (EMM) R2* (EMM = 22.33, confidence interval {CI} = [22.22, 22.44]), followed by nucleus accumbens (EMM = 17.59, CI = [17.48, 17.69]), putamen (EMM = 16.96, CI = [16.85, 17.07]), and caudate (EMM = 15.17, CI = [15.06, 15.28]) (Fig. 3). All regions significantly differed from each other (Table 2).
Sample distribution of R2* across basal ganglia regions of interest. Individual points represent regional R2* values for individual scans. Boxplots indicate the median value, as well as the first and third quartiles of the sample distribution. GP, Globus pallidus; NAcc, nucleus accumbens; Pu, putamen; Ca, caudate.
Model estimated differences in R2* across the basal ganglia controlling for age, sex, and visit
Age-related increases in R2* across the basal ganglia
R2* significantly increased with age across all basal ganglia regions of interest while covarying for sex and visit number (Fig. 4, Table 3). There was a significant interaction with sex in the globus pallidus such that R2* began to reach plateau at age 22.2y in females and continued to increase in males. Females tended to have greater R2* regardless of age in the nucleus accumbens, though the effect did not survive at the Bonferroni adjusted α of 0.0125 (t = 2.26, p = 0.024). Analysis of the derivatives of the fitted age trajectories identified that R2* significantly increased between ages 8.2–17.3 years in nucleus accumbens, 10.2–26.9 years in the putamen, ages 8.2–16.1 years in the caudate, and 8.2–25.9 years for males and 8.2–22.2 years for females in the globus pallidus (Fig. 4).
Development of R2* in the basal ganglia. Plots display fitted R2* trajectories from GAMM models plotted by age for males (blue) and females (red). Individual points represent individual scans, with lines indicating scans from the same participant. Bars below the x-axis depict the derivative of the fitted smooth function. The filled portion of the bar indicates periods where the magnitude of derivative is significant, with the saturation of the fill representing the value of derivative in units of ΔR2*/year. *p < 0.05 after Bonferroni correction; +p < 0.1 after Bonferroni correction; n.s., not significant.
GAMM regression table for age, sex, and interactions
Developmental trajectories of R2* are related to cognitive performance
We next investigated whether the developmental trajectories we observed varied with of cognitive ability. We observed that the developmental trajectory of R2* in the putamen significantly interacted with overall cognitive performance at a Bonferroni-adjusted α of .0125 (s(Age):Overall F = 3.67, p = 0.006; Fig. 5). This developmental interaction was such that the association between cognitive ability and R2* emerged during adolescence, with poorer performance being increasingly associated with diminished R2* levels. To understand the specific cognitive domains that contributed to this effect, we conducted specificity analyses focusing on four cognitive domains: complex cognition, social cognition, executive function, and memory. Cognitive factor loadings and correlations can be found in Table 4. We found that developmental trajectories of R2* most strongly interacted with complex cognition performance (s(Age):Complex F = 3.94, p = 0.004; Fig. 6A). A similar pattern was observed for social cognition (s(Age):Social F = 2.67, p = 0.031; Fig. 6B) and executive function (s(Age):Executive F = 2.66, p = 0.032; Fig. 6C), though the effects were not significant at a Bonferroni-adjusted α of .0125. The effect was not significant for memory performance (s(Age):Memory F = 1.03, p = 0.39; Fig. 6D).
The development of R2* in the putamen is moderated by overall cognitive performance. To visualize the interaction between continuous variables, fitted lines are displayed that depict the relationship between age and putamen R2* for the top 5% (high performers) and bottom 95% (low performers) of the range for overall cognitive performance. Individual points represent individual scans, with connecting lines indicating scans from the same participant. The color of the points represents the overall cognitive performance z score (higher values indicate more efficient performance).
Exploratory factor analysis of longitudinal CNB tests
Cognitive moderation effects on the development of putamen R2* across subdomains of cognition. Fitted values of R2* are plotted as functions of age and complex cognition (A), social cognition (B), executive function (C), and memory (D). To visualize the continuous interactions, fitted lines are displayed that depict the relationship between age and putamen R2* for the top 5% (High performers) and bottom 95% (Low performers) of the range for each cognitive measure. Individual points represent individual scans, with connecting lines indicating scans from the same participant. The color of the points represents the cognitive z score (higher values indicating more efficient performance).
Discussion
Here, we capitalized upon a large sample of youth with longitudinal neuroimaging data to characterize the developmental trajectories of tissue iron concentration in the basal ganglia. We demonstrated that brain tissue iron increases throughout adolescence, with the greatest and most prolonged increases occurring in the putamen and globus pallidus. Within the globus pallidus, we also found evidence for a more protracted developmental trajectory in males relative to females. Further, we found that the developmental trajectory of tissue iron in the putamen is associated with individual differences in cognitive ability, such that individuals with diminished iron accumulation also have poorer cognitive performance in late adolescence and early adulthood.
Prior work has indicated that brain tissue iron increases most rapidly during the first two decades of life before beginning to reach a developmental asymptote (Hallgren and Sourander, 1958; Aquino et al., 2009; Wang et al., 2012; Larsen and Luna, 2015; Hect et al., 2018; Peterson et al., 2019a). The findings presented here generally support this pattern, identifying windows of significant age-related increases in R2* up to ages 16–17 in the nucleus accumbens and the caudate. However, we find that there are prolonged windows of rapid iron accumulation in the putamen and globus pallidus, where we observed significant increases in tissue iron that continued into early adulthood. Of note for future lifespan studies of the basal ganglia, the magnetic susceptibility of iron that allows for its quantification using R2* also shortens T1 and T2 relaxation time (Vymazal et al., 1995; Ogg and Steen, 1998; Stüber et al., 2014). Thus, the development of basal ganglia iron concentration may impact other MR metrics commonly used in developmental studies, such as T1-weighted imaging and diffusion weighted imaging (DWI) (Pfefferbaum et al., 2010; Peterson et al., 2019b). Given the pronounced developmental increases in brain iron concentration observed in this study, future work applying DWI or T1-based imaging metrics in developmental studies should ideally consider and control for the impact of iron.
To date, there has not been a thorough examination of sex differences in tissue iron accumulation during adolescence. In the globus pallidus, we observed an earlier developmental plateau in females, occurring ∼age 22, relative to a continued increase throughout the observed age range in males. This finding indicates a potentially longer window of iron accumulation for males during this critical developmental stage. These findings may be related to earlier work suggesting increased incidence of iron deficiency in adolescent and young adult females. The prevalence of iron deficiency in females increases from childhood to adolescence, and among nonpregnant adolescent and adult females in the United States is as high as 15% (compared with <3% in males) (Halterman et al., 2001; Cogswell et al., 2009; U.S. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2012), and this proportion rises even higher, >30%, in female university students (Worthington-Roberts et al., 1988; Houston et al., 1997; Wilcock et al., 2004; Hawk et al., 2012; Blanton, 2013). Although we did not assess dietary iron intake or serum ferritin levels in this sample, it is possible that the developmental sex differences we observe in the globus pallidus reflect developmental increases in the prevalence of iron deficiency in females during this developmental window.
Atypically low iron content in basal ganglia has been associated with cognitive impairment in studies of iron deficiency in early development (Beard and Connor, 2003; Beard, 2003; Carter et al., 2010; Lozoff, 2011). Here we demonstrate a similar association between reduced basal ganglia iron concentration and cognitive ability occurring within a normative developmental sample during late-adolescence and adulthood. Specifically, we find that an association between poorer overall cognitive performance and diminished iron concentration in the putamen emerges during late adolescence. When we assessed this relationship across individual subdomains of cognition, we observed that the effect was strongest in the domain of complex cognition. Adolescence has been characterized as a critical period of development of complex, higher-order cognitive functions (e.g., Larsen and Luna, 2018), and our results suggest that continued enrichment of basal ganglia iron concentration may be an important component of this developmental process. In late life, the association between brain iron concentration and cognition is reversed such that elevated brain iron is associated with neurodegeneration and impaired cognition (Bartzokis and Tishler, 2000; Ghadery et al., 2015; Zucca et al., 2017). Changing associations between brain iron content and cognitive ability during development and aging are likely driven by different underlying mechanisms. Brain iron is necessary for a myriad of critical neurobiological processes, including cellular respiration (Paul et al., 2017), myelination (Connor and Menzies, 1996; Möller et al., 2019), and neurotransmitter synthesis (Youdim et al., 1976; Lu et al., 2017). In development, a deficiency in this critical resource may impair these neurobiological processes, impairing cognition. In contrast, in aging, a breakdown of the essential homeostatic process of iron storage and metabolism can increase the risk of iron-related oxidative stress and accelerate neurodegeneration (Bartzokis et al., 2007; Núñez et al., 2012; Li and Reichmann, 2016). Longitudinal studies assessing how brain iron impacts cognitive health across the lifespan are needed.
Limitations
Despite the strengths of this study, three limitations should be noted. First, we did not collect any data on dietary iron content nor obtained serum iron status. As such, though the participants in this study were generally healthy, we cannot quantify the prevalence of iron deficiency in this sample. Thus, it is not clear whether the neurodevelopmental associations between cognitive performance and basal ganglia iron concentration we observe here are mechanistically driven by iron deficiency, as in early developmental studies (Grantham-McGregor and Ani, 2001; Beard, 2003), or if these effects are occurring within individuals with normative dietary iron content. Future work assessing brain iron concentration, peripheral iron concentration, and measures of dietary iron status should address this question directly. Additionally, as this work is focused on a normative developmental sample, and future work should investigate how developmental abnormalities of iron are associated with neuropsychiatric illness. Second, a small subset of T2* scans were acquired with minor variations in acquisition parameters. Though we do not expect these variations to significantly impact T2* estimation, we rigorously addressed this possibility using ComBat harmonization (Johnson et al., 2007), which has been effective in harmonizing neuroimaging datasets (Fortin et al., 2017, 2018). Third, the data acquired to calculate R2* was generated with a two-echo mGRE sequence with somewhat lower in-plane resolution (3.75 × 3.75 mm) than has been used in other studies (e.g., Langkammer et al., 2010; Sedlacik et al., 2014). Calculating T2* from a greater number of echo images with higher in-plane resolution can yield greater sensitivity (Anderson et al., 2001). We compensated for this limitation by using strict quality assurance procedures and having a large sample for all statistical analyses.
Conclusion
This study leverages a large longitudinal sample to characterize the normative development of tissue iron concentration across the basal ganglia. Iron accumulation follows a nonlinear developmental trajectory that begins to plateau during adolescence in the nucleus accumbens and caudate and continues to increase into the twenties in the putamen and globus pallidus. Importantly, individual differences in cognitive ability are associated with differences in putamen iron concentration during the transition from adolescence to adulthood. Together, these results highlight the transition from adolescence to adulthood as a period of dynamic maturation of tissue iron concentration in the basal ganglia that may affect individual variability in complex cognitive performance in adulthood. Importantly, as iron supplementation has been found to improve cognitive performance in iron deficient adolescents (Bruner et al., 1996; Blanton, 2013), it is possible that efforts to enhance brain iron concentration through dietary iron supplementation may have beneficial effects on cognitive development during this critical period of development. Studies that prospectively evaluate the longitudinal impact of iron supplementation on cognition and brain development are necessary.
Footnotes
- Received October 9, 2019.
- Revision received December 19, 2019.
- Accepted January 4, 2020.
This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health (T32MH019112 to B.L., T32MH014654 to B.L., R01MH119219 to R.E.G. & R.C.G., R01MH117014 to R.C.G., U01-MH081902 to R.E.G., MH063381 to P.J.M., MH108895 to P.J.M., R01MH099156 to B.I.T., R01MH113565 to D.H.W., K01MH102609 to D.R.R., R01MH112847 to R.T.S. & T.D.S., R01MH113550 to T.D.S., R01MH120482 to T.D.S., R01MH107703 to T.D.S.); Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR396349 to J.B.).
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Bart Larsen at bart.larsen{at}pennmedicine.upenn.edu
- Copyright © 2020 the authors