Abstract
Accurately decoding external variables from observations of neural activity is a major challenge in systems neuroscience. Bayesian decoders, which provide probabilistic estimates, are some of the most widely used. Here we show how, in many common settings, the probabilistic predictions made by traditional Bayesian decoders are overconfident. That is, the estimates for the decoded stimulus or movement variables are more certain than they should be. We then show how Bayesian decoding with latent variables, taking account of low-dimensional shared variability in the observations, can improve calibration, although additional correction for overconfidence is still needed. Using data from males, we examine (1) decoding the direction of grating stimuli from spike recordings in the primary visual cortex in monkeys, (2) decoding movement direction from recordings in the primary motor cortex in monkeys, (3) decoding natural images from multiregion recordings in mice, and (4) decoding position from hippocampal recordings in rats. For each setting, we characterize the overconfidence, and we describe a possible method to correct miscalibration post hoc. Properly calibrated Bayesian decoders may alter theoretical results on probabilistic population coding and lead to brain–machine interfaces that more accurately reflect confidence levels when identifying external variables.
- Bayesian
- confidence
- decoding
- spikes
Significance Statement
Bayesian decoding is a statistical technique for making probabilistic predictions about external stimuli or movements based on recordings of neural activity. These predictions may be useful for robust brain–machine interfaces or for understanding perceptual or behavioral confidence. However, the probabilities produced by these models do not always match the observed outcomes. Just as a weather forecast predicting a 50% chance of rain may not accurately correspond to an outcome of rain 50% of the time, Bayesian decoders of neural activity can be miscalibrated as well. Here we identify and measure miscalibration of Bayesian decoders for neural spiking activity in a range of experimental settings. We compare multiple statistical models and demonstrate how overconfidence can be corrected.
Introduction
Decoding, estimating external variables given observations of neural activity, is a fundamental tool in systems neuroscience for understanding what information is present in specific brain signals and areas (deCharms and Zador, 2000; Kriegeskorte and Douglas, 2019). Decoders have been widely used for studying the representation of movement variables, such as speed, force, or position (Humphrey et al., 1970; Georgopoulos et al., 1986), the representation of visual stimuli (Warland et al., 1997; Quiroga and Panzeri, 2009), and the representation of sounds (Theunissen et al., 2004), touch (Diamond et al., 2008), odors (Uchida et al., 2014), and tastes (Lemon and Katz, 2007). Here we examine Bayesian decoders that estimate the probability of each possible stimulus or movement given neural observations (Sanger, 1996; Zhang et al., 1998; Koyama et al., 2010; Chen, 2013). Bayesian models explicitly represent the uncertainty about external variables, and this uncertainty may be useful for understanding perceptual/behavioral confidence (Vilares and Kording, 2011; Meyniel et al., 2015) or for creating more robust brain–machine interfaces (BMIs; Shanechi et al., 2016). However, Bayesian models are not always well calibrated (Degroot and Fienberg, 1983; Draper, 1995). Here we ask whether the uncertainty estimates for Bayesian decoders are correct.
With Bayesian decoders, the conditional probability of stimulus or movement variables given neural responses is calculated using the Bayes theorem (Quiroga and Panzeri, 2009). This posterior is the product of a likelihood that describes the probability of neural activity given external variables (an encoding model) and a prior that accounts for other knowledge about the external variable. This framework is very general and can be used to decode categorical or continuous variables in trial-by-trial designs or with continuous time series using spiking timing features or counts as well as other population neural signals (van Bergen et al., 2015; Lu et al., 2021). One common likelihood model for the counts of spiking activity is based on the Poisson distribution and the assumption that the neural responses are conditionally independent given their tuning to the external variable. However, since neural activity has shared (Arieli et al., 1996; Tsodyks et al., 1999) and non-Poisson variability (Amarasingham et al., 2006; Goris et al., 2014), recent studies have focused on better modeling latent structure and dispersion (Scott and Pillow, 2012). Modeling this shared and non-Poisson variability can improve decoding (Graf et al., 2011; Ghanbari et al., 2019).
In this paper, we compare Bayesian decoders with Poisson versus negative binomial noise models as well as decoders with or without latent variables with the goal of understanding how differences in the model structure affect the posterior uncertainty. In well-calibrated models, the posterior of the external variables should accurately reflect their true probability. For instance, a 95% credible interval—analogous to the confidence interval in frequentist descriptions—should have a 95% chance of containing the true value. However, miscalibration can occur due to model misspecification—when the data is generated by a process that does not match the model assumptions—or when there is unmodeled uncertainty about the model structure (Draper, 1995). Previous studies suggest that neural variability may be an important dimension of the neural code (Urai et al., 2022), and the uncertainty of neural population codes may determine perceptual/behavioral confidence (Knill and Pouget, 2004). Accurate descriptions of population uncertainty in experimental data may, thus, inform theoretical understanding. In this study, we illustrate the basic problem of miscalibration through simulations and evaluate calibration for experimental data.
We focus on several experimental settings: trial-by-trial decoding of the stimulus movement direction from the primary visual cortex (V1) and the reach direction from the primary motor cortex (M1), trial-by-trial decoding of categorical natural images from multiple brain regions, and time-series decoding of the animal position from hippocampal recordings (HCs). We find that using both negative binomial likelihoods and latent variables improves calibration. However, even with these improvements, Bayesian decoders are overconfident. To solve this problem, we introduce a post hoc correction for miscalibration that yields more accurate uncertainty estimates.
Materials and Methods
The code for the results in this paper is available at https://github.com/ihstevenson/latent_bayesian_decoding.
Data
To assess the calibration of Bayesian decoders, we use previously collected, publicly available data from (1) a macaque primary motor cortex during a center-out reaching task, (2) a macaque primary visual cortex during presentation of drifting or static sine-wave gratings, (3) mouse multiregion recordings during presentation of static natural images, and (4) a rat hippocampus (HC) during running on a linear track.
Data from the primary motor cortex (M1) were previously recorded from the arm area of an adult male macaque monkey during center-out reaches. Reaches were made in a 20 × 20 cm workspace while the animal was grasping a two-link manipulandum, and single units were recorded using a 100 electrode Utah array (400 mm spacing, 1.5 mm length, manually spike sorted manually—Plexon). On each trial, we analyzed spike counts during the window 150 ms before to 350 ms after the speed reached its half-max. Data and additional descriptions of the surgical procedure, behavioral task, and preprocessing are available in Walker and Kording (2013).
Data from the primary visual cortex (V1) were previously recorded and shared in the CRCNS PVC-11 dataset (Kohn and Smith, 2016). Single units were recorded using a 96 channel multielectrode array from an anesthetized adult male monkey (Macaca fascicularis, Monkey 3) during presentations of drifting sine-wave gratings (20 trials for each of 12 directions). On each trial, we analyzed spike counts between 200 ms and 1.2 s after stimulus onset. Detailed descriptions of the surgical procedure, stimulus presentation, and preprocessing can be found in Smith and Kohn (2008) and Kelly et al. (2010).
We also examine an additional previously recorded, shared dataset from the primary visual cortex where stimuli were presented with multiple contrasts (Berens et al., 2012). Here single units were recorded using custom-built tetrodes from an awake male monkey (Macaca mulatta). Static sine-wave gratings were presented with different contrasts. Here we use data from subject “D” recorded on April 17, 2002. Detailed descriptions of the surgical procedure, stimulus presentation, and preprocessing can be found in Ecker et al. (2010) and Berens et al. (2012).
Multiregion data (ABI) were analyzed from the Allen Institute for Brain Science—Visual Coding Neuropixels dataset (https://portal.brain-map.org/explore/circuits). Detailed descriptions of the surgical procedure, stimulus presentation, and preprocessing can be found in Siegle et al. (2021). Briefly, during the recordings, head-fixed mice were presented with visual stimuli (including Gabor patches, full-field drifting gratings, moving dots, and natural images and movies) while they were free to run on a wheel. We analyze spike sorted (Kilosort 2) single unit data from six Neuropixels arrays (electrophysiology session 742951821, a male wild-type C57BL/6J) with n = 267 single units (SNR > 3, rate > 1 spike/trial) responding to 118 natural images (4,873 trials in total).
Data from the HC were previously recorded from the dorsal HC of a male Long–Evans rat and shared in CRCNS hc-3 (Mizuseki et al., 2013). Recordings were made using an eight-shank silicon probe, each shank with eight recording sites, while the animal ran on a linear track, and single units were automatically spike sorted with KlustaKwik and refined with Klusters. Data from recording id ec014_468 were analyzed in 200 ms bins. Data and additional descriptions of the surgical procedure, behavioral task, and preprocessing are available in Mizuseki et al. (2014).
Encoding models
Our goal is to decode an external stimulus or movement variable
Poisson and negative binomial GLMs and GLLVMs
The Poisson GLM and negative binomial GLM model the spiking of neuron
i on trial
k as
Since the responses of different neurons may be correlated, the GLM does not generally capture noise correlations—dependencies between neurons beyond what the external variable induces. The GLLVMs extend the GLMs described above by including low-dimensional latent factors in the model for the mean parameters. In other words, the Poisson GLLVM and NB-GLLVM assume
In this basic form, the latent variable model is not identifiable, and we put several constraints on
In cases where the number of trials is relatively small, when p is large, or when the spiking is extremely sparse, both the GLM and GLLVM can overfit or fail to converge (Zhao and Iyengar, 2010). In addition to the Gaussian prior (i.e., L2 penalty) on
β, we also include a Gaussian prior
Approximate Bayesian decoding
Once the encoding model is fitted with training data
x and
y, we then decode the external variable
For the GLLVM, we additionally need to account for the latent variables. Since the data used for fitting the encoding model is not the same as decoding dataset, the latent state
Our goal is then to calculate the marginal predictive likelihood
Since the posterior distribution of
Greedy decoders
To better understand how the composition of the population affects our results, we compare GLM and GLLVM decoders that use the full population of neurons to those with only a subset of neurons. Here we select subsets of the 20 “best” or “worst” neurons using a greedy optimization (Ghanbari et al., 2019). We use a beam search approach where we add neurons one at a time to the population and keep the top (or bottom) five performing populations that minimize (or maximize) the absolute median error on the training data for the M1 and V1 datasets or the top one accuracy on the training data for the ABI dataset. Although not guaranteed to be the optimal best/worst set of 20 neurons, this approach generates subpopulations where the decoding error is substantially better/worse than randomly selected sets of 20 neurons.
Decoders based on optimal linear estimation (OLE)
For comparison, we also fit non-Bayesian decoders to trial-by-trial data from M1 and V1 and continuous data from HC (Ghanbari et al., 2019). Briefly, we use OLE, where the core assumption is that the external variable on trial
k can be reconstructed using a linear combination of functions weighted by the activity of each neuron as follows:
Coverage and constant correction
To assess the calibration of these decoders for continuous variables, we compare the frequentist coverage (fraction of trials on which the true stimulus/movement falls within a highest density region) to the nominal/desired probability. For a well-calibrated Bayesian model, the highest posterior density (HPD) regions of a given size (e.g., the 95% region) should contain the true values with the nominated probability (e.g., 95%). Here we compute the (cross-validated) proportion of trials for which the true stimulus/movement falls within the HPD regions (the “coverage”) as we vary the size of the credible set.
For categorical posteriors, there are several scoring rules that have been previously described, such as the Brier score (Gneiting and Raftery, 2007), but, here, to emphasize “coverage,” we extend our calculations with continuous credible regions to use discrete credible sets. We construct the HP set, as before, by adding the highest probability categories until the probability
m in the set meets the nominated probability
Since most Bayesian decoders appear to be badly calibrated, we consider a post hoc correction (i.e., recalibration). This correction is similar to the “inflation factor” in ensemble probabilistic forecasting (Wilks, 2002; Gneiting and Raftery, 2007), where similar types of overconfidence can occur (Raftery et al., 2005). Namely, here we consider decoding with a modified posterior
Conformal prediction intervals
As an alternative to the post hoc correction, we also consider split conformal prediction based on the MAP point estimates in our Bayesian models and the OLE point estimates. Here our approach is based on Algorithm 2 from (Lei et al., 2018). Briefly, we split the data in half. Then, after fitting our models to one half of the data, we evaluate the residuals for the other half. For a desired coverage
Dynamic models
The GLM and GLLVM described above assume that trials are independent. However, in many cases, it is more appropriate or desirable to decode with a dynamic model. Rather than decoding the external variable on trial
k, we wish to decode the external variable
Briefly, we assume that the observation for neuron
i at time
t follows the following:
When fitting the encoding model,
Results
Bayesian decoders are based on first fitting tuning curves for each neuron using training data. The encoding model determines the likelihood distribution, and, for traditional (naive) Bayesian models, neurons are assumed to be conditionally independent given the external variables. During decoding, we then use Bayes’ rule to calculate the posterior distribution over possible stimuli or movements given the observed neural activity. Here we focus on assessing not just the decoding accuracy but the uncertainty of the posterior under different models and experimental settings. Our goal is to determine to what extent the traditional models, as well as more recently developed latent variable models, have well-calibrated posterior estimates (i.e., where the posterior probabilities match the true probabilities of the external variable taking specific values).
To illustrate the problem of model calibration, we consider a hypothetical set of Bayesian decoders (Fig. 1A). The average error is the same for each of these decoders, since the maximum and means of the posteriors are identical, but the uncertainty of the decoders varies. There is underconfidence or overconfidence on single trials, and, across trials, the posterior distributions do not necessarily match the distribution of errors. When errors occur, an overconfident decoder will not have a proper coverage of the true value. On the other hand, an underconfident decoder will cover the true value too often for the desired confidence level. In our example case, imagining five trials and an 80% credible interval, a well-calibrated decoder correctly covers the true value for four of five trials, while the overconfident decoder only covers one of five and the underconfident decoder covers five of five. In general, overconfident decoders will have lower coverage than desired, while underconfident decoders will have higher coverage than desired (Fig. 1B).
Bayesian models can have poor calibration when the model is misspecified. To illustrate how such misspecification could occur with neural data, we simulate the impact of latent variables on a traditional Bayesian decoder. Here noisy spike observations are generated by a population of identically tuned neurons (Fig. 1D, top) with Poisson variability. However, in addition to their stimulus/movement tuning, neurons receive a common one-dimensional latent input that increases or decreases the activity on individual trials. Since this input is shared by the entire population (of 20 neurons in this case), it produces correlated variability. A traditional Bayesian decoder first fits tuning curves for each individual neuron (here using a GLM with Poisson observations). The posterior is calculated assuming that neural responses are conditionally independent given the stimuli, and, as before, we can quantify the coverage by identifying the HPD regions. In this more realistic simulation, the posterior can be multimodal resulting in multiple credible regions rather than just a single credible interval. However, since the GLM decoder does not account for the latent variable, the decoder is overconfident (Fig. 1C, top) and less accurate (Fig. 1D, bottom). When the latent variable has a larger impact on neural responses relative to the impact of the stimulus, errors increase, and the decoder is increasingly overconfident. Hence, traditional Bayesian decoders used in the literature by assuming the independence between responses given the stimuli can have high error and overconfidence in the presence of latent variables.
Modeling the latent variable reduces error and provides well-calibrated posteriors. Here we use a Poisson GLLVM (see Materials and Methods) where the encoding model is fit to account for the tuning curve, as well as the contribution of a shared low-dimensional latent variable. Under the GLLVM, neural responses are not conditionally independent given the stimulus. Rather, for each trial, the latent variable is estimated, and, during decoding, the latent variable is marginalized over in order to generate the posterior distribution over stimuli. The error for the GLLVM decoder still increases as the latent variable has a larger relative impact on neural responses (Fig. 1D, bottom), but the coverage closely follows the desired credibility level (Fig. 1C, bottom). Well-calibrated decoders (such as the GLLVM in this simulation) have the advantage that the posterior appropriately covers the true stimulus.
To further illustrate how overconfidence arises, we consider a single tuned neuron in the GLLVM (Fig. 2). Here a neuron is tuned with a preferred stimulus/movement direction of 0°. However, a latent variable that changes from trial to trial can shift the tuning curve up or down. This latent variable creates an additional source of ambiguity when a specific spike count is observed. We cannot distinguish between a situation where the neuron is spiking during the presence of a preferred stimulus and a situation where the neuron is spiking during a nonpreferred stimulus that coincides with an excitatory latent input. For stimulus
x and neural responses
y, the key difference between the GLM and GLLVM decoders is that instead of using the posterior
Trial-by-trial experimental data
For experimental data, we do not know the true model. However, the calibration and accuracy of Bayesian decoders can be assessed empirically. Here we compare GLM and GLLVM Bayesian decoders in three experimental settings: (1) decoding reach direction during a center-out task using recordings from the primary motor cortex (M1), (2) decoding sine-wave grating movement direction using recordings from the primary visual cortex (V1), and (3) decoding the identity of a natural image stimulus using multiregion Neuropixels recordings from the ABI. These data were previously collected and publicly shared (see Materials and Methods), and for each setting we evaluate the decoding accuracy as well as the coverage—the fraction of trials where the true stimulus falls within the highest density regions of the posterior (HPD).
We compare four models: (1) Poisson GLM, (2) negative binomial GLM, (3) Poisson GLLVM, and (4) negative binomial GLLVM. For M1 and V1, we model tuning curves using a Fourier basis. For ABI, we model the spike counts in response to each of 118 images and regularize to prevent overfitting
For experimental data, there is substantial heterogeneity in tuning curves (Fig. 3A–C), and posteriors may be continuous or discrete depending on the experimental context. However, as with the toy examples above, the GLLVM (in this case, with a negative binomial observation model) tends to have posteriors with higher uncertainty compared with the GLM (Fig. 3E,F). On single trials, the posteriors tend to be wider and to have lower probabilities for the (MAP) point estimate for the GLLVM. In both continuous and discrete cases, outcomes that were assigned near-zero probability under the GLM are assigned nonzero probability under the GLLVM.
As with the simulations above, we find that Bayesian decoders tend to be overconfident (Fig. 4A–C). For all three experimental settings (M1, V1, and ABI), the HPD regions cover the true stimulus/movement less often than desired for all credible levels when decoding from all recorded neurons. For the Poisson GLM, for example, when we specify a 95% credibility level, the posteriors from M1 only include the true target direction 70% of the time, posteriors from V1 only include the true stimulus direction 51%, and posteriors from ABI only include the true natural image stimulus 31% of the time. The negative binomial GLM has better coverage than the Poisson GLM, while adding latent variables improves coverage even more. The best-calibrated model of these four is the negative binomial GLLVM—here when we specify a 95% credibility level, the posteriors from M1 include the true target direction 81% of the time, posteriors from V1 include the true stimulus direction 82% of the time, and posteriors from ABI include the true natural image stimulus 86% of the time. Traditional Bayesian decoders can thus have substantial overconfidence, and calibration is improved by adding latent variables.
As previous studies have noted, non-Poisson observation models and latent variables can alter, and in many cases improve, the decoding accuracy. Here, for M1 and V1, we calculate the absolute circular distance between the true target/stimulus direction and the MAP estimate of the target/stimulus direction from the Bayesian decoders on each trial. For ABI, we assess the accuracy based on whether the top one or top five categories of the discrete posterior include the true stimulus image on each trial. For the full populations of M1 data, the models do not have substantially different errors (median across trials 9.8°, 9.5°, 9.8°, and 9.8° for the P-GLM, NB-GLM, P-GLLVM, and NB-GLLVM, respectively). For the V1 data, the Poisson GLM outperforms the NB-GLM (median error 3.8° vs 4.5°; Wilcoxon signed rank test; p < 10−12; z = 7.5), and the Poisson GLLVM outperforms the NB-GLLVM (median error 2.8° vs 3.0°; Wilcoxon signed rank test; p < 10−12; z = 7.7). For ABI data, however, the NB models outperform the Poisson models (top one accuracy 15.6% [14.6, 16.7] for P-GLM vs 23.0% [21.9, 24.2] for NB-GLM). For V1, the GLM-based models have slightly lower error than the GLLVM (p < 10−12; z = 17.0; Wilcoxon signed rank test for Poisson GLM vs GLLVM), but for the ABI data, the GLLVM models improve the accuracy substantially (22.3% [22.1, 24.5] for P-GLLVM and 30.1% [29.2, 31.8] for NB-GLLVM). In all cases, for randomly sampled subnetworks, we find that the cross-validated error decreases (or accuracy increases) as a function of how many neurons are included in the decoder for all models (Fig. 4D–F).
These error and accuracy measures are based on the MAP estimates of the external variable; however, there are also differences across models in the dispersion of the posteriors. The NB models have higher circular standard deviations than the Poisson models for the M1 and V1 data and substantially higher entropy for ABI (Fig. 4G–I). For M1, the circular standard deviation of the posterior is 7.2° for the Poisson GLM (median across trials) compared with 8.8° for the NB-GLM (p < 10−12; z = 14.3; two-sided Wilcoxon signed rank test), and 7.7° and 9.0° for the P-GLLVM and NB-GLLVM (p < 10−12; z = 13.9; two-sided Wilcoxon signed rank test). For V1, the median circular standard deviation is 2.0° for the P-GLM compared with 4.0° for the NB-GLM (p < 10−12; z = 38.8) and 2.0° versus 3.3° for the P-GLLVM and NB-GLLVM (p < 10−12; z = −35.0; two-sided Wilcoxon signed rank test). For ABI, the average entropy is 1.26 bits for the P-GLM and 2.7 bits for NB-GLM [t(4,872) = 136.4; p < 10−12; paired t test], 1.8 bits for P-GLLVM, and 4.0 bits for NB-GLLVM [t(4,872) = 19.6; p < 10−12; paired t test compared with NB-GLM]. In the case of decoding natural images from the ABI, the GLLVMs are less certain and more accurate than the GLMs.
Differences in the dispersions of the posteriors are reflected in differences in coverage. As more neurons are used for decoding, the models become increasingly overconfident and badly calibrated (Fig. 4J–L), even as the error decreases (Fig. 4D,E) or accuracy improves (Fig. 4F). The negative binomial GLLVM has the best coverage across datasets and population sizes, but note that the coverage is still less than desired (95% for Fig. 4J–L).
Interpreting latent variable models
Including a latent variable allows the GLLVMs to account for variation in neural responses to the same stimulus/movement. Here, with a one-dimensional model, the GLLVM primarily accounts for the overall fluctuations in population activity from trial to trial (Fig. 5). While the GLM only predicts variation between stimuli and movements for both M1 (Fig. 5A) and V1 (Fig. 5B), the GLLVM accounts for the fact that some trials tend to have higher overall activity across the population, while other trials have lower activity. This trend is apparent when examining the overall population activity—here calculated as the sum of the log activity. We also examine correlations between responses of pairs of neurons (Fig. 5, right). Here we calculate stimulus and noise correlations by shuffling responses to the same stimuli/movements. Stimulus correlations reflect the average on shuffled data, while noise correlations are given by the observed correlations minus the shuffled correlations, and, for the models, we sample spike counts to mimic the observed data. Since the GLM assumes that neurons are conditionally independent given the stimulus/movement, it accounts for stimulus correlations but tends to underestimate noise correlations. The GLLVM, on the other hand, accurately accounts for both stimulus and noise correlations. This pattern is present in the overall correlation matrices, as well as when averaging over pairs of neurons based on the differences in their preferred directions
The dimensionality of the latent variable may have some impact on the encoding and decoding accuracy and on the calibration of Bayesian decoders. To characterize the potential effects of dimensionality, we fit GLLVMs with 1–5 dimensional latent states for the M1, V1, and ABI datasets. We find that, in most cases, the GLLVMs with >1 dimensionality have similar error and coverage to the models with one dimension, with the exception of the Poisson GLLVM, which tends to have better coverage with more dimensions (Fig. 6). In all cases, the coverage of the NB models is better than that of the Poisson models.
Since the size of the population appears to have an impact on coverage, we also examine how the composition of the population impacts accuracy and decoding. Here we use a greedy optimization (see Materials and Methods) to find the population of size N neurons that minimizes the error (M1 or V1) or maximizes the top one accuracy (ABI) of the Poisson GLM creating the greedy “best” subpopulation. Moreover, for comparison, we also consider maximizing the error (M1 or V1) or minimizing the top one accuracy (ABI) of the Poisson GLM to create the greedy “worst” subpopulation. Like previous studies, we find that the full population is often unnecessary for accurate decoding—a greedy best subpopulation of N = 20 often has error/accuracy comparable to the full population. Here we additionally show that these greedy best models often have better coverage than the models based on the full population (Fig. 6). However, the population size is not the only factor determining coverage, since the greedy best and greedy worst populations have substantial differences in coverage despite both consisting of 20 neurons.
Post hoc correction for miscalibration
Since even decoders based on GLLVMs are overconfident, it may be useful to consider calibration as a distinct step in neural data analysis in situations where accurate uncertainty estimation is needed. One approach to correcting calibration errors is to simply inflate the posterior uncertainty post hoc. That is, rather than decoding using
For the overconfident examples above, we estimate a single constant
h using the full data for each case (see Materials and Methods) and find that this transformation produces well-calibrated decoding distributions at all desired confidence levels (Fig. 7A–C). The transformation does not change the decoding accuracy (based on MAP estimates) but allows for substantially more accurate uncertainty estimation. In the examples above, we showed that overconfidence depends on the encoding model and the number of neurons used in the decoder. The optimal value of
h, thus, also depends on the model as well as the size and composition of the population with higher overconfidence needing greater correction (smaller
h). We also note that, at least in some cases, underconfidence is possible (Fig. 7D), but can be similarly corrected by
Within a given experimental setting, there is a consistent relationship between the degree of over-/underconfidence and the optimal correction parameter (here optimized by minimizing the mean squared error in the nominated coverage vs empirical coverage plots). Across models (GLM, GLLVM, Poisson, and NB) and populations (full population and greedy best), the correction parameters are well predicted by a power law,
For some settings, rather than trial-by-trial decoding of spike counts, the goal is to decode a continuous, typically smoothly varying, external variable. To illustrate how general the problem of overconfidence in Bayesian decoders is, we consider continuous estimates of an animal's position from hippocampal activity (Fig. 8A). Here, rather than distinct trials with a controlled stimulus/behavior, a rat runs freely on a linear track. GLM and GLLVMs can still be used to decode the animal's position. We fit encoding models based on place fields (direction-selective cubic B-spline bases with 10 equally spaced knots), and for the GLLVMs, we additionally include a one-dimensional latent variable. However, to more accurately decode the continuous behavior, we also add a process model that ensures that the position and latent state vary smoothly from one time to the next (see Materials and Methods).
As before, we assess the coverage of each model. Here we find that, decoding the time series of the animal position, the Poisson GLM is the most overconfident, and the NB-GLLVM is the most well calibrated. The 95% credible regions for the posterior include the true position only 48% of the time for Poisson GLM, while the NB-GLLVM covers the true position 63% of the time (Fig. 8B). All four models have better calibrated posteriors following post hoc correction (Fig. 8C). The coverage of 95% credible regions increases to 91% for the P-GLM and 94% for the NB-GLLVM, for example.
Conformal prediction intervals
One potential alternative to the post hoc correction described above that may be useful for continuous decoding is conformal prediction (Shafer and Vovk, 2008; Lei et al., 2018). Rather than using a posterior distribution, this approach constructs prediction intervals by using the quantiles of the distribution of residuals (see Materials and Methods). Here we evaluate split conformal prediction (Lei et al., 2018) and find that this approach produces well-calibrated intervals around the point estimates of both the GLM and GLLVM (one latent dimension) on trial-by-trial stimulus direction or movement direction in the V1 and M1 datasets and position in the HC dataset (Fig. 9).
Conformal prediction has the advantage that it is parameter-free and can also be used for non-Bayesian decoders. To illustrate this possibility, here we fit additional decoders to the M1, V1, and HC data using OLE (see Materials and Methods). These decoders do not have explicit measures of uncertainty but, in some cases, perform on par with the Bayesian models in terms of accuracy—here with (10-fold) cross-validated median absolute errors of 9.8° for M1 and 3.5° for V1. Moreover, for HC, the dynamic Poisson GLM has the median absolute error of 4.7 cm, and the dynamic NB-GLLVM has 4.6 cm compared with the median absolute error of 7.8 cm for OLE. Using split conformal prediction, the intervals are, like the Bayesian decoders, well calibrated (Fig. 9). However, since the conformal prediction intervals are based only on point predictions and the residuals across all trials, they do not capture changes in uncertainty across stimuli/movements or from trial to trial.
Posterior uncertainty and task variables
From trial to trial, there are substantial variations in both posterior uncertainty and accuracy. The exact relationship between error/uncertainty and accuracy depends somewhat on the decoder, since different models have different uncertainties. However, in the data examined above, we find that for all models, error increases with increasing posterior uncertainty (M1 and V1) or accuracy decreases with increasing posterior uncertainty (ABI; Fig. 10). Fitting a linear model (in the log–log domain) for the post hoc-corrected Poisson GLM, M1 error increases to 252% [187, 340] (95% CI) for each doubling of the posterior (circular) standard deviation. For V1 with the post hoc-corrected Poisson GLM, the error increases to 160% [150, 169] for each doubling of the posterior (circular) standard deviation. Fitting a logistic model for ABI, the accuracy decreases with OR = 0.75 [0.68, 0.83] per bit of the posterior entropy. These results are for the posteriors of the post hoc-corrected Poisson GLM, but all models show statistically significant dependencies between error/accuracy and uncertainty both with and without post hoc correction.
In experiments where a task variable is expected to influence behavioral/perceptual uncertainty, we may also expect Bayesian decoders to reflect differences in this uncertainty. Here, for instance, we examine V1 data from an additional experiment with static-oriented grating stimuli, where the contrast of the stimulus was explicitly varied. Fitting separate (categorical) Poisson GLMs to the different time points (50 ms window) and contrast conditions, we find that accuracy for decoding categorical stimulus orientation increases following stimulus onset and increases with increasing stimulus contrast (Fig. 11A, top). Accuracy for the high-contrast trials is substantially higher than for low-contrast trials (66% for high, 43% for low; z = 7.4; p < 10−12; two-sided test for difference of proportions; 200 ms following stimulus onset). Additionally, posterior entropy decreases following stimulus onset and is lowest for high-contrast stimuli (Fig. 11A, middle). In this example, since the population is relatively small (18 units), the degree of overconfidence for the Poisson GLM (Fig. 11A, bottom) is not as extreme as the previous V1 population. Here, the post hoc-corrected posteriors for the Poisson GLM (corrected separately for each time point and contrast) show a similar pattern with high-contrast trials having lower entropy than low-contrast trials [1.3 bits for high; 2.1 bits for low; two-sided unpaired t test t(955.4) = 21.0; p < 10−12; at 200 ms following stimulus onset]. As in Figure 10, we find that single-trial accuracy is well predicted by the posterior uncertainty (Fig. 11B). The relationship between entropy and accuracy is consistent across contrasts, and the logistic fits do not differ substantially for the different contrasts (OR = 0.18/bit [0.12, 0.27] 95% CI for high contrast, OR = 0.21/bit [0.14, 0.31] for low contrast). These trends mirror recent results from Boundy-Singer et al. (2023) also characterizing stimulus orientation uncertainty in macaque V1.
We use a similar analysis to assess the impact of reach speed in M1. Just as stimulus contrast may impact uncertainty when decoding visual stimuli, movement features beyond reach direction may impact uncertainty when decoding behavior. Here we use the M1 data during center-out reaching examined above. We fit a single decoder for reach direction at each time point (50 ms window), but assess accuracy, entropy, and coverage separately for different trials based on the peak movement speed. Splitting the trials into speed terciles (Fig. 11C), we find that accuracy increases shortly before movement onset, and trials with the fastest reaches are decoded more accurately than those with slower reaches (80% for fast; 64% for slow; z = 2.6; p = 0.01; two-sided test for difference of proportions; 100 ms following movement onset). Posterior entropy also decreases shortly before movement onset and is lowest for the fast reaches (Fig. 11C, middle). Here, as before, the Poisson GLM tends to be overconfident. The post hoc-corrected posteriors have substantially higher entropy, but show the same pattern where fast reaches have the lowest entropy [0.8 bits for fast; 1.4 bits for low; two-sided unpaired t test t(184.5) = 7.8; p < 10−12; at 100 ms following movement onset). The entropy on single trials again predicts single-trial accuracy (Fig. 11D), and the logistic fits do not differ substantially for the different speeds (OR = 0.16/bit [0.06, 0.44] 95% CI for fast, OR = 0.35/bit [0.13, 0.94] for slow).
Discussion
Using data from a range of brain regions and experimental settings, we have shown how Bayesian decoders of neural spiking activity are often miscalibrated. In particular, the posterior estimates tend to be overconfident. Overconfidence increases with increasing numbers of neurons, is reduced by using negative binomial observation models (compared with Poisson), and is reduced by modeling latent variables. However, since even the best calibrated models tested here are not well calibrated, we introduce a post hoc correction and show how it can be used, in multiple settings, to recalibrate uncertainty estimates. Finally, we present results illustrating how the posterior uncertainty of Bayesian decoders can vary substantially from trial to trial. Single-trial posterior uncertainty predicts single-trial accuracy and may be useful for understanding variation in perceptual or behavioral confidence due to task variables such as stimulus contrast or movement speed.
Similar to previous work (Macke et al., 2011), we show here how latent variables (GLLVMs) can better account for noise correlations and shared variability in the simultaneously recorded neurons. Correlations are known to play an important role in population coding, generally (von der Malsburg, 1994; Nirenberg, 2003), and failing to accurately account for these dependencies can lead to decoding errors (Ruda et al., 2020). Latent variable models represent one approach to describing shared variability. Fitting latent variables alone, without explicit tuning to external variables often reveals interesting task structure (Gao et al., 2016; Zhao and Park, 2017), and the latent states fit here may reflect both internal and unmodeled external, task-related effects. Previous work has shown how these models can improve encoding and decoding accuracy (Santhanam et al., 2009; Chase et al., 2010; Lawhern et al., 2010). Here we additionally show how latent variable models increase the uncertainty of Bayesian decoders and improve their calibration.
Bayesian decoders have advantages over other decoding methods in that they provide probabilistic predictions and can flexibly incorporate prior assumptions, such as sparseness and smoothness. However, many non-Bayesian decoders exist, including vector decoders (Georgopoulos et al., 1986; Salinas and Abbott, 1994), nearest-neighbor methods, support vector machines, and artificial neural networks (Quiroga and Panzeri, 2009). However, well-tuned Bayesian methods can often outperform non-Bayesian approaches (Zhang et al., 1998). Machine learning and recent deep learning approaches to decoding have been shown to be more accurate than simple Bayesian models in many settings (Pandarinath et al., 2018; J. I. Glaser et al., 2020; Livezey and Glaser, 2021). Since calculating the full posterior distribution can be computationally expensive, these methods can also be substantially faster for situations where predictions are time-sensitive. Almost all work with non-Bayesian decoders of neural activity focuses on the accuracy of point predictions. Here we show how conformal prediction can be used to generate well-calibrated uncertainty estimates for OLE. However, miscalibration is a known problem in a work on artificial neural networks (Guo et al., 2017), and a recent work on Bayesian neural networks and conformal prediction (Shafer and Vovk, 2008) could potentially be used to create and calibrate uncertainty estimates for these models as well.
Accurate uncertainty estimates may potentially be useful for the robust control of BMIs. For instance, although many BMIs directly control effectors, such as a cursor position (decoding movement) or a desired word (decoding speech), based on point predictions (Nicolelis, 2003), it may be beneficial to distinguish between predictions based on their confidence level. Here, we find substantial variation in uncertainty for trial-by-trial off-line decoding, and we also illustrate how contrast (in V1) and speed (in M1) might impact decoding uncertainty. These results are limited by the fact that we do not explicitly include contrast or speed in the encoding model (Moran and Schwartz, 1999) or decode these variables directly (Inoue et al., 2018), but they suggest how uncertainty may be a separate and worthwhile consideration for decoding problems. Additionally, our results suggest that recalibration could be necessary to avoid overconfidence in BMIs that make use of posterior uncertainty during control.
The uncertainty estimates from Bayesian decoders of neural activity may also be useful for studying behavioral and perceptual uncertainty. Normative models of population coding (Ma et al., 2006) and broader descriptions of uncertainty in the brain (Knill and Pouget, 2004) often directly relate neural activity to probabilistic descriptions of the external world. Although several features of neural activity have been proposed as indicators of behavioral/perceptual uncertainty (Vilares and Kording, 2011), the posteriors from Bayesian decoders represent a principled framework for translating noisy, high-dimensional data into a single probabilistic description (Zemel et al., 1998; Dehaene et al., 2021; Kriegeskorte and Wei, 2021). The impacts of tuning curve shapes (Pouget et al., 1999; Zhang and Sejnowski, 1999) and correlations between neurons (Averbeck et al., 2006; Lin et al., 2015; Kohn et al., 2016) on the uncertainty of population coding have been well studied, and here we add to this work by demonstrating how different encoding models (GLM vs GLLVM and Poisson vs negative binomial) have systematically different degrees of overconfidence in experimental recordings across many settings.
Since even the best Bayesian models (negative binomial latent variable models up to five dimensions) are overconfident, recalibration appears to be necessary to ensure that the uncertainty of Bayesian decoders matches the distribution of errors. On the one hand, this may suggest that there is additional mismatch between the GLLVM and the data generating process. It may be that low-dimensional latent variable models only partially capture noise correlations (Stevenson et al., 2012), that there is unmodeled nonstationarity in the tuning curves (Cortes et al., 2012; Rule et al., 2019), that responses are underdispersed (DeWeese et al., 2003; Stevenson, 2016), or some combination of these factors. On the other hand, humans and other animals are often over- or underconfident during perceptual and cognitive judgments (Baranski and Petrusic, 1994; Kepecs and Mainen, 2012; Mamassian, 2016). It is possible that the original (miscalibrated) uncertainty estimates better predict psychophysical uncertainty or metacognitive reports of confidence, even if recalibrated uncertainty estimates better predict the distribution of external variables.
Finally, it is important to note that when Bayesian models are recalibrated post hoc they are no longer following a coherent Bayesian framework (Dawid, 1982). From a practical standpoint, such as when developing BMIs, model calibration may be more important than model coherence. However, additional work is needed to better understand the alignment of perceptual/behavioral uncertainty and decoder posterior uncertainty (Panzeri et al., 2017). Models with more accurate descriptions of single neuron variability (Gao et al., 2015; Ghanbari et al., 2019), with nonstationarity (Shanechi et al., 2016; Wei and Stevenson, 2023), additional stimulus/movement nonlinearities (Schwartz and Simoncelli, 2001), state dependence (Panzeri et al., 2016), and more complex latent structure (J. Glaser et al., 2020; Williams et al., 2020; Sokoloski et al., 2021; Williams and Linderman, 2021), may all show better coverage while maintaining coherence. Our results here indicate that Bayesian decoders of spiking activity are not necessarily well calibrated by default.
Footnotes
- Received November 17, 2023.
- Revision received January 29, 2024.
- Accepted March 11, 2024.
This work was supported by the National Science Foundation under Grant 1931249 and Grant 1848451.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
- Correspondence should be addressed to Ian H. Stevenson at ian.stevenson{at}uconn.edu.
- Copyright © 2024 the authors